Skip to main content
The British Journal of Ophthalmology logoLink to The British Journal of Ophthalmology
. 2006 Jul;90(7):836–838. doi: 10.1136/bjo.2006.090910

Unilateral visual impairment and neurodevelopmental performance in preschool children

S Hrisos 1,2,3,4, M P Clarke 1,2,3,4, T Kelly 1,2,3,4, J Henderson 1,2,3,4, C M Wright 1,2,3,4
PMCID: PMC1857168  PMID: 16540485

Abstract

Background

Unilateral visual impairment (UVI) as a result of amblyopia or refractive error is common in childhood, but its functional significance remains largely unexplored.

Aim

To investigate the influence of visual acuity and stereoacuity on the performance of preschool children on tasks requiring visuomotor skills and visuospatial ability.

Methods

Children with normal (6/6) visual acuity (VA) in both eyes and children with UVI ranging from 6/9 to 6/60, with no strabismus and normal vision in the fellow eye, were assessed on a neurodevelopmental test battery of visually guided tasks.

Results

50 children (mean age (SD): 52.4 (5.7) months; median (range) VA: 6/9 (6/6 to 6/60); median (range) stereoacuity: 70 seconds arc (40–absent)) completed the test battery. UVI and stereoacuity correlated moderately (Pearson's r = 0.537, p<0.001) but seven of 28 children with impaired VA had normal stereoacuity (<70 seconds arc) while five of 22 with normal VA had abnormal stereoacuity. Stereoacuity correlated with performance on a task requiring fine hand‐eye coordination and a task measuring visuomotor integration. UVI did not correlate with performance on any test battery items.

Conclusions

UVI itself does not appear to relate to visuomotor actions, except when associated with reduced stereoacuity. Stereoacuity appears to have an influential role in fine visuomotor actions and spatial representation in preschool children.

Keywords: visual acuity; stereoacuity; child, preschool; psychomotor performance


Preschool vision screening commonly detects unilateral visual impairment (UVI), a monocular loss or reduction in visual acuity. UVI may be due to amblyopia (“lazy eye”) or anisometropia (an inequality of refractive error between the two eyes). Treatment for UVI consists of spectacle correction for refractive error and/or occlusion of the normally sighted eye when amblyopia is present.1 Ideally, treatment should be undertaken during a critical period of development that lasts up to approximately 7 years of age.2

To ensure early intervention, many countries routinely screen for UVI in early childhood. Yet despite being the most common paediatric ophthalmological disorder, the functional significance of living with UVI is not fully understood.3 Reduced stereopsis may be a feature of UVI. Stereopsis is the binocular perception of depth and allows fast and easy access to information contributing to our spatial awareness.3

Imperfect stereoacuity may disqualify a person from professions requiring a high level of visual skills—for example, piloting an aircraft or joining the police force, and has been linked to poorer academic performance in primary schoolchildren and the neurodevelopmental performance of infants with strabismus (cited by Richardson et al,4 Kulp and Schmidt,5 and Rogers et al6).

No previous studies have investigated the influence of UVI on the neurodevelopmental functioning of preschool children. This paper reports the performance of a group of children with UVI detected at preschool screen, on a battery of tasks requiring fine and gross visuomotor skills and visuospatial ability.

Methods and materials

Approval for this study was gained from Newcastle upon Tyne and Sunderland local research ethics committees. Informed, parental consent was gained for all participating children.

Neurodevelopmental test battery development

A test battery was developed to include items targeting performance in visuomotor integration, fine and gross visuomotor skill, and visuospatial processing. Most items were taken from existing, validated test batteries, one item (throw beanbag into basket) was devised for the study. The test battery was found to have adequate test‐retest and interobserver reliability, with all items showing good stability over time in this age group (table 1).

Table 1 Neurodevelopmental test battery items with scoring criteria and source.

Task Description Scoring criteria Ability assessed Source
Visual search Find rabbits hidden in array Total rabbits located in one minute Visual attention NEPSY7
Block building Copy construction of various designs Graded points: 0–12 Visuospatial ability NEPSY, McCarthy8
VMI (short format) Design copying As test manual Visuomotor integration Beery‐Buktenika9
Visual tracking Track route from start to finish with pencil line Summary score based on time taken to track route and number of errors Visuomotor control and accuracy of trace NEPSY, McCarthy
Bead threading Thread 10 beads onto shoelace Time taken to thread 10 beads Fine hand‐eye coordination NEPSY, McCarthy
Ball/beanbag catch Catch a ball/beanbag thrown towards child's midline from a distance of 6 feet 3 trials each variation. Number of catches with 2 hands (ball and beanbag). Number of catches with 1 hand (beanbag only) Gross visuomotor coordination Bruininks‐Oseresky test of motor proficiency10 (ball assessor's own item)
Beanbag target Throw beanbag into basket from a distance of 6 feet 3 trials. Number of correct hits Gross visuomotor coordination Assessor's own item
Balance Walk forward along a taped line with hands on hips without stepping off line. Step over response speed stick held at child's midline Pass/fail (minimum 6 consecutive steps to pass walking task) (step over stick without going off the line or touching stick) Performance balance Bruininks‐Oseresky test of motor proficiency

Items were presented in the order they appear in this table. Intraclass correlation coefficients (95% CI) ranged from 0.70 (0.37 to 0.88) to 0.84 (0.62 to 0.93) across the test items (based on a reliability study using a convenience sample of 19 preschool children, 10 female, 9 male, at mean age of 48 months).

Study participants

Two groups of children, aged 3 years to 4 years 9 months, comparable in sex and socioeconomic status:

  1. Children with UVI taking part in a three arm, multicentre randomised controlled trial of treatment for UVI, with uncorrected UVI of 6/9 to 6/60, no strabismus and at least normal (6/6) vision in the fellow eye.1

  2. Comparison children recruited from nurseries, with normal vision in both eyes at preschool screening.

The visual status of all children was confirmed on the day of neurodevelopmental assessment. Some children with UVI had already started glasses treatment.

Procedure

To investigate the potential consequence of untreated UVI, all vision tests and neurodevelopmental assessments were performed without refractive correction.

Visual sensory assessment

All vision tests were performed by an orthoptist on the day of, and before, the neurodevelopmental assessment. Visual acuity for distance was assessed using a Snellen based, vision test.11 Stereoacuity was assessed using the “circles” item of the Randot stereogram test, which grades stereoacuity from 400–40 seconds of arc. At least 75% of visually normal children aged 3–5 years can achieve 70 seconds arc on the circles part of this test.12 Children unable to correctly identify the largest disparity (400 seconds of arc) were classified as “negative” responders and allocated a notional score of 600 seconds of arc to enable inclusion in the analysis.

Neurodevelopmental assessment

All neurodevelopmental assessments were conducted by the first author (SR) who was masked to the visual sensory measurements made on the same day. Assessments lasted approximately 30 minutes. The British Picture Vocabulary Scale (BPVS‐II) was included to screen for developmental delay, with a cut‐off score of 70 or below for exclusion.13

Data analysis

The dependent variables were visual acuity (of the impaired eye for children with UVI and 6/6 for children with normal vision in both eyes) and stereoacuity. Logarithmic transformation of these visual sensory scores was performed to allow parametric analysis and use of the geometric mean.14 As the test battery items were scored on different scales, standardised z scores were generated and a composite score calculated (Z score = (raw score – group mean)/group SD). The effect of UVI on test battery performance was analysed using Pearson correlation and linear regression.

Results

Thirty children with UVI (of 59 eligible trial participants) were recruited. Ten children had been in glasses treatment for up to 6 weeks and five for more than 12 weeks. On the day of their neurodevelopmental assessment the uncorrected median visual acuity in the impaired eye of the recruited children was 6/12 (range 6/6–6/60). Visual acuity in all fellow eyes was 6/6. Four children (13%) had normal, uncorrected vision in both eyes; three had not been in treatment. The 29 non‐participating children (49%) had a median referral visual acuity of 6/18 (range 6/9–6/60) in their impaired eye. Children with poorer monocular acuities (6/18–6/60) were significantly under‐represented in this UVI sample (χ2 = 4.800, df = 1, p = 0.028).

Twenty children with normal vision in both eyes at preschool screen (non‐UVI) were also recruited to the study. On the day of their neurodevelopmental assessment 2/20 non‐UVI children had reduced vision in one eye (6/9 and 6/12). Substantial variation in stereoacuity values was also apparent—five non‐UVI children had stereoacuity worse than 70 seconds of arc and five with UVI had stereoacuity better than 70 seconds of arc (table 2).

Table 2 Uncorrected Snellen visual acuity and randot stereoacuity as measured on the day of the neurodevelopmental assessment.

Visual acuity category Stereoacuity category (seconds of arc)
<70 70–100 101–400 600 (notional score) Total
6/6 6/6 17 3 1 1 22
6/6 6/9–6/12 7 6 3 2 18
6/6 6/18–6/24 4 1 5
6/6 6/36–6/60 2 1 2 5
Total 24 15 6 5 50

The data from these 50 children, at a mean age (SD) of 52.4 (5.7) months, 52% male, provided a spectrum of visual acuities from 6/6 in both eyes to a monocular impairment of 6/60, median 6/9, and stereoacuities from 40 seconds of arc to absent, median 70 seconds of arc. Five children (one non‐UVI) had no measurable stereoacuity and were given a nominal score of 600 seconds of arc. No child had developmental delay.

Impaired visual acuity correlated moderately with reduced stereoacuity (Pearson's r = 0.525, p<0.001, two tailed) but did not correlate with the composite battery score or any of the individual test battery items. Reduced stereoacuity showed moderate correlations of statistical significance with poorer performance on the composite battery score: (r = −0.417, p = 0.002) and on three individual test battery items (bead threading: r = −0.445, p = 0.001; VMI: r = −0.341, p = 0.0; block building: r = −0.305, p = 0.03). In the regression model, visual acuity and stereoacuity were entered as the main predictor variables, with age and sex entered to control for any confounding effect of these variables. Impaired visual acuity did not predict performance on the composite battery score or any of the test battery items.

Stereoacuity and age significantly predicted the composite score (stereoacuity: β = −0.298, p = 0.019; age: β = −0.433, p = 0.001; adjusted R2  = 0.320; F2,47 = 12.516, p<0.001). Stereoacuity and sex (positive female bias) predicted performance on the bead threading item (stereoacuity: β = −0.353, p = 0.004; sex: β = −0.429, p = 0.001; adjusted R2  = 0.348; F2,47 = 14.050, p<0.001). Stereoacuity predicted visuomotor integration (stereoacuity: β = −0.341, p = 0.015; adjusted R2  = 0.098; F1,48 = 6.307, p = 0.015).

Discussion

This study demonstrated a relation of comparable strength between impaired visual acuity and stereoacuity to that previously reported in preschool children with UVI.4 We also found that stereoacuity, and not impaired visual acuity, significantly predicted performance on a task requiring fine hand‐eye coordination and on a task measuring visuomotor integration. Though this is the first study to present these findings for preschool children, a similar link between stereoacuity and fine visuomotor control has been reported in adult subjects, though the reported relation was non‐linear.15 The present findings suggest evidence of a linear relation.

Our findings support a maturational effect for the development of stereoacuity in this age group as reduced stereoacuity, though more frequently observed in children with UVI, was neither exclusive to nor predicted by having this condition.4 These findings are limited to some extent in that some children who had begun treatment were then assessed without the benefit of their glasses. For the few children who had worn their glasses for longer than 12 weeks this may have produced a treatment effect for their VA and stereoacuity.16 However, it is also possible that children with UVI who had achieved normal visual acuity by the time of their neurodevelopmental assessment, may not yet have normalised their stereo function, if indeed they ever would do so.4

Children rely heavily on visual guidance for the spatial execution of tasks involving hand and arm movements during the early years of education.17 Binocular cues are considered to be pre‐eminent in the control of visually guided reaching and grasping behaviour.18 Low VMI scores have been found to significantly predict handwriting skill in first grade children and to be important correlates of reading achievement.19,20 A recent study found lower levels of measurable stereoacuity to be significantly related to poorer outcome on teacher ratings of reading performance, writing ability and mathematics in children aged between 5 years and 7 years.5 Though children with UVI do not regularly present with obvious learning difficulties, Packwood et al found that 52% of respondents in their survey felt their visual condition had interfered with their schoolwork.21 Other authors have failed to find a relation between visual function and academic achievement, but such studies do not directly investigate stereoacuity.22,23

The present study represents the first attempt to investigate the relation between UVI detected at preschool vision screen and neurodevelopmental function in a large group of preschool children. Monocular reduced visual acuity itself was not found to relate to any of the tasks presented to our young participants, but our results suggest that stereoacuity may have an influential role for the execution of fine visuomotor actions and the development of cognitive processes involving spatial representation in preschool children of normal intelligence. As children with worse degrees of UVI were under‐represented in the study group, it is conceivable that the impact of a deficiency in stereoacuity may be stronger than that reported here.

Acknowledgements

We thank all the children, their parents and preschool groups for their generous participation in this study and the orthoptists in the Children's Eye Clinic at the Royal Victoria Infirmary and the Sunderland Eye Infirmary who volunteered their free time to perform the vision assessments for this study.

Abbreviations

BPVS - British Picture Vocabulary Scale

UVI - unilateral visual impairment

VA - visual acuity

VMI - visuomotor integration

Footnotes

Grant support: UK National Health Service Research and Development, Northern and Yorkshire region.

Competing interests: None.

References

  • 1.Clarke M P, Wright C M, Hrisos S.et al Randomised controlled trial of treatment of unilateral visual impairment detected at preschool vision screening. BMJ 20033271251–1254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.PEDIG A randomised controlled trial of treatment of amblyopia in children aged 7 to 17. Arch Ophthalmol 2005123437–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fielder A, Moseley M. Does stereopsis matter in humans? Eye 199610233–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Richardson S R, Wright C M, Hrisos S.et al Stereoacuity in unilateral visual impairment detected at preschool screening: Outcomes from a randomised controlled trial. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 200546150–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kulp K T, Schmidt P P. Depth perception and near stereoacuity: is it related to academic performance in young children. Binoc Vis Strabismus Q 200217129–134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rogers G, Chazan S, Fellows R.et al Strabismus surgery and its effects upon infant development in congenital esotropia. Ophthalmology 198289479–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S.NEPSY: developmental assessment of neuropsychological function. San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation, 1994
  • 8.McCarthy D.McCarthy scales of children's abilities. New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1972
  • 9.Beery K.The Beery‐Buktenika developmental test of visuo‐motor integration (VMI): administration, scoring and teaching manual. 4th revised ed. Cleveland, OH: Modern Curriculum Press, 1997
  • 10.Bruininks R H.Bruininks‐Oseretsky test of motor proficiency. Minnesota: American Guidance Service Inc, 1978
  • 11.Sheridan M D. Diagnosis of visual defect in early childhood. Br Orthoptic J 19632029–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Simons K A. A comparison of the Frisby, Random‐dot E, TNO and Randot Circles stereotests in screening and office use. Arch Ophthalmol 198199446–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dunn L, Whetton C, Burley J.The British picture vocabulary scales. 2nd ed: NFER‐Nelson Publishing Company, 1997
  • 14.Swinscow T D V, Campbell M J.Statistics at square one. 10th ed. London: BMJ Publishing Group, 2002
  • 15.Murdoch J R, McGhee C N J, Glover V. The relationship between stereopsis and fine manual dexterity: pilot study of a new instrument. Eye 19915642–643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Moseley M J, Neufeld M, McCarry B.et al Remediation of refractive amblyopia by optical correction alone. Ophthal Physiol Optics 200222296–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hay L. Spatial‐temporal analysis of movements in children: motor programs versus feedback in the development of reaching. J Motor Behav 197911189–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Watt S J, Bradshaw M F. Binocular cues are important in controlling the grasp but not the reach in natural prehension movements. Neuropsychologia 2000381473–1481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cornhill H, Case‐Smith J. Factors that relate to good and poor handwriting. Am J Occup Ther 199650732–739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kavale K. Meta analysis of the relationship between visual perceptual skills and reading achievement. J Learning Disabil 19821542–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Packwood E A, Cruz O A, Rychwalski P J.et al The psychosocial effects of amblyopia study. J AAPOS 1999315–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Helveston E M, Weber J C, Miller K.et al Visual function and academic performance. Am J Ophthalmol 198599346–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bishop D V M, Jancey C, McSteel A. Orthoptic status and reading disability. Cortex 197915659–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The British Journal of Ophthalmology are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES