Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2007 Apr 20.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry. 2006 Oct 25;61(5):599–608. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.017

Personality Traits of Agreeableness and Extraversion are Associated with ADH4 Variation

Xingguang Luo 1, Henry R Kranzler 1, Lingjun Zuo 1, Shuang Wang 1, Joel Gelernter 1
PMCID: PMC1853245  NIHMSID: NIHMS20099  PMID: 17069770

Abstract

Background

Personality traits are associated with substance dependence (SD); genetic factors may influence both. Strong associations between ADH4 variation and SD have been reported. We aimed to investigate the relationship between ADH4 variation and personality traits in the present study.

Methods

We assessed dimensions of the five-factor model of personality in 243 subjects with SD (175 European Americans [EAs] and 68 African Americans [AAs]) and 296 healthy control subjects (256 EAs and 40 AAs). We also genotyped 7 ADH4 markers (spanning the locus) and 38 unlinked ancestry-informative markers in these subjects. The relationships between the diplotypes, alleles, and genotypes at ADH4 and personality traits were examined using multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), controlling for potential confounders.

Results

Generally, SD patients, older individuals, and male subjects scored higher on neuroticism and lower on other personality factors. Personality factors were associated with the diplotypes. The allele A or genotype A/A of single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP)6 (rs1800759 at the gene promoter) was significantly associated with agreeableness scores. There were associations between extraversion and SNP1 (hcv2033010 at the 3′ end) and SNP2 (rs1042364 in exon 9) in subjects with higher conscientiousness scores.

Conclusions

The personality traits of agreeableness and extraversion are related to ADH4 polymorphism. Among the ADH4 markers that appear to predispose to certain personality traits, the functional variant rs1800759 (SNP6) in the promoter region is most important. We conclude that personality traits and SD have a partially overlapping genetic basis.

Keywords: ADH4, MANCOVA, personality traits, substance dependence


According to the five-factor model of personality (Digman 1990), the full range of personality traits can be well defined in terms of five basic dimensions. These dimensions are: factor I (extraversion or surgency), which contrasts such traits as talkativeness, assertiveness, and activity level with silence, passivity, and reserve; factor II (agreeableness or pleasantness), which contrasts kindness, trust, and warmth with hostility, selfishness, and distrust; factor III (conscientiousness or dependability), which contrasts organization, thoroughness, and reliability with carelessness, negligence, and unreliability; factor IV (neuroticism versus emotional stability), which includes nervousness, moodiness, and temperamentality; and factor V (openness to experience or intellect), which contrasts imagination, curiosity, and creativity with shallowness and imperceptiveness (Goldberg 1993).

This structure of traits in the five-factor model is consistent among highly diverse cultures with distinct languages and between men and women, older and younger adults, and European Americans (EAs) and non-European Americans (Costa et al 1991; McCrae and Costa 1997). This universality could reflect a common biological basis of personality traits across distinct cultures, populations, ages, and sexes. Based on its wide applicability and consistency, the five-factor model has been widely accepted.

Genetic factors have been implicated as contributing to individual difference in major dimensions of personality traits, evidenced by a linkage study and many association studies. A genomewide-scan linkage study mapped five trait-influencing sites for neuroticism at chromosomes 1q, 4q, 7p, 12q, and 13q (Fullerton et al 2003). Subjects expressing one or both S alleles of a regulatory region polymorphism at the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) tended to score lower on the NEO Five-Factor Inventory factor of agreeableness than subjects homozygous for the L allele; within this factor, the facet subscale of tendermindedness was lower for subjects expressing one or both S alleles compared with subjects homozygous for the L allele (p = .003) (Wand et al 2002). There have also been reports of an association between this polymorphism and harm avoidance (Wiesbeck et al 2004), neuroticism (Du et al 2000; Lesch et al 1996), violent behavior (Retz et al 2004), or other anxiety-related personality traits (Melke et al 2001). Many other studies have also reported on the relationship of personality traits with other candidate loci, such as variation at the 5-HT2C gene (Ebstein et al 1997), the 5-HT2A gene (Golimbet et al 2002), the DRD2 gene (Jonsson et al 2003; Lee et al 2003; Ponce et al 2003; Rosmond et al 2001), the DRD3 gene (Thome et al 1999), the DRD4 gene (Bau et al 1999; Gelernter et al 1997; Lee et al 2003), the COMT gene (Rujescu et al 2003; Tsai et al 2004), the BDNF gene (Itoh et al 2004; Lang et al 2004, 2005; Sen et al 2003), the OPRM1 gene (Wand et al 2002), the TFAP2B gene (Damberg et al 2000), and the ESR1 gene (Westberg et al 2003). These findings provide additional support for the hypothesis that personality traits are influenced by genetic factors.

Personality traits may play a central role in the development of substance dependence (SD; including alcohol dependence and opioid dependence in the studies by Caspi et al 1997; Cloninger 1987; Cloninger et al 1988; Loper et al 1973; Verheul et al 2004; Zucker and Lisansky Gomberg 1986). Specifically, some premorbid personality traits, such as “behavioral under-control” (including impulsivity, thrill seeking, rebelliousness, irresponsibility, nonconformity, and aggressiveness) (Sher et al 1991), rejection of societal values, antisocial behavior, and hyperactivity, are robust predictors of alcohol dependence (Cox et al 1983; Otter and Martin 1996). In contrast, it has been argued that personality traits, especially “negative emotionality” (anxiousness, inhibition, moodiness, and unhappiness), may be a consequence rather than a cause of alcohol dependence (Schuckit 1986).

Common genetic factors may largely underlie the association between personality traits and alcohol dependence. For example, in a community sample of 2682 twins, Slutske et al (1998) found that 70% of the association between antisociality and alcohol dependence was explained by common genetic factors; this study also showed that alcohol dependence and conduct disorder shared many personality traits (also confirmed by Krueger et al 2000; Sher and Trull 1994) that could be accounted for by common genetic risk factors. In a follow-up study, these authors confirmed the finding that these personality dimensions shared genetic risk factors with alcohol dependence (Slutske et al 2002). Studies of the offspring of alcoholics suggested that behavioral undercontrol might be related to the familial diathesis underlying alcohol dependence risk (e.g., Finn et al 2000; Sher et al 1991, 1999). There is also direct evidence that personality traits and alcohol dependence are linked to polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) and the μ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) (Sander et al 1998a, 1998b). These findings support the idea of a shared genetic basis for personality features and alcohol dependence.

