Abstract
In guinea pig retina, immunostaining reveals a dual gradient of opsins: cones expressing opsin sensitive to medium wavelengths (M) predominate in the upper retina, whereas cones expressing opsin sensitive to shorter wavelengths (S) predominate in the lower retina. Whether these gradients correspond to functional gradients in postreceptoral neurons is essentially unknown. Using monochromatic flashes, we measured the relative weights with which M, S, and rod signals contribute to horizontal cell responses. For a background that produced 4.76 log10 photoisomerizations per rod per second (Rh*/rod/s), mean weights in superior retina were 52% (M), 2% (S), and 46% (rod). Mean weights in inferior retina were 9% (M), 50% (S), and 41% (rod). In superior retina, cone opsin weights agreed quantitatively with relative pigment density estimates from immunostaining. In inferior retina, cone opsin weights agreed qualitatively with relative pigment density estimates, but quantitative comparison was impossible because individual cones coexpress both opsins to varying and unquantifiable degrees. We further characterized the functional gradients in horizontal and brisk-transient ganglion cells using flickering stimuli produced by various mixtures of blue and green primary lights. Cone weights for both cell types resembled those obtained for horizontal cells using monochromatic flashes. Because the brisk-transient ganglion cell is thought to mediate behavioral detection of luminance contrast, our results are consistent with the hypothesis that the dual gradient of cone opsins assists achromatic contrast detection against different spectral backgrounds. In our preparation, rod responses did not completely saturate, even at background light levels typical of outdoor sunlight (5.14 log10 Rh*/rod/s).
Keywords: cone opsin dual gradient, horizontal cell, brisk-transient ganglion cell, cone weights, light adaptation, spectral sensitivity
Introduction
Most mammals are dichromatic, encoding spectral information with one cone opsin sensitive to medium wavelengths (M) and another sensitive to shorter wavelengths (S) (Jacobs, 1993; Peichl, 2005). Generally, S cones comprise ∼10% of the total and distribute evenly over the retina (Ahnelt and Kolb, 2000). However, in certain rodents, such as guinea pig, rabbit, and mouse, immunostaining shows a dual gradient: cones expressing M opsin peak in superior retina and decline inferiorly; whereas cones expressing S opsin peak in inferior retina and decline superiorly (Röhlich et al., 1994; Applebury et al., 2000; Haverkamp et al., 2005; Nikonov et al., 2005). This dual gradient in animals that scurry along the ground might be an evolutionary adaptation to detect objects against different spectral backgrounds, such as ground vegetation (seen by superior retina) and sky (seen by inferior retina) (Szél et al., 2000; Osorio and Vorobyev, 2005).
If this idea is correct, the gradients revealed by immunostaining should also be present in the responses of postreceptoral neurons. The existing data are suggestive but qualitative (Calderone and Jacobs, 1995; Ekesten and Gouras, 2005). Here we study the chromatic properties of horizontal and brisk-transient ganglion cells in superior and inferior retina of guinea pig. The horizontal cell is directly postsynaptic to cones and in dichromats generally collects from most cones within its dendritic field (Peichl and Gonzalez-Soriano, 1994; Hack and Peichl, 1999). However, in some species, the type A horizontal cell is selective for S cones (Sandmann et al., 1996).
The brisk-transient ganglion cell is postsynaptic to bipolar cells, some of which are known to be cone selective (Calkins and Sterling, 1999; Haverkamp et al., 2005). Thus, horizontal and ganglion cells might simply propagate signals from all of the overlying cones and reflect the predictions from immunostaining (Diller et al., 2004; Jusuf et al., 2006) or they might collect selectively from one cone type, for example, to smooth out the gradients. Another possible mechanism for smoothing the gradient would be if synaptic gains on signals from M and S cones varied with retinal location. The brisk-transient ganglion cell was chosen from among the many known ganglion cell types because it is thought to mediate detection and processing of achromatic contrast (Merigan and Maunsell, 1993; Dhingra et al., 2003; Silveira et al., 2004).
Because ∼90% of guinea pig photoreceptors are rods (Peichl and Gonzalez-Soriano, 1994) and under steady bright light they recover from saturation (Demontis et al., 1993), rods might contribute to vision at intensities normally considered photopic. Accordingly, we measured the rod contribution to horizontal cell responses across a wide range of intensities and incorporated these measurements into our estimates of cone weights in horizontal and ganglion cells. The cone weights matched the immunostaining pattern, showing that the dual gradient is preserved in the centrally projecting neurons that mediate achromatic contrast detection. This fits the broad hypothesis that the dual opsin gradient is an adaptation to optimize contrast detection.
Materials and Methods
Tissue preparation.
We used albino guinea pigs weighing ∼300–500 g.Vingrys and Bui (2001) showed that albino guinea pigs give large but otherwise normal ERG responses and that the ERG components develop with age in the same way as with the pigmented animal. Although albino animals such as mouse and rat have a lower rod density compared with pigmented ones (Jeffery et al., 1997; Ilia and Jeffery, 2000), the transverse absorbance of rat rod outer segments has been shown to be the same (Grant et al., 2001).
An animal was overdosed with anesthetic (ketamine, 100 mg/kg; xylazine, 20 mg/kg; and pentobarbital, 50 mg/kg), and both eyes were removed. The eyecup was flattened on a membrane filter with ganglion cells up. This preparation was placed into a chamber on a microscope stage (BX50WI; Olympus Optical, Melville, NY) and superfused with oxygenated (95% O2 and 5% CO2) Ames' medium (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) containing sodium bicarbonate (1.9 g/L) and glucose (0.8 g/L) at ∼5–7 ml/min. The chamber temperature was maintained between 34 and 37°C. The retina was dark adapted for 0.5 h before measurements were made.
To help find particular retinal regions during the experiment, three circular holes were cut in the membrane filter. When the tissue was flattened onto the filter, the middle hole (1 mm diameter) was positioned over the optic disk and the filter was oriented so that the other two holes were located above or below the optic disk along the superior–inferior axis of the tissue. Recordings were made from within the locations defined by these two holes (2 mm diameter, 3 mm superior or inferior to the optic disk) (see Fig. 1B).
Electrical recording.
Horizontal cells were recorded intracellularly with sharp electrodes (tip impedance, 70–200 MΩ). Responses were amplified (NeuroData IR-283; Cygnus Technology, Delaware Water Gap, PA), digitized at a 5 kHz sampling rate with 12-bit precision (DigiData 1200; Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA), and stored for later analysis (AxoScope; Molecular Devices). Voltage traces were smoothed by averaging the sampled data within 1 ms bins.
Horizontal cells were targeted by advancing electrodes in the presence of full-field flicker and observing whether the recorded potential was modulated. A cell was selected for recording if it responded strongly to full-field luminance flicker, sinusoidally modulated at ∼40% contrast. The mean resting membrane potential in light (at the main background) of cells selected for recording was −36 mV, with large variation across cells (±17 mV, SD). For many cells, the resting potential drifted gradually toward more positive values during recording.
Brisk-transient ganglion cells were recorded extracellularly with loose patch electrodes (tip impedance, 2–4 MΩ). Responses were recorded using the same equipment as for the horizontal cells. Spikes were identified using a threshold detection method. Spike rates were calculated for ∼15 ms bins. Some cells were repatched after the seal between pipette and cell membrane weakened. With this preparation, stable recording from a single cell was possible for up to 2 h.
Brisk-transient ganglion cells were identified for recording by observing the tissue through the infrared differential interference contrast optics of the microscope and locating cells with large somas (18–23 μm diameter). A ganglion cell was selected for recording if it gave clear and stable responses to a 10% contrast flickering spot and if the recorded spike was biphasic with amplitude of at least 2 mV. Cell identity was confirmed by the large size of the receptive field center (400–600 μm diameter) and the “brisk-transient” response (Cleland et al., 1975; Demb et al., 2001; Dhingra et al., 2003). In pilot experiments, cells were sometimes penetrated with a sharp electrode after recording and stained with Lucifer yellow (3%, in 0.1 m LiCl solution) for visualization. This method provided morphological validation of the cell identification procedures used. It was not used during the main experiments to avoid excessive exposure of the tissue to light.
Measuring photoreceptor inputs to horizontal and ganglion cells. Cone and rod inputs were characterized by recording responses to spots of different spectral composition. “Flash experiments” measured the action spectra to flashed monochromatic spots on a spatially uniform background. This was slow but rather completely characterized the spectral properties of a cell.“Flicker experiments” measured responses to spots on a spatially uniform background produced by modulating mixtures of two primary lights. This was more efficient, but analyzing the data to estimate cone weights required assumptions about the photopigment action spectra and the strength of rod input.
We used two optical systems: (1) In the “LCD system,” light from a liquid crystal display (LCD) projector (PowerLite 730c; Epson America, Long Beach, CA) was used. The relative spectra and input–output relationships (gamma functions) of the independent color primaries (red, green, and blue) of this projector were measured at the tissue plane using a CVI SM-240 (CVI Spectral Products, Albuquerque, NM) spectrometer. The total irradiance (integrated over wavelength) arriving at the tissue was also characterized (IL1400A; International Light, Peabody, MA). In the plane of the tissue, the full image subtended ∼2.4 × 3.2 mm, with each pixel corresponding to ∼3.1 × 3.1 μm. The refresh rate of the display was 70 Hz. (2) In the “lamp system,” light from a xenon lamp (HLX 64642; Osram, Munich, Germany), collimated and fed through a narrowband, tunable filter (VariSpec; Cambridge Research and Instrumentation, Woburn, MA) and an adjustable aperture to produce a uniform spot of monochromatic light (∼10 nm bandwidth) on the tissue was used. The temporal profile of the spot was controlled by a mechanical shutter (VS25S2T1 shutter and 122-BP controller; Uniblitz, Rochester, NY). Lamp voltage, wavelength, and shutter timing were all computer controlled. To deliver lights of desired intensities during the experiment, the relationship between lamp voltage and irradiance across wavelengths at the tissue was coarsely characterized beforehand. The exact irradiances of the stimuli presented were measured after each experiment. The outputs of the two optical systems were combined through a beam splitter and delivered to the tissue through the camera port on the microscope and the objective.
The flash experiments used monochromatic spots from the lamp system on a background produced by the LCD system. The main background produced nearly equal isomerization rates in M and S cones [4.17 and 4.12 log10 photoisomerizations per cone per second (Rh*/photoreceptor/s)] and a somewhat higher rate in rods (4.76 log10 Rh*/photoreceptor/s). In some measurements, neutral density filters were inserted into (or removed from) the light path to vary the overall light level. The spectral transmittance of these filters was individually measured, and the measurements were included in the stimulus characterization. A ∼2000-μm-diameter spot was generally flashed for 300 ms, and the flash was repeated at least 10 times per trial at 1 s intervals. The exception was for some of the measurements using the dimmest backgrounds, at which the duration was 400 ms with 1.25 s intervals.
Horizontal cell responses were characterized by the peak response (maximum hyperpolarization), in which response amplitude was measured from the peak to resting level (see Figs. 2A,D, 4A). Horizontal cell action spectra were measured with monochromatic flashes (425–620 nm). For most cells (43 of 48), we corrected for nonstationarities in the response. To do so, a 520 nm reference flash was interspersed approximately every five flashes, and the responses to it were used to compute a multiplicative drift-correction factor (see Appendix B). A few cells (n = 5) were recorded without interposed 520 nm flashes, but these recordings were stable over time. Where shown in the plots, SEs of responses at each wavelength were computed from the responses across multiple trials. Appendix B describes how we analyzed the flash data to account for a horizontal cell response nonlinearity in our estimation of action spectra.
The flicker experiments used the LCD system alone to modulate a spot (horizontal cells, ∼2000 μm diameter; ganglion cells, ∼400–600 μm diameter) around the background at 2 Hz with a temporal sinusoid. The background was the same as the main background used for the flash experiments, and the cell was adapted continuously throughout the recording. The flickering stimulus was repeated for at least 16 cycles. The response was taken as the signed fundamental response amplitude (F1), with the sign indicating whether the response was best described as in-phase or out-of-phase with the stimulus modulation (after accounting for a small response delay.) Responses during the first and last stimulus cycles were excluded from the analysis.
For the flashed monochromatic spots produced using the lamp system, stimulus strength at each wavelength was characterized in terms of irradiance (quanta per square micrometer per second). For the flickering spots produced using the LCD system, when the stimulus was a mixture of two primaries, the most natural units are those of cone and rod contrast (CM, CS, and CRod) at the time of maximum excursion from the background (Brainard, 1996). Stimuli from the flash experiments were also converted to contrast units for some of the analyses.
To convert physical light spectra to contrast units, spectral sensitivities for guinea pig cone photopigments were generated using the nomogram developed by Govardovskii et al. (2000), with λmax values obtained using ERG (Jacobs and Deegan, 1994); rod spectral sensitivities were estimated directly from our spectral measurements (see Fig. 4C). From these spectral sensitivities, we computed isomerization rates and contrasts for each opsin type (M, S, and rod), using estimates of receptor properties described in Appendix A. Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, MA) code for performing conversions between physical spectra and pigment isomerization rates is available as part of the Psychophysics Toolbox (http://psychtoolbox.org) (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997).
Immunostaining.
We stained the retina with antibodies to M and S opsin to confirm previous measurements (Röhlich et al., 1994) of opsin gradients in guinea pig. An eyecup was kept in dark for 0.5 h in oxygenated Ames' medium, then fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m phosphate buffer (PB) for 1 h, and washed briefly in PB. The retina was peeled from the pigment epithelium and incubated in blocking buffer (10% normal goat serum and 5% Triton X-100 in PB with 5% sucrose) for 1 h to reduce nonspecific binding. To stain M and S opsins, the tissue was reacted with primary antibodies (in blocking buffer). For M opsin, we used mouse anti-COS1 (kindly provided by Prof. Á. Szél, Department of Human Morphology and Developmental Biology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary) (Röhlich et al., 1994) at a dilution of 1:40, and, for S opsin, we used rabbit anti-S opsin (Chemicon, Temecula, CA) at a dilution of 1:600 for 35 h at 4°C. After several washes in PB with 5% sucrose (SPB), the tissue was reacted with secondary antibodies (4 h, room temperature) to visualize the opsin staining. M opsin was tagged with FITC (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA), and S opsin was tagged with cyanine 5 (Invitrogen). Finally, after several washes in SPB, the tissue was mounted on a glass slide with the photoreceptor side up and then covered with mounting medium (Vectashield; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) and a coverslip.
The tissue was examined using a confocal microscope (Leica TCSNT; Leica, Nussloch, Germany) with 40× objective (1.25 numerical aperture) and 100× objective (1.4 numerical aperture). A preliminary scan was performed to optimize the photomultiplier tube settings. Image stacks of the photoreceptor layer from both superior and inferior retina were then obtained, with a total z-depth covering the cone outer segments. Within each image stack, cone outer segments were outlined in Volocity (Improvision; Lexington, MA) and counted manually.
Outdoor spectra of skylight and ground cover.
Using a spectraradiometer (PR-650; Photo Research, Chatsworth, CA), we measured the spectra of skylight and light reflected from ground cover (e.g., grass, leaves, fruits, tree barks, and earth) in direct sunlight or under shade of trees in a local botanic garden (Philadelphia, PA; early afternoon in July, clear sky with clouds). To convert the spectra into retinal irradiance, we used a posterior nodal distance of 5.05 mm for guinea pig eye (Do-Nascimento et al., 1991) and adjusted the pupil size used in the calculation according to the light intensity. Photopigment isomerization rates for each opsin type (M, S, and rod) were calculated from retinal irradiance as described above.
Results
Immunostaining
We confirmed the observations of Röhlich et al. (1994). Superior retina contained ∼20,000 cones/mm2. Individual cones stained for either M opsin (94%) or S opsin (6%) but not both (Fig. 1A, left). Inferior retina contained ∼24,000 cones/mm2. All cones stained for S opsin, and some also stained for M opsin (Fig. 1A, right). The prevalence of coexpression and the intensity of M-opsin staining declined toward the inferior margin of the retina. At distances >3.0 mm inferior to the optic disk, M-opsin staining was detected in fewer than 1% of the cones.
To estimate the M/S opsin ratio in inferior retina, one would need to assume that the antibodies stain each opsin with equal efficiency. Because this assumption probably does not hold (Hack and Peichl, 1999; Applebury et al., 2000; Nikonov et al., 2005), we view the measurements in inferior retina as indicating only qualitatively that S opsin is expressed much more strongly than M opsin.
Measuring horizontal cell M, S, and rod weights: flash experiments
We estimated cone contributions to horizontal cell response (“cone weights”) from the responses of the cell to monochromatic flashes. As described in Appendix B, the analysis took into account a horizontal cell contrast–response nonlinearity (Fig. 2B,E) (see Fig. 7).Figure 2 shows action spectra obtained for two cells, one from superior retina (C) and one from inferior retina (F). Fits to individual cells as weighted sums of M, S, and rod contrast spectral sensitivities are also shown in Figure 2, C and F. The fitting procedure is described in Appendix B. The fits are generally good with some deviation at the shortest wavelengths, for which the data become somewhat irregular.
There is a striking difference between the photoreceptor contrast weights obtained from the spectral fits in superior and inferior retina. Mean ± SE weights obtained in superior retina (n = 14) were as follows: M, 0.52 ± 0.03; S, 0.02 ± 0.01; rod, 0.46 ± 0.03. Mean ± SE weights obtained in inferior retina (n = 8) were as follows: M, 0.09 ± 0.01; S, 0.50 ± 0.03; rod, 0.41 ± 0.04. Rod weights did not differ significantly between superior and inferior retina (t test, p = 0.31). The distribution of weights obtained in individual horizontal cells at the main background intensity studied (4.76 log10 Rh*/rod/s) is shown in Figure 3A as green dots (superior retina) and green circles (inferior retina). The weights obtained for the two retinal regions are well separated, with M-opsin input dominant in the superior retina and S-opsin input dominant in the inferior retina.
In addition to cone input, horizontal cells also received significant input from rods at the main background intensity used. This is revealed by the fact that fits to the action spectra without a rod contribution deviate from the data (Fig. 2C,F). We return below to the question of how rod and cone weights vary with background intensity.
Our fits were obtained using M- and S-opsin spectral sensitivities based on the nomogram of Govardovskii et al. (2000), with λmax values of 430 and 529 nm (Jacobs and Deegan, 1994). Rod spectral sensitivity was derived directly from our measurements at low-light levels (see below). Because there is an ∼30 nm range in values for guinea pig S-cone photopigment λmax reported in the literature (Jacobs and Deegan, 1994; Parry and Bowmaker, 2002; Parry et al., 2004), we verified our choice of cone pigment λmax by fitting the data at which M- and S-opsin λmax values were also allowed to vary as free parameters. The optimal value of M-opsin λmax obtained was 527.5 nm for the cell shown in Figure 2C and 527.3 nm for all cells from superior retina (n = 14) at the main background. The optimal value of S-opsin λmax obtained was 433.4 nm for the cell shown in Figure 2F and 430.2 nm for all cells from inferior retina (n = 8) at the main background. These values are in good agreement with the published values of Jacobs and Deegan (1994). We used their published values in our primary analyses. Assuming a value of 410 nm for S-opsin λmax leads to a poor fit to the action spectrum shown in Figure 2F, with 2.1 times larger root mean squared error but similar weights: M, 0.07; S, 0.52; and rod, 0.41.
We estimated cone and rod weights across a broad range of background light intensities. All of the backgrounds had similar relative spectra (for the isomerization rates produced in each receptor type for each background, see supplemental Table 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). As background intensity increased from 2.08 to 5.14 log10 Rh*/rod/s, rod contribution decreased but still remained significant at the most intense background (Fig. 3). In parallel, cone input gradually increased and was evident at a background of 2.67 log10 Rh*/rod/s (2.09 log10 Rh*/M cone/s and 1.99 log10 Rh*/S cone/s) (Fig. 3A, yellow dot). The ratio of M- and S-opsin weights is relatively stable across background intensities. The red dashed lines in Figure 3A are fit to the data across background intensities and correspond to M/S ratios of 0.96:0.04 (superior retina) and 0.17:0.83 (inferior retina).
Measuring rod spectral sensitivity
Horizontal cells can receive rod input through rod–cone gap junctions (Sharpe and Stockman, 1999; Bloomfield and Dacheux, 2001; Sterling, 2004). We measured action spectra of horizontal cells against a dim background with isomerization rates of 1.50 (M), 1.39 (S), and 2.08 (rod) log10 Rh*/photoreceptor/s (Fig. 4C). At this background, the waveforms of the flash responses (Fig. 4A) resembled those reported for primate retina at low (10 troland) light levels (Smith et al., 2001). The action spectra we measured were well fit using rod spectral sensitivities derived from two types of nomograms (Baylor et al., 1987; Govardovskii et al., 2000), with λmax close to 500 nm. This value matches that reported previously for guinea pig rods (Jacobs and Deegan, 1994; Parry and Bowmaker, 2002). In addition, the similarity of the spectral sensitivity obtained from superior and inferior retina indicates minimal cone contributions for this background intensity. We fit the mean data with a polynomial and used this fit as our best estimate of rod spectral sensitivity for the other calculations reported in this paper.
Measuring horizontal cell M, S weights: flicker experiments
To estimate M- and S-cone contrast weights for the flicker experiments, we presented each cell with modulations of various combinations of M and S contrasts produced as the mixture of the blue and green primaries of the LCD system (CM and CS) (Fig. 5A, top row). Negative M contrast represents modulations in which the M component of the modulation is 180° out-of-phase with the S component. We estimated M and S contrast weights from the data using a linear–nonlinear (LNL) model (Appendix C) (Fig. 5B) (Chichilnisky, 2001; Chatterjee and Callaway, 2002; Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2002; Zaghloul et al., 2003).
Because our flicker stimuli consisted of the mixture of two primaries, we could not independently estimate rod input from the flicker data (Appendix C). Therefore, we took the estimates of rod input obtained in the flash experiment at the same background level (wRod = 0.44 ± 0.12, mean ± SD, combining measurements from superior and inferior retina) and included this in the LNL model calculations. We then found the M- and S-cone contrast weights and parameters of the static nonlinearity that best accounted for the data from each cell. The green points plotted in Figure 6A (and B) show the horizontal cell cone weights obtained in this manner. As with the flash experiments, the data reveal large differences between superior and inferior retina. As shown in the figure, these differences are robust with respect to variation in the assumption about the magnitude of rod input.
In six horizontal cells (four at main background and two at dimmer backgrounds), we were able to obtain cone weight estimates from both the flash and flicker protocols. To compare the estimates obtained from the two protocols, we reanalyzed the flicker data from these cells using the estimate of rod input obtained for the same cell from the flash data. The estimates obtained from the two methods were in good agreement. (For both M- and S- cone weights, expressed as fraction of total cone inputs, mean absolute deviation was 0.08 and maximum deviation was 0.16.)
Measuring brisk-transient ganglion cell M, S weights
We measured responses of brisk-transient ganglion cells to the flicker stimuli. Cone contrast weights for these cells were estimated using the same procedure as for the horizontal cell flicker data. As with horizontal cells, cone contrast weights to brisk-transient ganglion cells differ greatly with retinal location (Fig. 6C,D). The difference in cone weights between regions also survived variation in the assumption about the magnitude of rod input. Comparison across the two cell types, but at the same retinal location and with the same assumption about rod input, reveals similar M and S weights (Fig. 6B,D).
Discussion
Opsin expression predicts cone input to horizontal and brisk-transient ganglion cells
Cone inputs to horizontal cells correlate strongly with the dual gradients of M and S opsin demonstrated by immunostaining. In superior retina, the agreement was quantitative, as shown in Figure 6B. In inferior retina, we could not quantify the M/S opsin ratio by immunostaining; nevertheless, the two measures agreed qualitatively. Furthermore, the cone weights were the same for brisk-transient ganglion cells (Fig. 6B,D); thus, as signals flow from outer to inner retina, the weighting is preserved. This conclusion assumes that rod weights are the same for horizontal and ganglion cells. This assumption seems justified because, at the high light levels used here, the rod signal enters the cone pathway by coupling to the cone terminal electrically and provides a common signal to all downstream neurons (Sharpe and Stockman, 1999; Bloomfield and Dacheux, 2001; Sterling, 2004).
Previous studies in primate retina on L- and M-cone inputs to horizontal cells and midget and parasol ganglion cells suggested a principle that relative expression of cone opsins is preserved postreceptorally (Dacey et al., 2000; Deeb et al., 2000; Diller et al., 2004). Our finding is consistent with this principle.
Our findings about the functional consequences of the dual opsin gradient agree with those from previous studies in mouse that used regional electroretinography (Calderone and Jacobs, 1995) and that mapped the distribution of ganglion cells with different cone weights (Ekesten et al., 2000; Ekesten and Gouras, 2005). Our studies extend the previous results by quantifying the effect for two specific classes of retinal neurons.
Brisk-transient ganglion cells correspond to M cells in the primate retina, which are responsible for behavioral contrast detection (Merigan and Maunsell, 1990, 1993; Dhingra et al., 2003; Silveira et al., 2004). That these cells follow the dual opsin gradient supports the hypothesis that the opsin gradients optimize contrast detection across spectral variation along the vertical meridian. Although the ecological basis for this optimization presumably lies in the spectral shift between ground and sky (Szél et al., 2000; Osorio and Vorobyev, 2005), a detailed account awaits a fuller characterization of the guinea pig's photic environment and the signal-to-noise properties of its photoreceptors. Recent work has begun to characterize noise sources in the photoreceptors of various species and how these vary between rods and cones and between cones of different classes (Schneeweis and Schnapf, 1999; Rieke and Baylor, 2000; Burns and Baylor, 2001; Sampath and Baylor, 2002; Ala-Laurila et al., 2004; Dunn and Rieke, 2006; Nikonov et al., 2006).
Contrast versus intensity weights
In our analysis, we followed previous authors (Chatterjee and Callaway, 2002; Diller et al., 2004; Johnson et al., 2004; Horwitz et al., 2005) and derived photoreceptor weights using a contrast measure. These weights describe the relationship between the contrast of the stimuli and the response of the postreceptoral cells. The rationale for this choice is that contrast weights are invariant with respect to changes in the chromaticity and intensity of the background if photoreceptors adapt according to Weber's Law (i.e., gain for each class of receptor inversely proportional to background isomerization rate for that class). Weber's Law provides a reasonable first-order description of photoreceptor adaptation over much but not all of their operating range (Demontis et al., 1993; Schneeweis and Schnapf, 1999). Indeed, if Weber's Law adaptation perfectly described the behavior of the coupling between photoreceptors and postreceptoral neurons, the relative contributions of rods and cones would not change with background intensity when expressed as contrast weights. This prediction is clearly falsified by our data (Fig. 3) for large changes in background intensity.
Because there are deviations from Weber's Law adaptation, it is worth considering whether our conclusions depend strongly on the choice to use contrast weights. Intensity weights describe the relationship between the incremental intensity of the stimuli seen by each photoreceptor class and cell response. We describe this reanalysis and show the results in the supplemental material (particularly supplemental Figs. 1, 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). None of the basic conclusions of the paper are sensitive to the choice of whether to evaluate receptor contribution in terms of contrast or incremental intensity. In particular, the relative M- and S-cone weights are essentially similar in the intensity weight analysis. This invariance is in part a consequence of the fact that we carefully chose the chromaticity of our backgrounds so that they produced similar isomerization rates in the M and S cones.
The biggest change resulting from the reanalysis in terms of intensity weights is a reduction in the estimated magnitude of rod contribution. Note, however, that the spectral fits shown in Figure 2, C and F, do not change when the weights are expressed in terms of intensity instead of contrast, so that the deviations for the no-rod fits are invariant with respect to this choice.
Rods contribute to responses at high light levels
Mammalian rods, including those of guinea pig, show Weber's Law adaptation at background levels up to ∼4 log10 Rh*/rod/s (Tamura et al., 1989; Nakatani et al., 1991; Demontis et al., 1993). These measurements were made using suction microelectrode recording from isolated rod outer segments and relatively short adaptation times. If such adaptation continues to even higher light levels in whole-mount retina under the steady adaptation conditions that we used, it could explain why we observe rod contributions at yet higher light levels (5.14 log10 Rh*/rod/s).
Our spectral measurements show that light reflected from ground in shade produced 4.2–6.4 log10 Rh*/rod/s in guinea pig, whereas light reflected from ground in direct sunlight produced 5.0–6.8 log10 Rh*/rod/s. These comparisons suggest that rods contribute substantially to daylight vision.
Appendix A: photoreceptor parameters
Table 1 provides the parameters we used to determine the photoreceptor sensitivities and peak isomerization probabilities. For cones, the value of λmax was used in conjunction with the nomogram developed by Govardovskii et al. (2000) to generate the relative cone sensitivity. This was then adjusted for pigment self-screening and scaled according to the peak isomerization probability derived using the other constants given in Table 1. For rods, the spectral sensitivity was determined from a polynomial fit to horizontal cell action spectra measured on the dimmest background. This was then scaled to fit the rod sensitivity derived from the nomogram computed with λmax = 500 nm, and the peak isomerization sensitivity was taken from this scaled version.
Table 1.
Photoreceptor type | M cone | S cone | Rod | Units | Source |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
λmax | 529 | 430 | 500 | nm | (Jacobs and Deegan, 1994) |
Outer segment length | 8.0 | 8.0 | 16.2 | Unpublished data from our laboratory | |
Outer segment diameter | 2.0 | 2.0 | 2.0 | μm | Unpublished data from our laboratory |
Inner segment diameter | 2.8 | 2.8 | 2.4 | μm | Unpublished data from our laboratory |
Axial specific density | 0.0045 | 0.004 | 0.011 | μm−1 | (Parry and Bowmaker, 2002) |
Axial optical density | 0.036 | 0.032 | 0.170 | Calculated from other parameters | |
Peak isomerization probability | 0.053 | 0.047 | 0.229 | Calculated from other parameters |
Appendix B: estimating action spectra from flash data for horizontal cells
This appendix describes how we estimate horizontal cell action spectra. There are two key steps to the processing. The first step normalizes the measured response at each wavelength to that of a reference flash presented at 520 nm. This step compensates for a drift in gain that can occur during measurements from a single cell. The second step corrects for a static nonlinearity in the contrast–response function of horizontal cells.
Drift correction
As described in Materials and Methods, we measured the responses of horizontal cells to reference flashes presented at 520 nm. The intensity of the reference flashes was held constant for each cell but was chosen individually for each cell so as to produce an easily measurable response. We used linear interpolation on the responses to the reference flashes to provide an estimate of how the cell would have responded to the reference flash at any time during the experiment. We divided the measured response to each test flash by the estimate of the reference flash response at the corresponding time. This produced a drift-corrected response Rc(λ) = R(λ)/R(520) for each test flash, where R(λ) is the raw measured response to a test flash of wavelength λ, and R(520) denotes the interpolated estimate of the response of the reference flash response at the time of the test flash. We used this procedure for 43 of 48 cells. For the other five cells, interspersed reference flash measurements were not made, and, for these cells, we took Rc(λ) = R(λ).
Correction for static nonlinearity
If horizontal cells responded linearly with flash stimulus contrast at all wavelengths, we could convert drift-corrected responses to action spectra directly by dividing the drift-corrected response at each wavelength by the flash intensity I(λ) at that wavelength. This procedure, however, would produce distorted action spectra estimates if some of the flashes drove the cells outside of their linear operating range. To avoid such distortion, we corrected for response nonlinearity using an LNL model (Chichilnisky, 2001; Chatterjee and Callaway, 2002; Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2002; Zaghloul et al., 2003).
The LNL model describes the response of the cell as the result of passing an input contrast Cinput(λ) through a nonlinear response function. The input contrast is taken as the weighted sum of the contrasts CM(λ), CS(λ), and CRod(λ) seen by the M cones, S cones, and rods in response to the test flash at wavelength λ:
Here, uMc, uSc, and uRodc are the M-cone, S-cone, and rod contrast weights. The values of CM(λ), CS(λ), and CRod(λ) are calculated from the spectral sensitivity of the photoreceptors SMi(λ), SSi(λ), and SRodi(λ), the test flash intensity I(λ), and the isomerization rates seen by each receptor class to the background. We denote these background isomerization rates as RhM,bg*, RhS,bg*, and RhRod,bg*. The contrasts are then computed as CM(λ) = I(λ)SMi(λ)/RhM,bg*, CS(λ) = I(λ)SSi(λ)/RhS,bg*, and CRod(λ) = I(λ)SRodi(λ)/RhRod,bg*.
The nonlinear response function is taken to have the form
The parameters C0, n, and Rmax denote the semi-saturation constant, exponent, and maximum response amplitude. For each background level, we took the parameters C0 and n as common to all horizontal cells, whereas the value of parameter Rmax varied from cell to cell.
We analyzed the data from each background level separately. To fit the LNL model to the horizontal cell data from each background, we used an iterative procedure. First, we initialized the action spectrum of each cell under the assumption that the cell responded linearly. We then obtained initial estimates of uMc, uSc, and uRodc for each cell by fitting the action spectrum as the weighted sum of the contrast sensitivities of the three photoreceptor types. This initial fit allowed us to proceed by iterating between two steps.
The first step determined the parameters of the static nonlinearity that best accounted for the data from all cells at the same background. The second step redetermined the weights uMc, uSc, and uRodc for each cell, after correcting for the common nonlinearity determined in the first step. We found that this procedure converged rapidly and accounted for the data well.
Step 1: fitting the nonlinearity
Given values for uMc, uSc, and uRodc for each cell, we can compute the value of Cinput(λ) for every test flash presented to every cell. We assumed that individual cells could have different Rmax. As a numerical convenience, rather than fitting Rmax to each cell, we scaled the drift-corrected responses from each cell so that the initial slope of the contrast–response function [evaluated for data corresponding to Cinput(λ) ≤ 0.45] was the same. We then used numerical search to determine C0, n, and a single Rmaxscaled that best accounted for the set of scaled responses for all cells.
Step 2: correcting for the nonlinearity in the estimation of uMc, uSc, and uRodc
Given the results of step 1, we used numerical search to find the parameters uMc, uSc, and uRodc and Rmax that best fit the drift-corrected responses Rc(λ) responses for each cell, through Equations B1 and Equations B1. During this fit, we held C0 and n constant at the values obtained in step 1.
We then iterated the procedure, using contrast weights uMc, uSc, and uRodc obtained in step 2 to repeat step 1 and so forth. The error measure minimized in both steps 1 and 2 was the root mean squared error between the log10 transformed values of the test flash Rc(λ) and their predictions from the model. When the fit error after the first step was reduced by <1% compared with the previous iteration, we performed one final execution of the second step and then terminated the procedure.Figure 7 shows the nonlinearity obtained for the main background after the final step, along with the entire set of test flash data for that background. The plotted data are scaled as described for step 1.Figure 2, B and E, shows the same nonlinearity with the data (scaled in the same way) from two individual cells.
We applied this procedure separately for the horizontal cell flash data from each background, except for the dimmest background. On the dimmest background, the responses to our set of presented flashes were within the linear regimen (Fig. 4B), and, for this background intensity, we simply adopted the linear approximation.
The iterative analysis provides us with direct estimates of the photoreceptor contrast weights for each cell (uMc, uSc, and uRodc), and, from these, we can construct a predicted action spectrum. To correct the data for comparison with this prediction (Fig. 2C,F), we invert the static nonlinearity using the result of the final iteration of step 2. This allows us to compute an action spectrum A(λ) from the drift-corrected responses Rc(λ) through
Each datum plotted in Figure 2, C and F, was obtained using Equation B3. Only the shape of the action spectra A(λ) are of interest here, so the plotted data and predictions shown for each cell were shifted vertically by a common amount on the log10 sensitivity axis so that the theoretical value A(520) = 1. Similarly, we report relative contrast weights
in this study, because a scale factor common to uMc, uSc, and uRodc trades off perfectly with the semi-saturation constant through Equation B2. Indeed, the normalization in Equation B4 was applied during the fitting process to allow the parameter search to converge to a unique value of C0.
Appendix C: LNL model for horizontal and ganglion cell flicker data
To estimate M- and S-cone contrast weights from the flicker data, we also used the LNL model to describe cell responses (see Appendix B, Eqs. B1 and B2) (Fig. 5B). We fit the parameters of the model to each cell separately and also allowed a parameter that could account for the spontaneous activity of the cells. Because the flicker stimuli were constructed as the superposition of two primaries, the flicker data do not allow unique identification of wMc, wSc, and wRodc (see below). Thus, in analyzing the flicker data, we fixed wRodc at the value determined from the flash data for horizontal cells. (We also explored the effect of variation of the assumed wRodc on the obtained wMc and wSc.) In the fits, we constrained wMc and wSc to be positive by varying the value of wRodc if necessary. Predictions from LNL model generally accounted for the actual response data well (Fig. 5A). In the following, we show that the shape of the nonlinearity derived from the LNL model is independent of the strength of assumed rod input, when the stimuli are produced as a mixture of two primaries.
Static nonlinearity is independent of assumption about magnitude of rod contribution
We show that, for any choice of wM, wS, and w′Rod and w′Rod, we can find w′M and w′S, such that
with w′M and w′S independent of the color direction of the stimulus. This result means that the effect of varying the assumed rod weight may be compensated for perfectly by adjusting the M- and S-cone weights, so that the both the quality of the best fit to the data and the parameters of the static nonlinearity are independent of the assumed rod weight.
We proceed by showing that, when the flicker stimulus is the mixture of two primaries, we can express
where qM and qS depend on the spectra of the primaries, the spectral sensitivities of the photoreceptors, and the background but not on the color direction of the flickering stimulus. Using Equation C2, we rewrite
This establishes Equation C1 from C2 with w′M = (wM + qMwRod − qMw′Rod) and w′S = (wS + qMwRod − qSw′Rod).
To show the Equations C2 holds, we note that, when flicker is the mixture of two primaries (here green and blue), we can write CM = vMGΔPG + vMBΔPB and CS = vSGΔPG + vSBΔPB, where ΔPG is the flicker amplitude of the green primary, ΔPB is the flicker amplitude of the blue primary, and the constants v are weights that specify how strongly modulating each primary affects the contrast seen by each type of cone. We can invert this two-by-two system of linear equations to obtain expressions for the primary modulations in terms of the cone contrasts: ΔPG = vGMCM + vGSCS and ΔPB = vBMCM + vBSCS. Finally, the rod contrast is also the weighted sum of the primary modulations, so we have
This is in the same form as Equation C2, with qM = (vRodGvGM + vRodBvBM) and qS = (vRodGvGS + vRodBvBS).
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Eye Institute Grants EY10016, EY08124, and P30 EY001583 and National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH48168. We thank Ágoston Szél for providing cone opsin antibodies, Noga Vardi for providing advice on immunostaining, John Andrews-Labenski, Michael Suplick, Fred Letterio, Jian Li, and Marie Fina for technical assistance, and Sharron Fina for manuscript preparation.
References
- Ahnelt PK, Kolb H. The mammalian photoreceptor mosaic-adaptive design. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2000;19:711–777. doi: 10.1016/s1350-9462(00)00012-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ala-Laurila P, Donner K, Koskelainen A. Thermal activation and photoactivation of visual pigments. Biophys J. 2004;86:3653–3662. doi: 10.1529/biophysj.103.035626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Applebury ML, Antoch MP, Baxter LC, Chun LL, Falk JD, Farhangfar F, Kage K, Krzystolik MG, Lyass LA, Robbins JT. The murine cone photoreceptor: a single cone type expresses both S and M opsins with retinal spatial patterning. Neuron. 2000;27:513–523. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00062-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baylor DA, Nunn BJ, Schnapf JL. Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol (Lond) 1987;390:145–160. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloomfield SA, Dacheux RF. Rod vision: pathways and processing in the mammalian retina. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2001;20:351–384. doi: 10.1016/s1350-9462(00)00031-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH. Cone contrast and opponent modulation color spaces. In: Kaiser PK, Boynton RM, editors. Human color vision. Ed 2. Washington, DC: Optical Society of America; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10:433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burns ME, Baylor DA. Activation, deactivation, and adaptation in vertebrate photoreceptor cells. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:779–805. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calderone JB, Jacobs GH. Regional variations in the relative sensitivity to UV light in the mouse retina. Vis Neurosci. 1995;12:463–468. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800008361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calkins DJ, Sterling P. Evidence that circuits for spatial and color vision segregate at the first retinal synapse. Neuron. 1999;24:313–321. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80846-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chatterjee S, Callaway EM. S cone contributions to the magnocellular visual pathway in macaque monkey. Neuron. 2002;35:1135–1146. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00874-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chichilnisky EJ. A simple white noise analysis of neuronal light responses. Network. 2001;12:199–213. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chichilnisky EJ, Kalmar RS. Functional asymmetries in ON and OFF ganglion cells of primate retina. J Neurosci. 2002;22:2737–2747. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02737.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleland BG, Levick WR, Wassle H. Physiological identification of a morphological class of cat retinal ganglion cells. J Physiol (Lond) 1975;248:151–171. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp010967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM, Diller LC, Verweij J, Williams DR. Physiology of L- and M-cone inputs to H1 horizontal cells in the primate retina. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2000;17:589–596. doi: 10.1364/josaa.17.000589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deeb SS, Diller LC, Williams DR, Dacey DM. Interindividual and topographical variation of L:M cone ratios in monkey retinas. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2000;17:538–544. doi: 10.1364/josaa.17.000538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demb JB, Zaghloul K, Haarsma L, Sterling P. Bipolar cells contribute to nonlinear spatial summation in the brisk-transient (Y) ganglion cell in mammalian retina. J Neurosci. 2001;21:7447–7454. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-19-07447.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demontis GC, Bisti S, Cervetto L. Light sensitivity, adaptation and saturation in mammalian rods. Prog Brain Res. 1993;95:15–24. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)60353-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhingra NK, Kao YH, Sterling P, Smith RG. Contrast threshold of a brisk-transient ganglion cell in vitro. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:2360–2369. doi: 10.1152/jn.01042.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diller L, Packer OS, Verweij J, McMahon MJ, Williams DR, Dacey DM. L and M cone contributions to the midget and parasol ganglion cell receptive fields of macaque monkey retina. J Neurosci. 2004;24:1079–1088. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3828-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Do-Nascimento JL, Do-Nascimento RS, Damasceno BA, Silveira LC. The neurons of the retinal ganglion cell layer of the guinea pig: quantitative analysis of their distribution and size. Braz J Med Biol Res. 1991;24:199–214. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn FA, Rieke F. The impact of photoreceptor noise on retinal gain controls. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:363–370. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.06.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekesten B, Gouras P. Cone and rod inputs to murine retinal ganglion cells: evidence of cone opsin specific channels. Vis Neurosci. 2005;22:893–903. doi: 10.1017/S0952523805226172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekesten B, Gouras P, Yamamoto S. Cone inputs to murine retinal ganglion cells. Vision Res. 2000;40:2573–2577. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00122-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Govardovskii VI, Fyhrquist N, Reuter T, Kuzmin DG, Donner K. In search of the visual pigment template. Vis Neurosci. 2000;17:509–528. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800174036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant S, Patel NN, Philp AR, Grey CN, Lucas RD, Foster RG, Bowmaker JK, Jeffery G. Rod photopigment deficits in albinos are specific to mammals and arise during retinal development. Vis Neurosci. 2001;18:245–251. doi: 10.1017/s095252380118209x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hack I, Peichl L. Horizontal cells of the rabbit retina are non-selectively connected to the cones. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:2261–2274. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00647.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haverkamp S, Wassle H, Duebel J, Kuner T, Augustine GJ, Feng G, Euler T. The primordial, blue-cone color system of the mouse retina. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5438–5445. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1117-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horwitz GD, Chichilnisky EJ, Albright TD. Blue-yellow signals are enhanced by spatiotemporal luminance contrast in macaque V1. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:2263–2278. doi: 10.1152/jn.00743.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ilia M, Jeffery G. Retinal cell addition and rod production depend on early stages of ocular melanin synthesis. J Comp Neurol. 2000;420:437–444. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs GH. The distribution and nature of colour vision among the mammals. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 1993;68:413–471. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185x.1993.tb00738.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs GH, Deegan JF., II Spectral sensitivity, photopigments, and color vision in the guinea pig (Cavia porcellus) Behav Neurosci. 1994;108:993–1004. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.5.993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeffery G, Brem G, Montoliu L. Correction of retinal abnormalities found in albinism by introduction of a functional tyrosinase gene in transgenic mice and rabbits. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1997;99:95–102. doi: 10.1016/s0165-3806(96)00211-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson EN, Hawken MJ, Shapley R. Cone inputs in macaque primary visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:2501–2514. doi: 10.1152/jn.01043.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jusuf PR, Martin PR, Grunert U. Random wiring in the midget pathway of primate retina. J Neurosci. 2006;26:3908–3917. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4891-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merigan WH, Maunsell JH. Macaque vision after magnocellular lateral geniculate lesions. Vis Neurosci. 1990;5:347–352. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800000432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merigan WH, Maunsell JH. How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annu Rev Neurosci. 1993;16:369–402. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakatani K, Tamura T, Yau KW. Light adaptation in retinal rods of the rabbit and two other nonprimate mammals. J Gen Physiol. 1991;97:413–435. doi: 10.1085/jgp.97.3.413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikonov SS, Daniele LL, Zhu X, Craft CM, Swaroop A, Pugh EN., Jr Photoreceptors of Nrl−/− mice coexpress functional S- and M-cone opsins having distinct inactivation mechanisms. J Gen Physiol. 2005;125:287–304. doi: 10.1085/jgp.200409208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikonov SS, Kholodenko R, Lem J, Pugh EN., Jr Physiological features of the S- and M-cone photoreceptors of wild-type mice from single-cell recordings. J Gen Physiol. 2006;127:359–374. doi: 10.1085/jgp.200609490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osorio D, Vorobyev M. Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities in terrestrial animals: adaptations for luminance and colour vision. Proc Biol Sci. 2005;272:1745–1752. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parry JW, Bowmaker JK. Visual pigment coexpression in Guinea pig cones: a microspectrophotometric study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002;43:1662–1665. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parry JW, Poopalasundaram S, Bowmaker JK, Hunt DM. A novel amino acid substitution is responsible for spectral tuning in a rodent violet-sensitive visual pigment. Biochemistry. 2004;43:8014–8020. doi: 10.1021/bi049478w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peichl L. Diversity of mammalian photoreceptor properties: adaptations to habitat and lifestyle? Anat Rec A Discov Mol Cell Evol Biol. 2005;287:1001–1012. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peichl L, Gonzalez-Soriano J. Morphological types of horizontal cell in rodent retinae: a comparison of rat, mouse, gerbil, and guinea pig. Vis Neurosci. 1994;11:501–517. doi: 10.1017/s095252380000242x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. 1997;10:437–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rieke F, Baylor DA. Origin and functional impact of dark noise in retinal cones. Neuron. 2000;26:181–186. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81148-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Röhlich P, van Veen T, Szél Á. Two different visual pigments in one retinal cone cell. Neuron. 1994;13:1159–1166. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90053-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampath AP, Baylor DA. Molecular mechanism of spontaneous pigment activation in retinal cones. Biophys J. 2002;83:184–193. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(02)75160-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandmann D, Boycott BB, Peichl L. Blue-cone horizontal cells in the retinae of horses and other equidae. J Neurosci. 1996;16:3381–3396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03381.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneeweis DM, Schnapf JL. The photovoltage of macaque cone photoreceptors: adaptation, noise, and kinetics. J Neurosci. 1999;19:1203–1216. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-04-01203.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe LT, Stockman A. Rod pathways: the importance of seeing nothing. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:497–504. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01458-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silveira LC, Saito CA, Lee BB, Kremers J, da Silva Filho M, Kilavik BE, Yamada ES, Perry VH. Morphology and physiology of primate M- and P-cells. Prog Brain Res. 2004;144:21–46. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)14402-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith VC, Pokorny J, Lee BB, Dacey DM. Primate horizontal cell dynamics: an analysis of sensitivity regulation in the outer retina. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85:545–558. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.2.545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterling P. How retinal circuits optimize the transfer of visual information. In: Chalupa LM, Werner JS, editors. Visual neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 2004. pp. 234–259. [Google Scholar]
- Szél Á, Lukáts Á, Fekete T, Szepessy Z, Röhlich P. Photoreceptor distribution in the retinas of subprimate mammals. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2000;17:568–579. doi: 10.1364/josaa.17.000568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamura T, Nakatani K, Yau KW. Light adaptation in cat retinal rods. Science. 1989;245:755–758. doi: 10.1126/science.2772634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vingrys AJ, Bui BV. Development of postreceptoral function in pigmented and albino guinea pigs. Vis Neurosci. 2001;18:605–613. doi: 10.1017/s0952523801184105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaghloul KA, Boahen K, Demb JB. Different circuits for ON and OFF retinal ganglion cells cause different contrast sensitivities. J Neurosci. 2003;23:2645–2654. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-07-02645.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]