Skip to main content
Journal of General Internal Medicine logoLink to Journal of General Internal Medicine
. 2007 Jan 9;22(5):565–571. doi: 10.1007/s11606-006-0094-z

Why do General Practitioners Decline Training to Improve Management of Medically Unexplained Symptoms?

Peter Salmon 1,, Sarah Peters 2, Rebecca Clifford 2, Wendy Iredale 3, Linda Gask 3, Anne Rogers 3, Christopher Dowrick 4, John Hughes 4, Richard Morriss 5
PMCID: PMC1855690  PMID: 17443362

Abstract

Background

General practitioners’ (GPs) communication with patients presenting medically unexplained symptoms (MUS) has the potential to somatize patients’ problems and intensify dependence on medical care. Several reports indicate that GPs have negative attitudes about patients with MUS. If these attitudes deter participation in training or other methods to improve communication, practitioners who most need help will not receive it.

Objective

To identify how GPs’ attitudes to patients with MUS might inhibit their participation with training to improve management.

Design

Qualitative study.

Participants

GPs (N = 33) who had declined or accepted training in reattribution techniques in the context of a research trial.

Approach

GPs were interviewed and their accounts analysed qualitatively.

Results

Although attitudes that devalued patients with MUS were common in practitioners who had declined training, these coexisted, in the same practitioners, with evidence of intuitive and elaborate psychological work with these patients. However, these practitioners devalued their psychological skills. GPs who had accepted training also described working psychologically with MUS but devalued neither patients with MUS nor their own psychological skills.

Conclusions

GPs’ attitudes that suggested disengagement from patients with MUS belied their pursuit of psychological objectives. We therefore suggest that, whereas negative attitudes to patients have previously been regarded as the main barrier to involvement in measures to improve patient management, GPs devaluing of their own psychological skills with these patients may be more important.

Key words: medically unexplained symptoms, general practitioners, management, psychological skills, attitudes

Introduction

Medically unexplained symptoms (MUS) burden the many patients who suffer them and the general practitioners (GPs) whom they consult.1,2 Patients have as poor a quality of life as those with comparable symptoms caused by disease.3,4 Many doctors find these consultations particularly challenging,58 and these patients consume symptomatic health care disproportionately although, because there is no disease, treatment is often ineffective.4,911 There has long been concern that unnecessary symptomatic treatment increases dependence on medical care.1215 In addition, GPs often communicate with these patients in ways that have the potential to “somatize” their problems and intensify their dependence. To this extent, somatization is therefore an iatrogenic as well psychological process.16

Improved management will require change in the way that GPs communicate with patients.17 However, although GPs recognize MUS as a responsibility of primary care2 and an area of training need,18 many studies have shown negative attitudes to these patients, as the widespread designation of patients as “difficult” and “heartsink” illustrate.57,19,20 If such attitudes deter the take-up of interventions such as training or supervision from mental health professionals, which could change their communication, then the practitioners who most need these interventions would not receive them. Information about how practitioners’ views and experience of MUS might deter them from participation would allow future interventions to be designed in ways which increase their adoption.

Reattribution training has been successfully delivered to GPs in several countries. It leads to more positive attitudes towards MUS patients, reduces health care costs, modifies patients’ beliefs, reduces emotional distress, and improves function.2123 However, most GPs to whom it was offered in a recent trial (MUST: the UK Medical Research Council Medically Unexplained Symptoms Trial), declined it. Extensive social psychology research confirms that attitudes, even stereotypical and negative ones, are contextually dependent.24 Nevertheless, previous studies of practitioners’ attitudes to patients with MUS have lacked a specific context for eliciting attitudes, and therefore risk generalised and idealised accounts that may not be relevant to specific opportunities for improved practice. Therefore, we interviewed samples of practitioners who had declined or accepted participation in MUST, to identify attitudes to MUS that might have led most to decline participation.

Methods

Sample and Recruitment

GPs were invited by letter or telephone to participate in a study in which they would receive training to help patients reattribute unexplained physical symptoms to lifestyle, psychosocial circumstances, or innocuous causes. For participating practitioners, training in three 2-hour sessions took place in the GPs’ practices and incorporated techniques that are known to be preferred by GPs, including small group teaching and video feedback.25 Practitioners were offered financial compensation to employ locum GPs for the periods of training. Of 1934 GPs invited to participate in MUST, 70 agreed, confirmed agreement when visited and were from practices in which all GPs agreed.

To obtain a sample of GPs who had accepted participation but had not received training, 11 GPs allocated to the control condition were telephoned and asked to arrange an interview before they received training. One had left the practice, but the remainder agreed and was interviewed. It is difficult to involve in research a group of practitioners who are defined by their refusal to participate. Therefore, we wrote to GPs who had declined or were unable to participate, emphasizing that the research team had much to learn about the reasons why they could not participate, and inviting them to take part in a short interview to tell the research team about these reasons. Of 76 who were asked, 16 agreed. Subsequently, telephone calls to a further 21 GPs led to interviews with a further seven. Sampling in both groups was purposive, including GPs: from each participating area (Liverpool, Greater Manchester, Lancashire and Wirral); from postgraduate training and nontraining practices; and, for those who declined to take part in MUST, including some who declined at each stage of recruitment (initial letter of invitation, subsequent telephone call, and visit from one of the research team). In accordance with accepted principles of qualitative methodology, recruitment ended when no new information was emerging.

The 10 participants who had accepted training included 7 females and 5 from practices involved in training medical students or trainee GPs. Practice size ranged from 3 to 8 GPs (median 4). They did not differ in these variables from the remainder of participating GPs. The 23 participants who had declined to take part in MUST included 5 female GPs and 10 from practices involved in medical training. Practice size ranged from 1 to 4 (median 3). Ten had not responded to the initial letter, seven had responded negatively, five had decided against participation after further information and one could not participate for procedural reasons. Gender was not available for GPs who participated neither in the trial nor the interviews, but interviewed GPs who did not participate in the trial did not differ from ones who were not interviewed on practice size, whether practices were involved in medical training or stage at which they declined to participate in MUST.

Interview

Interviews were in person in the GPs’ practice at a time of their choosing and remunerated. Although the reason for interviewing some GPs was that they had declined to participate, interviewers avoided an accusatory stance and emphasised acceptance of their decisions and belief that the research team had much to learn from practitioners who could and could not participate. The interview then included discussion of: (1) GPs’ views of the training that was offered; (2) their reasons for agreeing or not feeling able to participate; and (3) their own experience of patients with MUS. The 2 interviewers used an interview guide but aimed for a conversational style, avoiding closed questions and using open questions, prompts, and reflection. Interviews lasted from 20–55 minutes. The interview guide was subsequently modified to test or develop aspects of the analysis, which was conducted in parallel with interviews (see below). Interviews were audiorecorded, and anonymized transcripts were prepared before audiorecordings were destroyed. All text that was informative about GPs’ decisions and about their attitudes to patients with MUS and to training was retained for the present analysis.

Analysis

Analysis, which proceeded in parallel with the interviews, was inductive in that no categories were specified in advance. Our procedure followed established conventions for grounding analysis in data rather than in preconceptions.26,27 Transcripts were first read and discussed by the multidisciplinary team, which encompassed expertise in psychiatry, primary care, clinical psychology, and sociology (PS, SP, LG, AR, RC, WI, and RM). Three members (PS, RC, and WI: a clinical psychologist with extensive research and clinical experience in primary care and the 2 interviewers) then separately identified recurrent patterns, testing and modifying them by constant comparison, “cycling” between sets of data and the developing account of them and by discussion. Regular discussion by the whole team was informed by their reading of the accumulating transcripts and the continuing thematic analyses by these 3 members. Periodically, the analysis was referred to the broader group of those involved in the MUS trial, which included academic and practicing GPs. Analysis focused first on GPs who had declined to participate in MUST, before examining the data from participating GPs for evidence of commonalities and divergences from these. Rather than taking a purely descriptive or phenomenological approach to analysis of practitioners’ speech, we considered its social function—the work that it did in constructing the problem of MUS and patients’ and GPs’ roles.28 Analysis was finalized when no further changes emerged from this process and when all the relevant text could be accommodated in it. In our final analysis, each category was defined by commonality of meaning across data from several GPs.

The importance of cycling between data and emerging analysis, and triangulation between perspectives of different authors in different roles, are widely recognized as procedural sources of trustworthiness of qualitative analyses. In addition, the presentation of raw data is important in exposing the analysis to scrutiny.29 Procedures are, however, insufficient to guarantee useful findings.30,31 Additional standards by which we assessed the developing analysis were coherence and “theoretical validity” whereby conclusions should connect with theoretical ideas beyond the present study.27 We were also concerned with the “catalytic validity” of the findings;32 that is, they should have the potential to render the subject more tractable to researchers or clinicians.

In reporting the final analysis, we present transcript to illustrate the range and commonality of meaning of each category of the analysis. The ellipsis indicates omitted speech.

RESULTS

Accounts of GPs who had agreed or declined to participate in MUST overlapped in their objectives for patients with MUS, but diverged strikingly in their attitudes to patients and to their own psychological work. Findings from these samples are therefore presented separately.

GPs Who had Declined Participation in MUST

As expected, attitudes implying disengagement from patients were prominent in these GPs’ accounts, but the GPs also indicated several objectives that they had for engagement. Therefore, after first describing their negative attitudes and their objectives, we examined the tensions within their accounts.

Negative and Dismissing Attitudes to Patients

Most GPs (N = 15, 65%) described attitudes that implied disengagement from patients (Box 1 in the Appendix); patients’ complaints were not legitimate demands on medical care, reflecting the absence of “real” illness; it was impossible to help them, or it was pointless to try, because they refused what GPs thought was necessary or they were unwilling to change. Denigratory language was common and a few GPs were explicit in their dislike.

Box 1.

GPs Who Declined Participation: Negative Attitudes to Patients Presenting MUS

Patients’ problems are not GPs’ responsibility
[GP30] “I do not consider myself to be burdened at all... it remains their problem, I don’t take it on board.”
Patients’ demands are not legitimate
[GP32] “If they had to pay for an examination every time they came along I think it would greatly reduce the amount coming in with nothing or silly things... Majority of patients I see here I don’t really need to see them...It’s those that sit there for ages, it’s people who waste your time.”
Patients are beyond help
[GP24] “They are always going to be there and cause problems.”
[GP16] “They’re chronic problems that you can’t really do anything about.”
Patients refuse what the GP can offer
[GP31] “They just won’t accept that [absence of disease] and they get very, very angry too.”
[GP23] “But they’re not so worried about what actually causes it. If you try and go down that avenue you hit the block.”
Patients do not want to change
[GP24] “They don’t really want their problems and the way they think re-structured.”
Patients are unlikeable
GPs routinely described patients as having [GP14] “a whine and a moan”, [GP20] “pestering”, or being [GP22] “dependent”. Many were explicit that they disliked these patients:
[GP32] “Some young girls, every time they walk in the room, my eyes just water in horror of seeing them again and knowing that they are going to keep me talking for ages about nothing that they really should be seeing me about.”
Or were challenged by them
[GP31] “It literally makes your heart sink. There’s one that came the other day who’s had headaches, and we can’t think what it is and he’s been everywhere for investigations and your heart sinks.”

Psychological Engagement with Patients

Notwithstanding their negativity, a few GPs voiced sympathy for patients ([GP19] “I think it’s a shame for them because we’re only getting an iceberg of their anxiety aren’t we, it’s probably there in the background all the time and they only present to us when they reach a peak that they can’t actually handle themselves”) and most (N = 15, 65%) described clear objectives for engagement. Invariably, the priority was medical investigation. However, 14 (61%) also described explicitly psychological objectives (Box 2 in the Appendix). The overriding aim was to maintain the relationship as a vehicle for helping the patient. Several were optimistic about the value of being “straight”, in describing the absence of treatable disease. Although one observed that [GP21] “It’s very difficult to explain something when you don’t know the explanation yourself”, a few described attempts to reframe symptoms and some used aphorisms and self-disclosure. Seven GPs described explicitly seeking to identify and manage emotional problems. Whereas 1 had used antidepressants (unsuccessfully), and another had [GP23] “more of a counselling model”, most described informal approaches (Box 2 in the Appendix).

Box 2.

GPs who Declined Participation: Objectives and Strategies for Working with Patients with MUS

Investigate to exclude disease
[GP20] “The first thing you try to do is to investigate them fully to try and make sure that there isn’t a physical explanation for their problem”.
Although one GP described this as a protection for the practitioner, others cited the need to protect the patient: [GP12] “Just because they haven’t found something doesn’t mean, umm well you can never really accept it... It’s better to make sure and not be over-confident... I think it’s better to have the heart-sink patients coming in generally, to give them that attention than to eliminate them from your normal attention because of what they come in with before. It’s part of the job at the end of the day so you should do your best”.
Maintain a relationship
[GP21] “It’s rewarding when you can actually connect and communicate better with them.”
[GP24] “Once you’ve made the connection with them, then you can work with them.”
Be “straight” with the patient
[GP20]“It’s perhaps realizing that you’ve helped her in some way in that you appreciate that she’s got a problem but she appreciates that you couldn’t do anything for her.”
Reframe the symptoms
[GP30] “Rather than pushing them into a physical interpretation, their bowel or whatever, I’ve framed it that it’s their body’s way of protesting about a set of stresses that exist in their life and it’s their body twisting their arm to do something about this. When I’ve reframed it like that and they’ve accepted it and gone off to do their own thing about it.”
Identify and manage emotional problems
One GP described the view that [GP30] “most medically unexplained symptoms are down to depression”, but others sought to engage with specific psychosocial problems: [GP17] “I think it’s spending time and gaining their confidence and they can accept you... that you’ve excluded anything that could be more sinister, being prepared to listen to them. More often you do find there is some reason for their problem, I think. I mean, it depends on people’s stress levels and other issues what’s going on to how they deal with physical symptoms doesn’t it.”
Informal support
[GP 32] “I’m personal with patients, I tell them about things wrong with me. I’ll tell them... I’ve got three kids... I’ll tell them I went to the hozzie [hospital] last year... I’ll occasionally tell them I’ve done my back in.”

Negative and dismissive attitudes and psychological objectives and strategies were generally described by the same GPs (Box 3 in the Appendix). Of 15 (65%) GPs who voiced attitudes of “disengagement”, i.e., not taking responsibility for patients’ problems, or regarding them negatively, 11 (73%) also indicated engagement with patients by describing sympathy for them, or objectives or strategies for working with them psychologically.

Box 3.

GPs who Declined Participation: Coexistence of Negative Attitudes with Objectives and Strategies for Working with MUS

Despair coexisted with conscientiousness
[GP31] “It literally makes your heart sink. There’s one that came the other day who’s had headaches, and we can’t think what it is and he’s been everywhere for investigations and your heart sinks...They won’t accept there’s nothing the matter.” “Well they are very real, their symptoms, whether they are real or imagined, they are there and its just its their experience of their symptoms... I think its better to have the heart-sink patients coming in generally, to give them that attention than to eliminate them from your normal attention because of what they come in with before. Its part of the job at the end of the day so you should do your best.”
Negative attitudes, or a sense of futility, coexisted with efforts to identify emotional problems
[GP15] “Problems that you can’t explain?... Oh yes you do get a few, two or three or ten a week... They’re usually the patients you don’t want to see aren’t they?” “More often or not there’s something else going on I think, either they’re depressed, some of them perhaps you’re not understanding what they’re trying to tell you... I’d usually say well your bloods are ok and then perhaps explore one of the psychological issues. Is there anything else going on, what are they worried about.”
[GP20] “I think the danger is burning yourself out trying to find an answer... a lot of experience of general practice is actually coming to terms with that and accepting there are some people you can’t do anything for.” “[I take] the psychological approach to see whether there may be some underlying psychological problem or even psychiatric problem in some instances, but certainly are there any problems at home, finances, or any deep seated worries that they’ve got that may possibly be responsible for some of the symptoms they’re getting.”
Locating responsibility with the patient rather than GP coexisted with identifying and formulating problems with which the GP would help
[GP30] “I do not consider myself to be burdened at all ... it remains their problem” “Somewhere there will be depression and somewhere that’s altered the way they perceive their health and their judgement... I think usually if a patient comes in for reassurance then... you can say to them you’ve obviously come in about another problem you know lets sit down and talk about it... rather than pushing them into a physical interpretation, their bowel or whatever, I have framed it that its their body’s way of protesting about a set of stresses that exist in their life and it’s their body twisting their arm to do something about this... it’s about understanding the patients complaints you know and listening.”
Complaints that problems lack legitimacy coexisted with acceptance of their legitimacy and engagement
[GP32] “I tell you what bloody annoys me, its dizziness, I hate old girls that come in with ‘dizziness’, you know, its like the longest of cases, I don’t like that... it’s those that sit there for ages, its people who waste your time.” (see also Box 1) “I’m a firm believer if someone has a headache it’s a headache. I don’t care whether it’s a brain tumor or it’s because they’ve got some psychological problem, you know, it’s a headache... it might well be an imagined pain but it’s a real feeling. So I’m very sympathetic towards people... A lot of heart sink patients I think are helped by the fact that they know you, and I’m personal with patients, I tell them about things wrong with me, I’ll tell them my parents are dead, I’ve got three kids, I’m married.”

GPs’ Devaluing of their Psychological Skills

GPs used contrasting language to describe their psychological work and excluding disease. Whereas the latter was described as highly technical and important, psychological approaches were typically preceded by “just”, as in [GP20] “it’s just supporting those patients” or [GP24] “they just want to be acknowledged that they have an issue”. One GP explicitly denigrated the ways that he had learned to support a patient: [GP32] “Basically my advice to anyone is that you’ve got to appear to listen because whether you do or not it doesn’t matter, but you’ve got to appear to listen ... I’ve got one woman and she goes all round the houses and she just wants me to give her a phrase of the week, something like ‘all downhill’ or ‘what goes around comes around’. She likes sayings like that and she uses them then and relies on them to get sympathy I think... She just needs someone to come and talk to. She just wants a chat.”

GPs Who had Agreed to Participate in MUST

Positive and Empathic Attitudes to Patients

These GPs regarded patients as making legitimate demands that merited GPs’ attention. GPs saw them as [GP14] “part of the job... if it’s there, it’s got to be dealt with”, several described them as “interesting” and one felt [GP1]“quite fond of them really”. GPs’ empathy with these patients was demonstrated by the complexity of their accounts of MUS (Box 4 in the Appendix), in which they linked psychological problems to specific difficulties in patients’ current or earlier life and to cultural factors and medicalization.

Box 4.

GPs who Agreed to Participate: Types of Explanation Given for MUS

1. Learning
[GP1] “I think these are patients who’ve learnt to present their unhappiness in physical ways and they may have been in a general, where they went to the doctor with every little bit of pain so their mothers might have been frequent attenders and its been a sort of learned behaviour.”
2. General history and trauma
[GP6] “You do see people for years and you know them quite well but you possibly don’t know that 20/30 years ago something really awful happened to them and it never comes out.”
3. Psychosocial problems
[GP7] “Sometimes it’s a way of presenting unhappiness with everything in their life, marital problems or difficulty raising children.”
4. Medicalization
[GP 6] “You see these people getting referred to the hospital with back pain and the next thing you know some bright spark is going to operate on them and you think ‘what!’ and then they don’t get better, and the awful part is that sometimes before they have the surgery you know they’re not going to get better and you think why are we doing that but they’re pushing because they think it’s the magic answer but they don’t get any better and you kind of wonder whether you’re really doing them a service. Maybe we’re here in a way as a gateway to try and prevent harm as well as anything else. I have patients who come in and say I always feel much better when I’ve seen you and I’m thinking is that the right thing because I’m really becoming their crutch.”
[GP8] “Lots of patients in my view have got lots of psychological problems from which they manifest physical complaints and doctors frequently miss the point and give them diagnoses which they’re then stuck with for ever.”
5. Culture
[GP3] “I can think of certainly two or three Asian ladies where its been difficult, whether it’s a kind of different cultural difference with their view to illness in general...perhaps mental health and depression issues are more taboo in those cultures”

Psychological Engagement with Patients

GPs’ objectives for MUS patients included those described by the nonparticipating GPs, and extended to include asking patients to complete diaries or a life story, problem-solving, giving coping skills, helping patients to gain insight, scheduling regular reviews or inviting patients to book longer appointments. Two had explicitly addressed problems in the GP–patient relationship, including dependence.

GPs’ Valuing of their Psychological Skills

Like the nonparticipating GPs, they distinguished the biomedical and psychological aspects of their role, emphasizing the importance of excluding disease. However, unlike the nonparticipating GPs, none denigrated their psychological work. Instead, they described this aspect of their work in ways that indicated skill, such as [GP1] “active listening” or in observing that [GP10] “the skill is getting the balance right” between meeting patients’ support needs and encouraging dependency. Many comments indicated enthusiasm or interest. One described [GP14] “the pleasure of repeat visits... time on your side” and several referred to “giving” time to the patients. For one [GP1] “the most fascinating part of your work is the consultation”. Another described the importance of the relationship to the patient [GP10] “It would be easy as soon as you suspect something to say go and see the psychologist. That I don‘t think would help quite a lot of patients because they would feel they were rejected by the doctor.”

Discussion

Previous reports, based on interviews about generalized attitudes in unselected practitioners, suggested that they find patients with MUS difficult or have negative views of them.5,7,8,20,33 Our study indicates a more complex picture, characterized by variability between practitioners and by tension between different parts of their accounts.

Attitudes and Engagement

GPs described psychological objectives for working with patients with MUS, some of which have been reported previously.34 Many aimed for honesty, and even therapeutic effect, in being “straight” that disease was absent. Some described acknowledging the reality of patients’ symptoms and helping them reattribute symptoms. Several described probing about emotional and social problems. The overriding psychological aim was to maintain a relationship, and to use this as the vehicle for addressing psychological problems and providing support. Support was generally informal and involved listening to the patient, as well as idiosyncratic techniques including self-disclosure and aphorisms to help patients make sense of their problems.

Negative and dismissing attitudes to these patients did not indicate lack of engagement with them. Although GPs who had declined training described not taking responsibility for patients’ problems, considering patients unrewarding or to be making illegitimate demands, they also reported sympathy, acceptance of responsibility, and careful attempts at engagement. Dismissing attitudes cannot, therefore, be taken at face value as explaining why GPs did not accept training.

Additional negative attitudes also distinguished the GPs who declined training. Although they valued the biomedical aspect of their role—excluding disease—and described it technically and as an important service to patients, they typically described their psychological work as “just” support or “just” reassurance and sometimes explicitly denigrated it. These GPs thereby devalued their own skills and expertise in psychological work and in maintaining effective clinical relationships.35 By contrast, GPs who accepted training were all positive about their psychological work, describing it as skilful, interesting, and valuable to patients. We cannot say whether these GPs devalued psychological work in general or only with MUS patients.

Explanations for Negative Attitudes

Negative attitudes to patients in GPs who declined training may be a way to cope psychologically with the emotional challenges that patients present.36 As well as by describing protean symptoms that defy biomedical explanation,6,20,37 patients with MUS challenge practitioners by their psychological distress6,20,38 and demand for emotional support.39 The cognitive dissonance40 that arises when practitioners say that patients cannot or should not be helped, but then try to help the patient, may further compound GPs’ discomfort. Devaluing their own psychological skills might also be a coping response. An alternative explanation lies in these GPs’ dualistic thinking, which contrasted with the holistic and biographical approach that characterizes academic accounts of primary care.41,42 They identified their professional role with the biomedical opportunities of physical medicine rather than the psychological aims and methods of a psychosocial approach.

Barriers to Engagement with MUS

The importance to GPs of preserving clinical relationships, which has been blamed for promoting collusive communication with patients with MUS,43,44 might be expected to deter adoption of approaches that are perceived to formalize or threaten the relationship. Emphasis on clinical relationship did not, however, distinguish those who declined training from those who accepted it. Negative attitudes to patients suggest barriers to engagement, and previous writers have suggested that these should be explicitly targeted by programs to improve the care of MUS.5 However, these attitudes were belied by evidence that the GPs who displayed them did engage psychologically with their MUS patients.

More fundamental barriers should be considered. If GPs are negative about psychological skills because they are emotionally challenged by patients with emotional demands, such as those with MUS, then approaches that are perceived to require increased involvement with such patients would be unattractive. Ongoing supervision and support may be required before GPs such as these engage with such approaches. If GPs are negative about their psychological skills because they devalue psychological work by comparison with biomedicine, interventions to improve these skills would be unattractive compared to ones focused on enhancing technical or biomedical skills. Efforts to enhance the professional status of psychological skills would need to precede the offer of training or other interventions.

Strengths and Limitations of the Study

Whereas previous reports described practitioners’ attitudes to MUS,5,7,8,20,33,34 our analysis extended to an interpretative level that suggested psychological functions of attitudes that have hitherto been regarded as primary. Interview studies can be compromised by respondents’ tendency to speak in socially desirable ways. However, GPs in our study readily voiced negative and critical attitudes. Our findings arose in the context of the offer of a specific form of training as part of a research trial, and the offer of a different intervention, or the absence of a research context, might evoke different responses. Nevertheless, our design avoids generalized or idealized accounts. Moreover, the intervention implemented treatment and training principles that are widely advocated for these patients and for primary care, and the key barrier that we identified—devaluing of one’s own psychological skills—would impede any formal attempt to engage practitioners in psychological care for MUS. Practitioners who have declined to participate in training or research are, while potentially critical informants, inevitably hard to recruit for a study of the reasons for their nonparticipation. Only 24% of GPs in this group who were asked to take part agreed. Because of this, and the small sample size in both groups, the attitudes that we have described cannot be regarded as representative. Ultimately, the general applicability of our analysis can be tested in future work that seeks to engage GPs in improving management of MUS.

Conclusions

Our findings have implications for attempts to extend training or other interventions to improve management of MUS in primary care to GPs such as those who declined to participate in MUST. To focus on changing GPs’ negative attitudes to these patients, as previously advocated,5 is probably mistaken. First, we need to understand whether some GPs are particularly challenged by psychological demands that patients with MUS present, and whether this accounts for their devaluing psychological skills. This will require the future study of practitioner, as much as patient, characteristics,39,45,46 including their reactions to the emotional needs of patients with MUS and their tolerance or intolerance of uncertainty.45 Secondly, we need to understand the role of GPs’ dualistic separation of physical and psychological care in their deprecation of psychological skills, and we need to explore how these putative effects of dualism can be overcome.45,47 Only if GPs value their psychological role will they accept help to enhance it.

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by the UK MRC. We are grateful for the collaboration of Professor Francis Creed, Professor Graham Dunn, and Dr Barry Lewis in the design of the MUST trial, and to Ms Judith Hogg for the management of the trial and for the cooperation of the participating general practitioners.

Contributors RM led the MUST study, from which this report arises, LG, SP, CFD, PS, and AR contributing to the overall design and management. PS led analysis of these data, to which SP, LG, AR, RC, WI, and RM contributed, and drafting of this report, to which all authors contributed.

Potential Financial Conflicts of Interest None disclosed.

Appendix

References

  • 1.Burton C. Beyond somatisation: a review of the understanding and treatment of medically unexplained physical symptoms (MUPS). Br J Gen Pract. 2003;53(488):231–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 2.Reid S, Whooley D, Crayford T, Hotopf M. Medically unexplained symptoms-GPs’ attitudes towards their cause and management. Fam Pract. 2001;18(5):519–23. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 3.Smith GR, Jr, Monson RA, Ray DC. Patients with multiple unexplained symptoms. Their characteristics, functional health, and health care utilization. Arch Intern Med. 1986;146(1):69–72. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 4.Stanley IM, Peters S, Salmon P. A primary care perspective on prevailing assumptions about persistent medically unexplained physical symptoms. Int J Psychiatry Med. 2002;32(2):125–40. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 5.Garcia-Campayo J, Sanz-Carrillo C, Yoldi-Elcid A, Lopez-Aylon R, Monton C. Management of somatisers in primary care: are general doctors motivated? Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1998;32(4):528–33. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 6.Hahn SR, Thompson KS, Wills TA, Stern V, Budner NS. The difficult doctor–patient relationship: somatization, personality and psychopathology. J Clin Epidemiol. 1994;47(6):647–57. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 7.Hartz AJ, Noyes R, Bentler SE, Damiano PC, Willard JC, Momany ET. Unexplained symptoms in primary care: perspectives of doctors and patients. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2000;22(3):144–52. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 8.Wileman L, May C, Chew-Graham CA. Medically unexplained symptoms and the problem of power in the primary care consultation: a qualitative study. Fam Pract. 2002;19(2):178–82. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 9.Barsky AJ, Ettner SL, Horsky J, Bates DW. Resource utilization of patients with hypochondriacal health anxiety and somatization. Med Care. 2001;39(7):705–15. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 10.Barsky AJ, Orav EJ, Bates DW. Somatization increases medical utilization and costs independent of psychiatric and medical comorbidity. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(8):903–10. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 11.Kroenke K, Mangelsdorff AD. Common symptoms in ambulatory care: incidence, evaluation, therapy, and outcome. Am J Med. 1989;86(3):262–6. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 12.Barsky AJ, Borus JF. Functional somatic syndromes. Ann Intern Med. 1999;130(11):910–21. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 13.Kihlstrom JF, Cantor Kihlstrom L. Somatization as illness behavior. Adv Mind Body Med. 2001;17(4):240–3; discussion 270–6. [PubMed]
  • 14.Page LA, Wessely S. Medically unexplained symptoms: exacerbating factors in the doctor–patient encounter. J R Soc Med. 2003;96(5):223–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 15.Wessely S, Nimnuan C, Sharpe M. Functional somatic syndromes: one or many? Lancet. 1999;354(9182):936–9. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 16.Salmon P. The potentially somatizing effect of clinical consultation. CNS Spectr. 2006;11(3):190–200. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 17.Ring A, Dowrick CF, Humphris GM, Davies J, Salmon P. The somatising effect of clinical consultation: What patients and doctors say and do not say when patients present medically unexplained physical symptoms. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61(7):1505–15. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 18.Kerwick S, Jones R, Mann A, Goldberg D. Mental health care training priorities in general practice. Br J Gen Pract. 1997;47(417):225–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 19.Mathers NJ, Gask L. Surviving the ‘heartsink’ experience. Fam Pract. 1995;12(2):176–83. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 20.Steinmetz D, Tabenkin H. The ‘difficult patient’ as perceived by general physicians. Fam Pract. 2001;18(5):495–500. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 21.Larisch A, Schweickhardt A, Wirsching M, Fritzsche K. Psychosocial interventions for somatizing patients by the general practitioner—A randomized controlled trial. J Psychosom Res. 2004;57(6):507–14. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 22.Morriss R, Gask L, Ronalds C, et al. Cost-effectiveness of a new treatment for somatized mental disorder taught to GPs. Fam Pract. 1998;15(2):119–25. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 23.Morriss RK, Gask L. Treatment of patients with somatized mental disorder: effects of reattribution training on outcomes under the direct control of the general doctor. Psychosomatics. 2002;43(5):394–9. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 24.Blair IV. The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2002;6(3):242–61. [DOI]
  • 25.Morriss R, Gask L, Dowrick C, et al. Turning theory into practice: rationale, feasibility and external validity of an exploratory randomised controlled trial of training general practitioners in reattribution to manage patients with medically unexplained symptoms (MUST). Gen Hosp Psych. 2006; Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2006; 28(4):343–351. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 26.Henwood KL, Pidgeon NF. Qualitative research and psychological theorizing. Br J Psychol. 1992;83:97–111. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 27.Stiles WB. Quality-control in qualitative research. Clin Psychol Rev. 1993;13(6):593–618. [DOI]
  • 28.Gwyn R. Communicating Health and Illness. London: Sage; 2002.
  • 29.Elliott R, Fischer CT, Rennie DL. Evolving guidelines for publication of qualitative research studies in psychology and related fields (vol 38, pg 215, 1999). Br J Clin Psychol. 2000;39:10. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 30.Barbour RS. Checklists for improving rigour in qualitative research: a case of the tail wagging the dog? BMJ. 2001;322(7294):1115–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 31.Salmon P. How do we recognise good research? Psychologist. 2003;16(1):24–7.
  • 32.Guba EG, Lincoln YS. Fourth Generation Evaluation. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1989.
  • 33.Chew-Graham C, May C. Chronic low back pain in general practice: the challenge of the consultation. Fam Pract. 1999;16(1):46–9. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 34.Woivalin T, Krantz G, Mantyranta T, Ringsberg KC. Medically unexplained symptoms: perceptions of physicians in primary health care. Fam Pract. 2004;21(2):199–203. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 35.Cocksedge S, May C. Pastoral and holding work in primary care: affect, subjectivity and chronicity. Chronic Illness. 2005;1:157–63. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 36.Byrne A, Morton J, Salmon P. Defending against patients’ pain: a qualitative analysis of nurses’ responses to children’s postoperative pain. J Psychosom Res. 2001;50(2):69–76. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 37.Ringsberg KC, Krantz G. Coping with patients with medically unexplained symptoms — Work-related strategies of physicians in primary health care. J Health Psychol. 2006;11(1):107–16. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 38.Sharpe M, Mayou R, Seagroatt V, et al. Why do doctors find some patients difficult to help? Q J Med. 1994;87(3):187–93. [PubMed]
  • 39.Salmon P, Ring A, Dowrick CF, Humphris GM. What do general practice patients want when they present medically unexplained symptoms, and why do their doctors feel pressurized? J Psychosom Res. 2005;59(4):255–60. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 40.Festinger L. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press; 1957.
  • 41.Mathers N, Rowland S. General practice—a post-modern specialty? Br J Gen Pract. 1997;47(416):177–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 42.Dowrick C, May C, Richardson M, Bundred P. The biopsychosocial model of general practice: rhetoric or reality? Br J Gen Pract. 1996;46(403):105–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 43.Chew-Graham C. Why do doctors feel pressurized? J Psychosom Res. 2005;59(4):261–2. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 44.Chew-Graham CA, May CR, Roland MO. The harmful consequences of elevating the doctor–patient relationship to be a primary goal of the general practice consultation. Fam Pract. 2004;21(3):229–31. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 45.Epstein RM, Quill TE, McWhinney IR. Somatization reconsidered: incorporating the patient’s experience of illness. Arch Intern Med. 1999;159(3):215–22. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 46.Mathers N, Jones N, Hannay D. Heartsink patients: a study of their general practitioners. Br J Gen Pract. 1995;45(395):293–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 47.McDaniel SH, Campbell TL, Seaburn DB. Ch 16: Integrating the mind–body split: a biopsychosocial approach to somatic fixation. General-Oriented Primary Care: A Manual for Medical Providers. NY: Springer-Verlag; 1990:248–62.

Articles from Journal of General Internal Medicine are provided here courtesy of Society of General Internal Medicine

RESOURCES