The development of alcohol dependence depends on heavy ethanol consumption, which appears to be influenced by alcohol dehydrogenase (ADH) activity. Increased ADH activity can increase the rate of alcohol metabolism, thereby increasing the production of the toxin acetaldehyde. If acetaldehyde is not metabolized quickly, it accumulates, resulting in an aversive “flushing reaction,” which can limit alcohol consumption and, hypothetically, thereby reduce the risk for alcohol dependence (Hasin et al 2002; Muramatsu et al 1995; Thomasson et al 1994). Conversely, decreased ADH activity may increase the risk for alcohol dependence (Hasin et al 2002; Thomasson et al 1994). Human ADH4 enzyme mainly contributes to liver ADH activity, and at intoxicating levels of alcohol, it may account for as much as 40% of the total ethanol oxidation rate (Ditlow et al 1984). A polymorphism mapped to the promoter region of the ADH4 locus at 4q22 (e.g., single nucleotide polymorphism [SNP]6: −75A/C [rs1800759]) could modulate ADH activity (Edenberg et al 1999). Guindalini et al (2005) reported that promoter variants, including −75A/C and −159A/G, were significantly associated with alcohol dependence in European Brazilians and African Brazilians. We (Luo et al 2006a) found that seven ADH4 markers (including −75A/C) were in strong linkage disequilibrium (LD) to form a haplotype block in EA and African American (AA) substance dependent subjects and healthy control subjects. The genotype frequency distributions of the seven ADH4 markers were in Hardy-Weinberg Disequilibrium (HWD) in the EA patients with SD but were in Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium (HWE) in the EA healthy control subjects and the AA cases and control subjects. The genotypes of seven ADH4 markers were significantly associated with alcohol and/or drug dependence in EAs. Among these markers in EAs, SNP6 and SNP2 (rs1042364) were most significant HWD in cases with alcohol dependence and drug dependence, respectively; also, these two markers were most significantly associated with alcohol dependence and drug dependence, respectively. Fine mapping the risk loci with an HWD measure (Feder et al 1996) revealed that these two markers were closest to the risk loci for alcohol dependence and drug dependence, respectively. After correcting for the effects of allele frequencies of markers on HWD (Jiang et al 2001), SNP6 was the marker closest to the risk loci for both alcohol dependence and drug dependence.

We hypothesized that personality traits would also be associated with ADH4 variation, due to their strong genetic link to SD and the strong association between SD and ADH4. The present study aimed to investigate the role of these seven ADH4 markers in determining differences in personality traits among individuals.

Methods and Materials

Subjects

Two hundred forty-three subjects with SD (175 EAs and 68 AAs) and 296 healthy control subjects (256 EAs and 40 AAs) were included in the present study. This is a subsample of the subjects among whom we previously studied the relationships between SD and ADH4 (Luo et al 2006a). The patients (140 male patients; 103 female patients) met lifetime DSM-III-R or DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association 1987, 1994) and had a diagnosis of alcohol dependence (n = 192) and/or drug dependence (n = 144 for cocaine dependence; n = 95 for opioid dependence). Diagnoses were made using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID) (Spitzer et al 1992), the computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for DSM-III-R (C-DIS-R) (Blouin et al 1988), or a checklist comprised of DSM-III-R symptoms. The control subjects (111 male subjects; 185 female subjects) were recruited by advertisement from the general population, with many recruited from local college campuses. The control subjects were screened using the SCID or the C-DIS-R to exclude major Axis I mental disorders, including alcohol or drug dependence, psychotic disorders (including schizophrenia or schizophrenia-like disorders), mood disorders, and anxiety disorders. The average ages were 37.9 ± 9.3 years for cases and 27.6 ± 8.6 years for control subjects.

The subjects were recruited at the University of Connecticut Health Center. All subjects gave informed consent before participating in the study, which was approved by the Institutional Review Board.

Marker Inclusion

Seven single nucleotide polymorphisms (all of those available from the public Applied Biosystems, Inc., ABI, Foster City, California, at the time of genotyping, designated by hcv# or rs#) were included in this study. These markers included SNP1 (hcv2033010 at the 3’ end), SNP2 (rs1042364 in exon 9), SNP3 (rs1126671 in exon 7), SNP4 (rs1126670 in exon 6), SNP5 (rs7694646 in intron 4), SNP6 (rs1800759 in the promoter region), and SNP7 (rs1984362 at the 5’ end). The allele frequencies and polymerase chain reaction (PCR) conditions for these markers have been validated by ABI. These markers, spanning the full length of ADH4, cover a range of 28,752 base pairs (bp), with an average spacing of 4792 bp. We have previously reported the relationships between these markers and risk for drug and alcohol dependence in a larger sample that includes these subjects (Luo et al 2005b, 2006a).

Thirty-eight ancestry-informative markers unlinked to ADH4, including 37 short tandem repeats (STR)s and one Duffy antigen gene (FY) marker (rs2814778), were genotyped to detect the population structure and obtain the ancestry proportions of our sample. These marker sets have been employed in the studies by Stein et al (2004), Kaufman et al (2004), Zhang et al (2006), and Luo et al (2005a, 2005b, 2006b), and their characteristics were described in the studies by Yang et al (2005) and Luo et al (2005b).

Genotyping

The seven ADH4 SNPs were genotyped with a fluorogenic 5′ nuclease assay method, i.e., the TaqMan technique (Shi et al 1999), using the ABI PRISM 7900 Sequence Detection System (ABI, Foster City, California). Polymerase chain reaction was performed in a final volume of 3 μL, including 1.5 μl 2X TaqMan Universal PCR Master Mix (ABI), .075 μL 20X Assays-on-Demand or .0375 μL 40X Assays-by-Design (ABI), .03 μL 100X BSA (New England Biolabs, Inc., Beverly, Massachusetts), and 1 ng DNA. All genotyping was performed in duplicate and compared to ensure validity of the data. Mismatched genotypes, which constituted<.5% of the total number of duplicate genotypes performed, were discarded. The 38 ancestry-informative markers were genotyped by polymerase chain reaction-restriction fragment length polymorphism (PCR-RFLP) technique or fluorescence capillary electrophoresis technique (more details are provided by Luo et al 2005b).

Assessment of Personality

Personality dimensions in patients and healthy individuals were assessed with the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI), a 60-item self-report questionnaire. This questionnaire is rated on a five-point scale to yield scores in five major domains of personality and requires a sixth-grade reading level. Usually, this questionnaire requires 10 to 15 minutes to complete. Internal consistency values (Cronbach’s alpha coefficient) for the NEO-FFI Form S ranges from .68 to .86 (Costa and McCrae 1997). Pairwise correlation analysis was performed on these traits by Pearson’s method. Some of the personality phenotype data were included in our earlier reports addressing the relationship of personality to DRD4 (Gelernter et al 1997) and SLC6A4 (Gelernter et al 1998).

Estimation of Ancestry Proportions and Reconstruction of Haplotypes

To detect admixture, measure its extent in our sample, and estimate ancestry proportions for each individual, we used the ancestry information content from the set of 38 ancestry-informative markers through the program STRUCTURE, which is based on the Bayesian approach (Falush et al 2003; Pritchard et al 2000).

The program PHASE was used to reconstruct haplotypes in this study (Niu et al 2002; Stephens et al 2001; Stephens and Donnely 2003). This program is based on a combination of the Bayesian statistical method and the so-called “divide-and-conquer” (i.e., partition ligation) strategy and has no requirement for the assumption of Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium. Diplotype (i.e., haplo-type pair) probabilities for each individual were estimated by PHASE.

Data Analysis

Multivariate Analysis of Covariance

Multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), implemented in SPSS 13.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois), was employed to test associations between ADH4 and personality factors. In the MANCOVA model, five personality factors that are correlated served as one composite response variable; diplotype probabilities served as predictor variables; diagnosis, age, ancestry proportions, and sex served as covariates; and the two-way interactions between each predictor variable and each covariate were also considered as independent predictor variables in this model. Statistical significance was based on the value of Pillai’s Trace statistic.

These seven markers were all in HWD. We have demonstrated that diplotypes based on such markers could be more powerful and valid than haplotypes in association analysis (Luo et al 2005b). Thus, only diplotypewise analysis was considered in this study.

The gene frequency distributions are highly significantly different between EAs and AAs (see Table 1), and EAs and AAs were both observed to be admixed populations (Luo et al 2005b). The population stratification and admixture effects could be important in our mixed sample and should be considered. Thus, ancestry proportions were included in the MANCOVA model to exclude both population stratification and admixture effects. Diagnosis (i.e., affection status), age, and sex were included in the model to exclude their potential confounding effects on associations. They have been reported to influence personality scores (Hernandez-Avila et al 2004); thus, it is reasonable and predictable that gene effects on personality scores could be different in different diagnosis, age, or sex subgroups. The probabilities, rather than the categories, of diplotypes were included because the probabilities preserve more information than directly using the categorical variables.

Table 1.

Frequency Distributions of Genotypes, Alleles, and Common Haplotypes and Diplotypes in EAs, AAs, and the Entire Sample

EAs
AAs
Total
EA vs. AA
n f n f n f p-Value
SNP1 (hcv2033010)
 T/T 41 .096 3 .029 44 .083
 T/C 185 .434 33 .314 218 .411
 C/C 200 .469 69 .657 269 .507 .001
 T 267 .313 39 .186 306 .288
 C 585 .687 171 .814 756 .712 2.4E-04
SNP2 (rs1042364)
 T/T 37 .089 2 .019 39 .074
 T/C 168 .403 20 .187 188 .359
 C/C 212 .508 85 .794 297 .567 2.0E-07
 T 242 .290 24 .112 266 .254
 C 592 .710 190 .888 782 .746 2.0E-08
SNP3 (rs1126671)
 A/A 44 .103 4 .038 48 .090
 A/G 189 .443 32 .305 221 .415
 G/G 194 .454 69 .657 263 .494 .001
 A 277 .324 40 .190 317 .298
 G 577 .676 170 .810 747 .702 1.0E-04
SNP4 (rs1126670)
 C/C 44 .104 3 .028 47 .089
 A/C 185 .436 34 .318 219 .412
 A/A 195 .460 70 .654 265 .499 4.1E-04
 C 273 .322 40 .187 313 .295
 A 575 .678 174 .813 749 .705 7.7E-05
SNP5 (rs7694646)
 A/A 37 .087 3 .029 40 .075
 A/T 175 .410 18 .171 193 .363
 T/T 215 .504 84 .800 299 .562 1.2E-07
 A 249 .292 24 .114 273 .257
 T 605 .708 186 .886 791 .743 3.2E-08
SNP6 (rs1800759)
 A/A 71 .168 73 .689 144 .273
 A/C 196 .464 27 .255 223 .422
 C/C 155 .367 6 .057 161 .305 < 2.2E-16
 A 338 .400 173 .816 511 .484
 C 506 .600 39 .184 545 .516 < 2.2E-16
SNP7 (rs1984362)
 A/A 37 .087 0 .000 37 .070
 A/G 172 .405 19 .181 191 .360
 G/G 216 .508 86 .819 302 .570 2.5E-09
 A 246 .289 19 .090 265 .250
 G 604 .711 191 .910 795 .750 1.5E-10
Haplotype
 CCGATCG 520 .603 40 .184 559 .519
 TTACAAA 239 .277 17 .078 256 .237
 CCGATAG 60 .070 133 .616 193 .179
 TCACTAG 28 .033 17 .079 45 .042
 CTACAAA 9 .011 1 .005 10 .010
 TTACAAG 2 .002 5 .023 7 .007 < 2.2E-16
Diplotype
 CCGATCG/CCGATCG 161 .374 6 .056 167 .310
 TTACAAA/CCGATCG 140 .324 2 .021 142 .263
 CCGATAG/CCGATCG 34 .078 21 .198 55 .102
 CCGATAG/CCGATAG 1 .003 42 .384 43 .079
 TTACAAA/TTACAAA 33 .078 0 .000 33 .062
 TTACAAA/CCGATAG 20 .047 13 .116 33 .061
 TCACTAG/CCGATCG 17 .040 2 .018 19 .035
 TCACTAG/CCGATAG 3 .007 13 .120 16 .029
 TTACAAA/TCACTAG 6 .014 0 .000 6 .011
 CTACAAA/CCGATCG 5 .012 0 .000 5 .009 7.9E-07

n, individual numbers (for genotypes, haplotypes, and diplotypes) or chromosome number (for alleles); f, frequency; EA, European American; AA, African American; EA vs. AA, exact tests for the comparisons of genotype, allele, haplotype, and diplotype frequency distributions between EAs and AAs; SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism; E, scientific format of 10n.

After we obtained positive findings from the above diplotypewise analysis, we performed allelewise and genotypewise MANCOVA using allele or genotype data, instead of diplotype data, to fine-map the trait loci for personality. Two-way interactions between the predictor variables and covariates and between any two ADH4 markers were also considered as independent predictor variables in these models.

Univariate Analysis of Covariance

A series of univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) tests was employed to assess the unique contribution of each personality factor to the association between the overall personality traits and covariates and genes. In these models, each personality factor served as one response variable in a univariate ANCOVA test. The predictor variables, covariates, and interaction variables were the same as those used in the aforementioned MANCOVA models.

This series of separate univariate ANCOVA tests was considered exploratory, because they ignored the correlations between personality factors and could reflect the same underlying phenomenon or inflate the probability of type I error. However, these univariate ANCOVA tests helped us to prioritize the theoretical importance of the five personality factors in their associations with ADH4, enabling us to perform the Roy-Bargmann stepdown analysis.

Roy-Bargmann Stepdown Analysis

Following positive findings from the MANCOVA, we sought to identify the genetic factors underlying the positive findings. This effort used the Roy-Bargmann stepdown analysis, a stepwise univariate ANCOVA (Tabachnick and Fidell 1996). First, we tested the highest priority personality factor using univariate ANCOVA. Then, we tested the next personality factor with ANCOVA, and used all the higher priority personality factors as covariates. This stepdown process decomposed the findings from MANCOVA without neglecting the correlations among the personality factors. We limited the Roy-Bargmann stepdown analysis to allele-wise and genotypewise models, so that we could fine-map the trait-influencing loci and decompose the sources of associations by the following methods. Two-way interactions between the predictor variables and covariates (including the higher priority personality factors) and between any two ADH4 markers were also considered as new independent predictor variables in these allelewise and genotypewise models. To be conservative in multiple testing and to avoid inflating the type I error rate in these analyses, we set the significance level (α) at .01.

If there are strong correlations between markers, between personality factors, or between markers and personality factors in these stepwise ANCOVA models, these independent variables will have a near-linear relationship that might lead to multicollinearity. During ANCOVA calculations, multicollinearity causes a division by zero (or a very small quantity), which, in turn, causes the calculations to be aborted (or to be distorted), creating inaccurate estimates of the regression coefficients, inflating the standard errors of the regression coefficients, and deflating the partial t tests for the regression coefficients, giving false, nonsignificant p-values and degrading the predictability of the model. Hence, during each step of ANCOVA, multicollinearity was detected first. First, pairwise correlation analysis between personality factors (see above) and pairwise linkage disequilibrium between markers (Luo et al 2006a) were tested, looking for near-linear relationships (although multicollinearity does not always show up when considering the variables two at a time). Second, the variance inflation factors, a measure of multicollinearity, were calculated (Haan 2002). Variance inflation factors over 10 indicate collinear variables. Third, eigenvalues of the correlation matrix of the independent variables and condition numbers were calculated. Eigenvalues of the correlation matrix near zero indicate multicollinearity. The condition number is the largest eigenvalue divided by each corresponding eigenvalue. Large condition numbers (>1000) indicate a severe multicollinearity problem. Fourth, if severe multicollinearity was detected, we planned to use the linear ridge regression method for ANCOVA; otherwise, we used general linear regression for ANCOVA.

T Test, One-Way Analysis of Variance, and Post Hoc Tests

After we obtained multiple risk predictor variables (including covariates, markers, and their interactions) using the Roy-Bargmann stepdown analysis, we decomposed them into a single variable and analyzed the specific risk covariates, risk alleles, and risk genotypes for every personality factor, using t test (for covariates and alleles) or one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) (for genotypes) followed by post hoc tests. To make the post hoc testing feasible, all the continuous covariates were converted into categorical variables. That is, ancestry proportions were converted into dichotomous population categories, i.e., “genetic” EAs (European ancestry proportion > .5) and “genetic” AAs (African ancestry proportion > .5), and age data were converted into dichotomous age categories, i.e., older subjects (> mean age of 32 years) and younger subjects (≤32 years), based on the cutoff point used by Koppes et al (2001) to differentiate younger and older individuals. Because of the strong correlation between personality factors, we would expect that an association between a gene and one specific individual personality factor might be observed independent of other personality factors only rarely; instead, an association with a personality factor might be dependent on other personality factors. That is, gene-personality association would be modified by another individual personality factor. However, due to the lack of published standard cutoff points available for the personality factors, there was no clear criterion for converting the continuous personality factor scores into categories. Thus, most personality factors were not decomposed using post hoc testing, except for one specific personality factor (i.e., conscientiousness) that was found to modify the gene-phenotype association (see Results below) and whose cutoff point was set at 22 on an exploratory basis.

Results

The scores for the different personality factors were: extraversion (range = 4–48, mean [± standard deviation] = 28.5 ± 6.9), agreeableness (range = 10–46, mean = 31.0 ± 6.6), conscientiousness (range = 0–48, mean = 31.8 ± 7.8), neuroticism (range = 2–48, mean = 20.4 ± 9.4), and openness to experience (range = 9–45, mean 28.4 ± 6.4). Neuroticism was negatively correlated with the other personality factors and all of the other personality factors were positively correlated with one another (p < .01). The genotype, allele, haplotype, and diplotype frequencies in the whole sample are shown in Table 1.

Multivariate analysis of covariance indicated that the personality factors were related to diagnosis, age, sex, ancestry, and ADH4 variation (Table 2). Diplotypewise MANCOVAs showed that 1) the personality factors were related to diagnosis, sex, age, and ancestry; 2) there was a significant association between the personality factors and the diplotype TTACAAA/TCACTAG (p = .030); this association was alternatively modified by ancestry (p =.028) or age (p =.019), that is, this association varied between different populations or age subgroups; and 3) there was a significant association between the personality factors and the diplotype TTACAAA/CCGATCG; this association was modified by diagnosis (p =.034) or by sex (p =.039), that is, this association is different for the different diagnosis subgroups or sex subgroups.

Table 2.

MANCOVAs Reflecting the Relationships Between Personality Traits and ADH4 Variation

Models Sources p-Values
Diplotypewise Diagnosis 9.0E-32
Sex 9.2E-05
Age 4.9E-08
Ancestry 1.2E-05
“TTACAAA/TCACTAG” .030
Ancestry × “TTACAAA/TCACTAG” .028
Age × “TTACAAA/TCACTAG” .019
Dx × “TTACAAA/CCGATCG” .034
Sex × “TTACAAA/CCGATCG” .039
Allelewise Diagnosis 2.4E-69
Age 2.2E-16
Ancestry 2.7E-11
Sex × SNP6 .030
(or) Sex × Age × SNP6 2.5E-13
Genotypewise Diagnosis 2.0E-35
Age 4.5E-08
Ancestry 5.0E-05
Sex × SNP6 5.4E-04
(or) Sex × Age × SNP6 2.2E-05

MANCOVA, multivariate analysis of covariance; E, scientific format of 10n. × denotes interaction.

Allelewise and genotypewise MANCOVAs showed that 1) the personality factors were related to diagnosis, age, and ancestry; 2) the marker SNP6 had a significant effect on the personality factors (modified by sex; p =.030, 5.4 × 10−4 for alleles and genotypes, respectively); and 3) inclusion in the model of both sex and age increased the observed effect of SNP6 on personality factors, such that p = 2.5 × 10−13 (for alleles) and 2.2 × 10−5 (for genotypes), respectively.

Univariate ANCOVAs identified the priority of the personality factors. A series of univariate ANCOVAs (including diplotypewise, allelewise, and genotypewise analyses [data not shown]) showed the priority order of the personality factors as follows: neuroticism (F4) > agreeableness (F2) > conscientiousness (F3) > extraversion (F1) > openness (F5). The priority was decided using the following criteria: 1) personality factors with higher priorities were associated with more diplotypes or markers and interaction variables than those with lower priorities; and 2) personality factors with higher priorities were more significantly affected by genetic factors than were those with lower priority. This priority order was also confirmed by testing other genes (unpublished data).

Roy-Bargmann stepdown analyses showed that different personality factors were related to diagnosis, age, and/or sex and that the ADH4 markers had effects on different personality factors (Table 3). First, although there were strong correlations between personality factors, all the correlation coefficients (|r|) were less than .6; all the variance inflation factors were less than 10; and all the condition numbers were less than 1000. Thus, the multicollinearity was not severe in any of the stepwise ANCOVA models, making it possible to examine a general regression model.

Table 3.

Roy-Bargmann Stepdown ANCOVAs for the Relationship Between Each Personality Factor and Predictor Variables

Sources F1 F2 F3 F4 F5
Allelewise
 Age 2.4E-09 5.9E-05 8.0E-04 .006
 Diagnosis 1.5E-04 1.2E-43 .004
 Sex 1.6E-06
 F2 .002
 F1 3.8E-05
 F4 1.9E-14 1.3E-34 6.8E-27
 Sex × SNP6 5.0E-04
 F3 × SNP1 .009
 F3 × SNP2 .003
Genotypewise
 Age 3.5E-05
 Diagnosis 1.5E-22
 Sex .006 .001
 F4 1.7E-06 1.8E-11 2.4E-15
 Sex × SNP6 1.9E-04

Only significant relationships are shown; statistical significance threshold = .01.

F1, Extraversion; F2, Agreeableness; F3, Conscientiousness; F4, Neuroticism; F5, Openness; X, interaction between; SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism; E, scientific format of 10n.

The allelewise and genotypewise Roy-Bargmann stepdown analyses based on the observed priority order showed that different personality factors were related to diagnosis, age, and/or sex and that ADH4 markers had effects on different personality factors. In summary, the following findings were obtained: 1) conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness to experience were significantly related to diagnosis (1.2 × 10−43p ≤.004); extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism, and openness to experience were significantly related to age (2.4 × 10−9p ≤.006); and agreeableness and neuroticism were significantly related to sex (1.6 × 10−6p ≤.006); 2) SNP1 alleles and SNP2 alleles had effects on extraversion (F1) (p = .009 and p =.003, respectively), which were modified by conscientiousness (F3); 3) SNP6 had significant effects on agreeableness (p = 5.0 × 10−4 for alleles; p = 1.9 × 10−4 for genotypes), which were modified by sex; and 4) no significant interaction effects were detected between different ADH4 SNPs on personality factors.

Generally, substance dependent patients, older individuals, and male subjects had significantly higher neuroticism and significantly lower scores on other personality factors. There were associations between extraversion and SNP1 and SNP2 in subjects with higher conscientiousness scores. Allele A or genotype A/A of SNP6 was significantly associated with agreeableness scores (around 31.0 ± 6.8), which were lower in female subjects, especially younger female subjects, and higher in male subjects, especially older male subjects (Table 4).

Table 4.

Decomposition Analysis of the Relationship Between Each Personality Factor and Each of the Predictors of Risk

Lower F1
Lower F2
Lower F2
Lower F3
Higher F4
Lower F5
Sample Covariates RF p RF p RF p RF p RF p RF p
Whole Diagnosis SD 7.3E-16 SD 5.4E-17 SD 3.6E-32 SD 3.6E-12
Age Older 8.5E-11 Older .035 Older .001 Older 1.5E-10
Sex Male .009 Female in SD 1.0E-04
Male subjects SNP6 Allele C .041 Allele C in older subjects .013
SNP6 Allele A .039 Allele A in younger subjects .028
Female subjects Genotype A/A .003 Genotype A/A in younger subjects .005

RF, risk factor; p, p-values; SD, substance dependence; E, scientific format of 10n; SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism.

Decomposing the risk variables obtained from the Roy-Bargmann stepdown analysis, we found the following: 1) patients had lower scores on agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience and higher scores on neuroticism than did control subjects; older individuals had lower scores on extraversion, agreeableness, and openness to experience and higher scores on neuroticism than did younger individuals; male subjects had lower scores (30.2 ± 6.3) on agreeableness than did female subjects (31.7 ± 6.8); and female patients had higher scores on neuroticism than did male patients (all p< .05); 2) in the subgroups with higher conscientiousness scores (>22), alleles of SNP1 or SNP2 were associated with extraversion (p <.05); 3) male subjects with allele A of SNP6 had higher scores on agreeableness (30.8 ± 5.9) than those with allele C (29.7 ± 6.8; p= .041); and female subjects with allele A or genotype A/A of SNP6 had lower scores on agreeableness (31.0 ± 6.8 for allele A, 29.5 ± 6.6 for genotype A/A) than those (32.2 ± 6.9 for allele C, 32.3 ± 6.9 for genotypes C/A+C/C) with allele C or other genotypes (p =.039 and p =.003, respectively). These higher agreeableness scores in male subjects (30.8 ± 5.9) are close to the lower scores in female subjects (31.0 ± 6.8), and both are associated with allele A. If decomposed by age, associations became more significant both in older male subjects (30.2 ± 5.9 versus 28.3 ± 6.4 for allele A vs. C, p =.013) and in younger female subjects (30.7 ± 6.5 versus 32.2 ± 6.9 for allele A vs. C, p =.028; 29.0 ± 6.0 versus 32.2 ± 6.8 for A/A versus C/A+C/C, p =.005, respectively). These higher agreeableness scores in older male subjects (30.2 ± 5.9) are closer to the lower scores in younger female subjects (30.7 ± 6.5); both are significantly associated with allele A.

Discussion

The present study provides strong evidence that personality traits are related to ADH4 variation. Several ADH4 markers contribute to variation in personality traits, among which the functional variant SNP6 (rs1800759) in the promoter region is the most important, as predicted. Using MANCOVA and the Roy-Bargmann stepdown ANCOVA analyses, we were able to correlate personality factors, the interactions among markers, and the interactions between markers and covariates, while controlling for potential biases. In these two analyses, subjects that differed on affection status, age, population, and sex were combined in a single model, thus maximizing the statistical power of the analysis. Use of MANCOVA made it possible to examine diplotypes, which incorporate the LD information between markers, in addition to the information from all single markers and unknown markers and thus are likely to be more representative of a “whole gene” than any single marker. Our use of quantitative, rather than categorical, phenotypic information is also an important advantage of this analytic approach.

Diplotypewise analysis showed that the personality factors were associated with the diplotypes TTACAAA/TCACTAG and TTACAAA/CCGATCG (which harbor the allele A or genotype A/A of SNP6; associations were modified by diagnosis, sex, age, or ancestry), suggesting that ADH4 harbors risk loci for the variation observed in the personality traits. Allelewise and genotypewise MANCOVAs, which in decomposing the findings from diplotypewise analysis also provided support for those analyses, demonstrated that the personality factors were related to diagnosis, age, and ancestry and that the marker SNP6 was the most important genetic variant affecting the personality factors.

The Roy-Bargmann stepdown ANCOVAs, which were used to decompose the overall personality traits to identify more specific sources of association, sometimes were more powerful than MANCOVAs. This is evidenced by the facts that at this step in the analysis: 1) in the genotypewise analyses, the associations between covariates and personality factors became more significant (Table 3 versus Table 2); and 2) markers (i.e., SNPs 1 and 2), in addition to SNP6, were found to be associated with personality traits. This analysis also demonstrated that a personality factor (i.e., extraversion) was affected by two ADH4 markers (i.e., SNPs1 and 2), although these associations were modified by another specific personality factor (i.e., conscientiousness), which may be because of the strong correlation (p = 1.2 × 10−15) between these two personality factors.

We also decomposed every risk covariate and marker revealed by the Roy-Bargmann stepdown analysis to identify the sources of association (i.e., specific risk covariates, risk alleles, and risk genotypes), using t test, one-way ANOVA, and post hoc tests (Table 4). However, because the interaction effects between markers and covariates and between markers were ignored in these analyses, the effects of markers became less significant or nonsignificant (Table 4 versus Table 3), which is consistent with the conclusion by Marchini et al (2005) that interaction analysis is more powerful than noninteraction analysis. In view of this point and other issues described above, our conclusions should be drawn primarily from the Roy-Bargmann stepdown analyses, the findings from which show very high levels of statistical significance (1.2 × 10−43p ≤ .009; Table 3), although some findings from decomposing analyses become considerably less strong or nonsignificant (Table 4).

Our analyses demonstrated that of the markers studied, SNP6 had the most significant effect on personality traits. This is a functional variant at the promoter that leads to an alteration of ADH4 promoter activity, significantly affecting the expression of π ADH; the A allele has promoter activity more than twice that of the C allele (Edenberg et al 1999). This variant has been reported to be associated with risk for alcohol dependence in European Brazilians and African Brazilians (Guindalini et al 2005). We (Luo et al 2006a) have reported that this variant was also strongly associated with SD and was the marker closest to the putative disease locus for SD; its minor A/A genotype increases risk for SD, that is, this is an SD-risk variant. In the present study, we observed that minor allele A increased the agreeableness score in male subjects (especially in older individuals), and allele A genotype A/A decreased the agreeableness score in female subjects (especially in younger individuals). This does not conflict in the sense that agreeableness scores in older male subjects and younger female subjects are very similar to one another; both groups of subjects have scores around 30.8 ± 5.9. The moderating effect of age on this marker between male subjects and female subjects indicates that the gene effect on personality traits is modified by sex and age, which is consistent with the extant literature on the topic. First, associations between personality traits and SD have been shown to be modified by sex. For example, the “positive emotionality” (outgoing, lively, persistent, and warm; measured by Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire [TPQ] and Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised [EPQ-R]) may be more strongly associated with alcohol dependence among women than among men (Slutske et al 2002); “negative emotionality” accounts for a portion of the genetic risk for alcohol dependence among men but not among women; and “behavioral undercontrol” accounts for significantly more of the genetic risk for alcohol dependence among women than among men (Slutske et al 2002). Second, these associations may be modified by age. For example, 1) the relationship between rigidity and alcohol consumption was found to be modified by age; it appears that a rigid adolescent refrains from drinking a second, third, or fourth drink, whereas a rigid young adult refrains from initiating drinking; in adulthood, rigid persons are less subject to influence to drink (Koppes et al 2001); and 2) in women only, especially adults, dominance was positively related to the level of wine consumption (Koppes et al 2001).

Single nucleotide polymorphism 2 (SNP2) is a nonsynonymous variant (Gly→Arg) in exon 9. Single nucleotide polymorphism 2 is in strong LD with SNP6 and was, like SNP6, strongly associated with SD (Luo et al 2005b, 2006a). Single nucleotide polymorphism 2 was also the marker closest to the risk locus for drug dependence (Luo et al 2005b, 2006a). In this study, SNP2 was associated with extraversion, although this association appeared only in subjects with higher conscientiousness scores. Single nucleotide polymorphism 1 had similar effects to those of SNP2. The modulating role of conscientiousness in the effects of SNPs 1 and 2 on extraversion indicates that there was a strong correlation between these two personality traits, and that this correlation makes it difficult to detect gene effects on extraversion independent of conscientiousness; thus, this correlation needs to be considered in studying these gene effects.

Both SD (Luo et al 2005b, 2006a) and personality traits (present article) were demonstrated to be strongly associated with ADH4. We, therefore, conclude that personality traits and SD have a partially shared genetic basis, consistent with the extant literature. Both phenotypes are influenced by the environment as well. It is probable that being substance dependent exerts some influence on personality structure during the course of the illness (Verheul et al 2004). If the influence of ADH4 variation on personality were mediated through an effect on SD (that is, if it were a secondary phenomenon), then findings in substance dependent subjects would be expected to differ from those in control subjects. We therefore divided the subjects into case and control groups and tested the relationship between ADH4 and personality within each group, and most of the results were similar (data not shown). That is, diagnosis does not generally affect the relationship between ADH4 and personality. Thus, some specific personality traits could be taken as phenotypes independent of SD, though mediated by ADH4 as SD. This is understandable because personality traits might have some underlying neurobiological mechanisms similar to those influencing risk for SD. The ADH4 enzyme (π ADH) catalyzes certain endogenous substrates (i.e., norepinephrine aldehydes, including 3,4-dihydroxymandelaldehyde [DHMAL] and 4-hydroxy-3-methoxymandelaldehyde [HMMAL]) to synthesize the intermediary glycols of norepinephrine metabolism, including 3,4-dihydroxyphenylglycol (DHPG) and 4-hy-droxy-3-methoxyphenylglycol (HMPG), respectively. The enzyme π ADH has 9- to 29-fold greater efficiency as a catalyst for this reaction than any class I isozyme (α, β, and γ ADHs); class III ADH (χ ADH) does not have any detectable catalytic activity for these substrates (Mardh et al 1986). Activated π ADH attributable to ADH4 gene variation, e.g., allele A of SNP6 (Edenberg et al 1999), would be predicted to increase levels of DHPG and HMPG, and this could result in a very high turnover of norepinephrine aldehydes. To block the turnover of norepinephrine aldehydes, an individual could self-administer alcohol to compete with DHPG and HMPG anabolism, because alcohol is an exogenous competitor for endogenous DHPG and HMPG on π ADH (Mardh et al 1986). Over time, this could lead to alcohol dependence. Alternatively, to mitigate the lower norepinephrine levels, an individual could self-administer cocaine, which partially functions as a norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor and can increase plasma norepinephrine concentrations (Sofuoglu et al 2001), or opiates, which stimulate norepinephrine release in cerebrospinal fluid (Bouaziz et al 1996). Over time, such self-administration of drugs (cocaine and opiates) could, similarly, lead to drug dependence. Norepinephrine is also well known to play a role in the development and expression of various personality traits (Coccaro et al 2003). A low level of norepinephrine is usually hypothesized to cause aggressive behavior (Coccaro et al 2003) that could be most closely predicted by (low) agreeableness (Gleason et al 2004). Agreeableness-related personality traits and aggression might be the outcome of a self-regulatory process predicated on a low level of norepinephrine (Gleason et al 2004). Thus, personality trait development might have a similar mechanism to SD risk, potentially mediated by norepinephrine levels, which are affected by ADH4 enzyme activity. Alternatively, other mechanisms via other pathways, e.g., a dopaminergic pathway, might be possible.

Footnotes

This work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health (NIH) Grants R01-DA12849, R01-DA12690, K24-DA15105, R01-AA11330, R01-AA016015, K24-AA13736, P50-AA03510, and M01-RR06192 (University of Connecticut General Clinical Research Center) by funds from the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs (the VA Medical Research Program and the VA Connecticut-Massachusetts Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center [MIRECC, and the VA Research Enhancement Award Program [REAP] research center); and Alcoholic Beverage Medical Research Foundation (ABMRF) Grant award R06932 (XL).

Dr. Frances Jurnak, University of California Irvine, provided a helpful suggestion regarding understanding ADH4 effects on norepinephrine metabolism. Ann Marie Lacobelle provided excellent technical assistance. The constructive and knowledgeable comments of the five anonymous reviewers are highly appreciated.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1987. revised. [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bau CH, Roman T, Almeida S, Hutz MH. Dopamine D4 receptor gene and personality dimensions in Brazilian male alcoholics. Psychiatr Genet. 1999;9:139–143. doi: 10.1097/00041444-199909000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blouin AG, Perez EL, Blouin JH. Computerized administration of the diagnostic interview schedule. Psychiatry Res. 1988;23:335–344. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(88)90024-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bouaziz H, Tong C, Yoon Y, Hood DD, Eisenach JC. Intravenous opioids stimulate norepinephrine and acetylcholine release in spinal cord dorsal horn. Systematic studies in sheep and an observation in a human. Anesthesiology. 1996;84:143–154. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199601000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Caspi A, Begg D, Dickson N, Harrington HL, Langley J, Moffitt TE, et al. Personality differences predict health-risk behaviors in adulthood: Evidence from a longitudinal study. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;73:1052–1063. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.73.5.1052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cloninger CR. Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. Science. 1987;236:410–416. doi: 10.1126/science.2882604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cloninger CR, Sigvardsson S, Bohman M. Childhood personality predicts alcohol abuse in young adults. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1988;12:494–505. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1988.tb00232.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Coccaro EF, Lee R, McCloskey M. Norepinephrine function in personality disorder: Plasma free MHPG correlates inversely with life history of aggression. CNS Spectr. 2003;8:731–736. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900019106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Stability and change in personality assessment: The revised NEO Personality Inventory in the year 2000. J Pers Assess. 1997;68:86–94. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6801_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Costa PT, McCrae RR, Dye DA. Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Pers Individ Dif. 1991;12:887–898. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cox WM, Lun K, Loper RG. Identifying pre-alcoholic personality characteristics. In: Cox WM, editor. Identifying and Measuring Alcoholic Personality Characteristics. San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass; 1983. pp. 5–19. [Google Scholar]
  13. Damberg M, Garpenstrand H, Alfredsson J, Ekblom J, Forslund K, Rylander G, et al. A polymorphic region in the human transcription factor AP-2beta gene is associated with specific personality traits. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5:220–224. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Digman JM. Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41:417–440. [Google Scholar]
  15. Ditlow CC, Holmquist B, Morelock MM, Vallee BL. Physical and enzymatic properties of a class II alcohol dehydrogenase isozyme of human liver: Pi-ADH. Biochemistry. 1984;23:6363–6368. doi: 10.1021/bi00321a012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Du L, Bakish D, Hrdina PD. Gender differences in association between serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and personality traits. Psychiatr Genet. 2000;10:159–164. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200010040-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ebstein RP, Segman R, Benjamin J, Osher Y, Nemanov L, Belmaker RH. 5-HT2C (HTR2C) serotonin receptor gene polymorphism associated with the human personality trait of reward dependence: Interaction with dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) and dopamine D3 receptor (D3DR) polymorphisms. Am J Med Genet. 1997;74:65–72. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8628(19970221)74:1<65::aid-ajmg15>3.0.co;2-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Edenberg HJ, Jerome RE, Li M. Polymorphism of the human alcohol dehydrogenase 4 (ADH4) promoter affects gene expression. Pharmaco-genetics. 1999;9:25–30. doi: 10.1097/00008571-199902000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Falush D, Stephens M, Pritchard JK. Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data: Linked loci and correlated allele frequencies. Genetics. 2003;164:1567–1587. doi: 10.1093/genetics/164.4.1567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Feder JN, Gnirke A, Thomas W, Tsuchihashi Z, Ruddy DA, Basava A, et al. A novel MHC class I-like gene is mutated in patients with hereditary haemochromatosis. Nat Genet. 1996;13:399–408. doi: 10.1038/ng0896-399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Finn PR, Sharkansky EJ, Brandt KM, Turcotte N. The effects of familial risk, personality, and expectancies on alcohol use and abuse. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109:122–133. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.109.1.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fullerton J, Cubin M, Tiwari H, Wang C, Bomhra A, Davidson S, et al. Linkage analysis of extremely discordant and concordant sibling pairs identifies quantitative-trait loci that influence variation in the human personality trait neuroticism. Am J Hum Genet. 2003;72:879–890. doi: 10.1086/374178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gelernter J, Kranzler H, Coccaro E, Siever L, New A. Serotonin transporter protein polymorphism and personality measures in African and European American subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:1332–1338. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.10.1332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gelernter J, Kranzler H, Coccaro E, Siever L, New A, Mulgrew C. D4 dopamine-receptor (DRD4) alleles and novelty seeking in substance-dependent, personality-disorder, and control subjects. Am J Hum Genet. 1997;61:1144–1152. doi: 10.1086/301595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gleason KA, Jensen-Campbell LA, Richardson DS. Agreeableness as a predictor of aggression in adolescence. Aggress Behav. 2004;30:43–61. [Google Scholar]
  26. Goldberg LR. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Am Psychol. 1993;48:26–34. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.48.1.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Golimbet VE, Alfimova MV, Manandyan KK, Mitushina NG, Abramova LI, Kaleda VG, et al. 5HTR2A gene polymorphism and personality traits in patients with major psychoses. Eur Psychiatry. 2002;17:24–28. doi: 10.1016/s0924-9338(02)00623-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Guindalini C, Scivoletto S, Ferreira RG, Breen G, Zilberman M, Peluso MA, et al. Association of genetic variants in alcohol dehydrogenase 4 with alcohol dependence in Brazilian patients. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1005–1007. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Haan CT. Statistical Methods in Hydrology. 2. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hasin D, Aharonovich E, Liu X, Mamman Z, Matseoane K, Carr L, et al. Alcohol and ADH2 in Israel: Ashkenazis, Sephardics, and recent Russian immigrants. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1432–1434. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hernandez-Avila CA, Covault J, Gelernter J, Kranzler HR. Association study of personality factors and the Asn40Asp polymorphism at the μ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) Psychiatr Genet. 2004;14:89–92. doi: 10.1097/01.ypg.0000107931.32051.c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Itoh K, Hashimoto K, Kumakiri C, Shimizu E, Iyo M. Association between brain-derived neurotrophic factor 196 G/A polymorphism and personality traits in healthy subjects. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004;124:61–63. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jiang R, Dong J, Wang D, Sun FZ. Fine-scale mapping using Hardy-Weinberg disequilibrium. Ann Hum Genet. 2001;65:207–219. doi: 10.1017/S0003480001008570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jonsson EG, Cichon S, Gustavsson JP, Grunhage F, Forslund K, Mattila-Evenden M, et al. Association between a promoter dopamine D2 receptor gene variant and the personality trait detachment. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:577–584. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01732-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kaufman J, Yang BZ, Douglas-Palumberi H, Houshyar S, Lipschitz D, Krystal JH, et al. Social supports and serotonin transporter gene moderate depression in maltreated children. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:17316–17321. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0404376101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Koppes LLJ, Twisk JWR, Snel J, Vente WD, Kemper HCG. Personality characteristics and alcohol consumption: Longitudinal analyses in men and women followed from ages 13 to 32. J Stud Alcohol. 2001;62:494–500. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2001.62.494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Krueger RF, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Epidemiological personology: The unifying role of personality in population-based research on problem behaviors. J Pers. 2000;68:967–998. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lang UE, Hellweg R, Gallinat J. BDNF serum concentrations in healthy volunteers are associated with depression-related personality traits. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:795–798. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lang UE, Hellweg R, Kalus P, Bajbouj M, Lenzen KP, Sander T, et al. Association of a functional BDNF polymorphism and anxiety-related personality traits. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;180:95–99. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2137-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lee HJ, Lee HS, Kim YK, Kim L, Lee MS, Jung IK, et al. D2 and D4 dopamine receptor gene polymorphisms and personality traits in a young Korean population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2003;121:44–49. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Loper RG, Kammeier MI, Hoffman H. MMPI characteristics of college freshman males who later became alcoholics. J Abnorm Psychol. 1973;82:159–162. doi: 10.1037/h0034959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Luo X, Kranzler HR, Zuo L, Lappalainen J, Yang BZ, Gelernter J. ADH4 gene variation is associated with alcohol and drug dependence in European Americans: Results from HWD tests and case-control association studies. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006a;31:1085–1095. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Luo X, Kranzler HR, Zuo L, Wang S, Blumberg HP, Gelernter J. CHRM2 gene predisposes to alcohol dependence, drug dependence, and affective disorders: Results from an extended case-control structured association study. Hum Mol Genet. 2005a;14:2421–2434. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddi244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Luo X, Kranzler HR, Zuo L, Wang S, Schork NJ, Gelernter J. Diplotype trend regression analysis of the ADH gene cluster and ALDH2 gene: Multiple significant associations for alcohol dependence. Am J Hum Genet. 2006b;78:973–987. doi: 10.1086/504113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Luo X, Kranzler HR, Zuo L, Yang BZ, Lappalainen J, Gelernter J. ADH4 gene variation is associated with alcohol and drug dependence in European Americans: Results from family-controlled and population-structured association studies. Pharmacogenet Genomics. 2005b;15:755–768. doi: 10.1097/01.fpc.0000180141.77036.dc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Marchini J, Donnelly P, Cardon LR. Genome-wide strategies for detecting multiple loci that influence complex diseases. Nat Genet. 2005;37:413–417. doi: 10.1038/ng1537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mardh G, Dingley AL, Auld DS, Vallee BL. Human class II (pi) alcohol dehydrogenase has a redox-specific function in norepinephrine metabolism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1986;83:8908–8912. doi: 10.1073/pnas.83.23.8908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McCrae RR, Costa PT. Personality trait structure as a human universal. Am Psychol. 1997;52:509–516. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.5.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Melke J, Landen M, Baghei F, Rosmond R, Holm G, Bjorntorp P, et al. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms are associated with anxiety-related personality traits in women. Am J Med Genet. 2001;105:458–463. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Muramatsu T, Wang ZC, Fang YR, Hu KB, Yan H, Yamada K, et al. Alcohol and aldehyde dehydrogenase genotypes and drinking behavior of Chinese living in Shanghai. Hum Genet. 1995;96:151–154. doi: 10.1007/BF00207371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Niu T, Qin ZD, Xu X, Liu JS. Bayesian haplotype inference for multiple linked single-nucleotide polymorphisms. Am J Hum Genet. 2002;70:157–169. doi: 10.1086/338446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Otter C, Martin C. Personality and addictive behaviours. In: Bonner A, Waterhouse J, editors. Addictive Behaviour: Molecules to Mankind Perspectives on the Nature of Addiction. New York: St Martins Press; 1996. pp. 87–117. [Google Scholar]
  54. Ponce G, Jimenez-Arriero MA, Rubio G, Hoenicka J, Ampuero I, Ramos JA, et al. The A1 allele of the DRD2 gene (TaqI A polymorphisms) is associated with antisocial personality in a sample of alcohol-dependent patients. Eur Psychiatry. 2003;18:356–360. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2003.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pritchard JK, Stephens M, Donnelly P. Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data. Genetics. 2000;155:945–959. doi: 10.1093/genetics/155.2.945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Retz W, Retz-Junginger P, Supprian T, Thome J, Rosler M. Association of serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism with violence: Relation with personality disorders, impulsivity, and childhood ADHD psychopathology. Behav Sci Law. 2004;22:415–425. doi: 10.1002/bsl.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rosmond R, Rankinen T, Chagnon M, Perusse L, Chagnon YC, Bouchard C, et al. Polymorphism in exon 6 of the dopamine D(2) receptor gene (DRD2) is associated with elevated blood pressure and personality disorders in men. J Hum Hypertens. 2001;15:553–558. doi: 10.1038/sj.jhh.1001231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rujescu D, Giegling I, Gietl A, Hartmann AM, Moller HJ. A functional single nucleotide polymorphism (V158M) in the COMT gene is associated with aggressive personality traits. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:34–39. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01831-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sander T, Gscheidel N, Wendel B, Samochowiec J, Smolka M, Rommel-spacher H, et al. Human muopioid receptor variation and alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998a;22:2108–2110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sander T, Harms H, Dufeu P, Kuhn S, Hoehe M, Lesch KP, et al. Serotonin transporter gene variants in alcohol-dependent subjects with dissocial personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1998b;43:908–912. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00356-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schuckit MA. Genetic and clinical implications of alcoholism and affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1986;143:140–147. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.2.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sen S, Nesse RM, Stoltenberg SF, Li S, Gleiberman L, Chakravarti A, et al. A BDNF coding variant is associated with the NEO personality inventory domain neuroticism, a risk factor for depression. Neuropsycho-pharmacology. 2003;28:397–401. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sher KJ, Trull TJ. Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: Alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:92–102. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.1.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sher KJ, Trull TJ, Bartholow BD, Vieth A. Personality and alcoholism: Issues, methods, and etiological processes. In: Leonard KE, Blane HT, editors. Psychological Theories of Drinking and Alcoholism. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 54–105. [Google Scholar]
  65. Sher KJ, Walitzer KS, Wood PK, Brent EE. Characteristics of children of alcoholics: Putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and psycho-pathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100:427–448. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.100.4.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Shi MM, Myrand SP, Bleavins MR, de la Iglesia FA. High throughput genotyping for the detection of a single nucleotide polymorphism in NAD(P)H quinone oxidoreductase (DT diaphorase) using TaqMan probes. Mol Pathol. 1999;52:295–299. doi: 10.1136/mp.52.5.295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Slutske WS, Heath AC, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Dunne MP, et al. Common genetic risk factors for conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107:363–374. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.3.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Slutske WS, Heath AC, Madden PA, Bucholz KK, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Personality and the genetic risk for alcohol dependence. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:124–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Sofuoglu M, Nelson D, Babb DA, Hatsukami DK. Intravenous cocaine increases plasma epinephrine and norepinephrine in humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2001;68:455–459. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00482-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB. The structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R (SCID), I: History, rationale, and description. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:624–629. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820080032005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Stein MB, Schork MJ, Gelernter J. A polymorphism of the β1-adrenergic receptor is associated with shyness and low extraversion. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:217–224. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Stephens M, Donnelly P. A comparison of Bayesian methods for haplotype reconstruction from population genotype data. Am J Hum Genet. 2003;73:1162–1169. doi: 10.1086/379378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Stephens M, Smith NJ, Donnelly P. A new statistical method for haplotype reconstruction from population data. Am J Hum Genet. 2001;68:978–989. doi: 10.1086/319501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 3. New York: Harper Collins College Publishers; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  75. Thomasson HR, Crabb DW, Edenberg HJ, Li TK, Hwu HG, Chen CC, et al. Low frequency of the ADH2*2 allele among Atayal natives of Taiwan with alcohol use disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1994;18:640–643. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1994.tb00923.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Thome J, Weijers HG, Wiesbeck GA, Sian J, Nara K, Boning J, et al. Dopamine D3 receptor gene polymorphism and alcohol dependence: Relation to personality rating. Psychiatr Genet. 1999;9:17–21. doi: 10.1097/00041444-199903000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Tsai SJ, Hong CJ, Yu YW, Chen TJ. Association study of catechol-O-methyltransferase gene and dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphisms and personality traits in healthy young Chinese females. Neuro-psychobiology. 2004;50:153–156. doi: 10.1159/000079107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Verheul R, van den Brink W, Ball S. Substance abuse and personality disorders. In: Kranzler HR, Tinsley JA, editors. Dual Diagnosis and Treatment: Substance Abuse and Comorbid Psychiatric Disorders. 2. New York: Marcel Dekker; 2004. pp. 261–316. [Google Scholar]
  79. Wand GS, McCaul M, Yang X, Reynolds J, Gotjen D, Lee S, et al. The muopioid receptor gene polymorphism (A118G) alters HPA axis activation induced by opioid receptor blockade. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:106–114. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00294-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Westberg L, Melke J, Landen M, Nilsson S, Baghaei F, Rosmond R, et al. Association between a dinucleotide repeat polymorphism of the estrogen receptor alpha gene and personality traits in women. Mol Psychiatry. 2003;8:118–122. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wiesbeck GA, Weijers HG, Wodarz N, Keller HK, Michel TM, Herrmann MJ, et al. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and personality traits in primary alcohol dependence. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2004;5:45–48. doi: 10.1080/15622970410029907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zhang H, Luo X, Kranzler HR, Lappalainen J, Yang BZ, Krupitsky E, et al. Association between two mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) haplotype blocks and drug or alcohol dependence. Hum Mol Genet. 2006;15:807–819. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddl024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Zucker RA, Lisansky Gomberg ES. Etiology of alcoholism reconsidered: The case for a biopsychosocial process. Am Psychol. 1986;41:783–793. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.41.7.783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES