Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Anim Behav. 2007 Mar;73(3):535–539. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.005

On the evolution of brain size in relation to migratory behaviour in birds

VLADIMIR V PRAVOSUDOV *, KIRSTEN SANFORD , THOMAS P HAHN
PMCID: PMC1865118  NIHMSID: NIHMS20235  PMID: 18311316

Abstract

Migratory birds appear to have relatively smaller brain size compared to sedentary species. It has been hypothesized that initial differences in brain size underlying behavioural flexibility drove the evolution of migratory behaviour; birds with relatively large brains evolved sedentary habits and those with relatively small brains evolved migratory behaviour (migratory precursor hypothesis). Alternative hypotheses suggest that changes in brain size might follow different behavioural strategies and that sedentary species might have evolved larger brains because of differences in selection pressures on brain size in migratory and nonmigratory species. Here we present the first evidence arguing against the migratory precursor hypothesis. We compared relative brain volume of three subspecies of the white-crowned sparrow: sedentary Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli and migratory Z. l. gambelii and Z. l. oriantha. Within the five subspecies of the white-crowned sparrow, only Z. l. nuttalli is strictly sedentary. The sedentary behaviour of Z. l. nuttalli is probably a derived trait, because Z. l. nuttalli appears to be the most recent subspecies and because all species ancestral to Zonotrichia as well as all older subspecies of Z. leucophrys are migratory. Compared to migratory Z. l. gambelii and Z. l. oriantha, we found that sedentary Z. l. nuttalli had a significantly larger relative brain volume, suggesting that the larger brain of Z. l. nuttalli evolved after a switch to sedentary behaviour. Thus, in this group, brain size does not appear to be a precursor to the evolution of migratory or sedentary behaviour but rather an evolutionary consequence of a change in migratory strategy.

Keywords: behavioural flexibility, brain size, cognition, migratory strategies, neuoethology, passerine, selection, white-crowned sparrow, Zonotrichia leucophyrs


Birds are well known for their migratory behaviour, and the evolution of avian migration has attracted much attention (e.g. Zink 2002; Rappole 2003). Whereas some gradation exists between purely migratory and purely sedentary strategies, many species do follow only one of these strategies by either showing seasonal migration every year or by staying on the same territories year round. Yet, it remains unclear why given similar ecological conditions in seasonal environments, some species regularly migrate from their breeding grounds while others remain sedentary throughout the year (Sol et al. 2005a). Sol et al. (2005a) hypothesized that evolution of migratory behaviour is related to behavioural flexibility. According to Sol et al. (2005a), birds that are flexible and capable of behavioural and, more specifically, foraging innovations are able to successfully survive during seasonally changing environments and thus have evolved sedentary habits. Less flexible species, on the other hand, might have evolved migratory behaviour to avoid energetically demanding seasons because they do not have the ability to quickly adjust to changing foraging conditions (Sol et al. 2005a). Sol et al. (2005b) showed that birds capable of flexible behavioural responses had enhanced survival when placed into novel environment, supporting the hypothesis that increased behavioural flexibility might be related to sedentary habits.

Interestingly, at least two studies compared multiple species and reported that compared to nonmigratory species, migratory species have relatively smaller brain size (Winkler et al. 2004; Sol et al. 2005a). In addition, it has been suggested that brain size is a good indicator of behavioural flexibility, and avian species with relatively larger brains are more behaviourally flexible and more capable of behavioural and especially foraging innovations compared to species with smaller brains (Lefebre et al. 1997, 2004; Sol et al. 2005b). Thus, if migratory species have evolved migratory behaviour because they are less behaviourally flexible (at least with regards to foraging behaviour), they should have relatively smaller brain size compared to nonmigratory species, which might provide an explanation for reported differences in brain size between migratory and nonmigratory species (Sol et al. 2005a).

Sol et al. (2005a) hypothesized that some pre-existing differences in behavioural flexibility and brain size preceded the evolution of migratory/sedentary strategies (behavioural flexibility–migratory precursor hypothesis) and suggested that species with larger brains were able to evolve sedentary behaviour, whereas species with smaller brains and associated reduced behavioural flexibility evolved migratory behaviour. At least one alternative hypothesis, however, suggests that changes in brain size might have evolved in response to changes in migratory strategies and that sedentary birds have evolved larger brains because of intense selection pressure for behavioural innovations stemming from the need to survive seasonally changing environment (Winkler et al. 2004). Sol et al. (2005a) argued that this alternative hypothesis is unlikely because differences in brain size between lineages seem to have evolved before the development of recent migratory systems, although there is currently no evidence to support this view. Sol et al. (2005a) also stated that it would be impossible to test whether relative brain size is a precursor or a consequence of migratory strategies because the ancestral states of migratory behaviour are difficult to establish. There are, however, multiple potential alternative hypotheses that might explain why migratory species have smaller brain sizes than nonmigratory species. For example, migratory species might have smaller brain sizes because selection might favour a reduction in brain weight and energy expenditure related to larger brain, which would make long-distance migration more efficient, whereas sedentary species might have evolved larger brains because of no limiting selection on brain size related to migratory behaviour. Alternatively, sedentary species might have evolved a larger brain size because of increased selection pressure for behavioural/foraging innovations, which would increase fitness of birds living in seasonally changing environments, whereas migratory species might have evolved a smaller brain size because they do not require cognitive skills related to behavioural/foraging innovations. There may be more alternative hypotheses, but none of them has been tested so far.

Here, we specifically tested the behavioural flexibility-migratory precursor hypothesis using a model system in which the ancestral migratory state appears to be well established. We compared telencephalon volume (both absolute and relative to body mass) in three subspecies of the white-crowned sparrow, Zonotrichia l. gambelii and Z. l. oriantha, which show ancestral migratory behaviour, and Z. l. nuttalli, which is strictly sedentary. There are five recognized subspecies of the white-crowned sparrow; four of them are migratory and only one, Z. l. nuttalli is nonmigratory. The rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, which has both migratory and nonmigratory populations, is considered the ancestral form for all other Zonotrichia species (Zink et al. 1991; Zink & Blackwell 1996). The white-crowned sparrow, Zonotrichia leucophrys, on the other hand, appears to be the youngest species together with the migratory golden-crowned sparrow, Z. atricapilla. Two Zonotrichia species that have also been assumed to be ancestral to leucophrys are migratory species: the Harris’ sparrow, Z. querula and the white-throated sparrow, Z. albicollis (Zink et al. 1991; Zink & Blackwell 1996). In addition, all species ancestral to Zonotrichia are migratory (Junco sp., Pipilo sp., Melospiza sp., Passerella sp.; Sibley & Ahlquist 1990; Zink & Blackwell 1996). Thus, the common ancestor of Z. leucophrys was probably migratory (Zink 2002). Within the white-crowned sparrow, Z. l. nuttalli and Z. l. pugetensis appear to be the most recent subspecies, whereas migratory Z. l. gambelii and Z. l. oriantha have an earlier origin (Sibley & Ahlquist 1990), which suggests that the ancestors of Z. l. nuttalli were also migratory and that the sedentary habits of Z. l. nuttalli evolved later. Interestingly, nonmigratory Z. l. nuttalli still express migratory restlessness at night during times of regular migration for migratory subspecies (Mewaldt et al. 1968; Smith et al. 1969), which strongly suggests that they still carry migration-related genes inherited from their migratory ancestors (Rappole 2003). Thus, potential differences in brain size between Z. l. nuttalli and the two migratory subspecies are likely to be a result of the switch from migratory habits to sedentary habits. Both Z. l. gambelii and Z. l. oriantha show typical migratory behaviour, migrating hundreds to thousands of kilometres between breeding and wintering grounds every year (Chilton et al. 1995). Zonotrichia l. nuttalli, on the other hand, is strictly sedentary and resides permanently within the same areas along the coastline of California, U.S.A. (Chilton et al. 1995). All other features of ecology such as social structure, breeding and foraging ecology appear to be extremely similar between these subspecies, and thus, the only striking difference between them seems to concern migratory tactics (Chilton et al. 1995). Thus these three subspecies present an excellent model to test the behavioural flexibility–migratory precursor hypothesis because their migratory behaviour falls within clear migratory/sedentary strategies and their ancestral state seems to be well known.

METHODS

We used brains of 13 migratory Z. l. gambelii captured in October 2004 near Davis, California, 13 nonmigratory Z. l. nuttalli captured in October 2004 in Sonoma county, northern California, and 20 migratory Z. l. oriantha captured in August 2004 in northern California. All birds were weighed upon capture and subspecies were identified using plumage and bill coloration in addition to trapping locations (Chilton et al. 1995). All birds were sacrificed within a few days after capture for the brain analyses. There are no data on seasonal variation in brain size in white-crowned sparrows, but work on song sparrows, Melospiza melodia, found no seasonal variation in telencephalon volume (Lee et al. 2001). Two subspecies in our study were collected at the same time, but the third subspecies was collected almost 2 months earlier. While we cannot rule out seasonal differences in brain size between these three subspecies, such differences seem unlikely. These sparrows were trapped as part of different projects investigating spatial memory and the hippocampus (Pravosudov et al. 2006; K. Sanford, T. P. Hahn & V. V. Pravosudov, unpublished), but these data also allowed us to test the behavioural flexibility–migratory precursor hypothesis for the evolution of overall brain size. Brain preparation procedures were identical for all three subspecies (Pravosudov et al. 2006). Sparrows were anaesthetized with nembutal-sodium solution and perfused transcardially with 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer. After perfusion, birds were decapitated and their brain was removed from the skull and placed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 1 week. The brains were cryoprotected in a 30% sucrose solution, frozen on dry ice and kept at −70°C until processing. We cut coronal sections at 40 μm on a sliding, freezing microtome. All details of perfusions and brain preparations have been described previously (Pravosudov et al. 2002, 2006; Pravosudov & Clayton 2002; Pravosudov & Omanska 2005a, b). All brain sections were processed using identical standard procedures to insure consistency of our comparisons.

We used StereoInvestigator software (version 3.15a, Microbrightfield, Colchester, VT) for all stereological measurements. We used the Cavalieri principle (Gunderson & Jensen 1987) to measure the volume of the hippocampus and the rest of the telencephalon on Nissl-stained sections (Pravosudov et al. 2006). The Cavalieri method is an unbiased stereological technique used to measure the volume of various structures, and it provides an alternative to older structure-tracing methods (Gunderson & Jensen 1987). This method has proven to be very reliable in estimating brain volumes in small passerines with extremely low measurements errors (Pravosudov et al. 2002, 2006; Pravosudov & Clayton 2002; Pravosudov & Omanska 2005a, b). To estimate hippocampal volume, we used grid size of a 200 μm and we measured every 12th section; sections were 480 μm apart. For telencephalon measurements, we used a 1142.86-μm grid for Z. l. nuttalli and Z. l. gambelii and a 1200-μm grid for Z. l. oriantha. For Z. l. nuttalli and Z. l. gambelii, we measured every 12th section, and sections were 480 μm apart, whereas for Z. l. oriantha, we measured every 16th section, and sections were 640 μm apart.

To calculate the total volume of the telencephalon, we combined our volume measurements for the hippocampus and the rest of the telencephalon, because these data were initially collected separately to investigate whether migratory behaviour was related to enhanced spatial memory and an enlarged hippocampus (Pravosudov et al. 2006; Sanford et al., unpublished data). We used an ANOVA to test for significant differences between migratory and nonmigratory subspecies in total telencephalon volume. To compare relative telencephalon volume, we used a general linear model (GLM) with subspecies as a fixed factor and body mass as a covariate in all analyses. All assumptions for these statistical tests were met.

RESULTS

Body mass differed significantly between the three subspecies of white-crowned sparrow (F2,43 = 7.9, P < 0.01). Planned comparisons analyses showed that mean ± SD body mass of migratory Z. l. oriantha (28.6 ± 3.11 g) was significantly greater than that of the other two subspecies (P < 0.05), but there were no significant differences in body mass between migratory Z. l. gambelii (24.58 ± 2.89 g) and nonmigratory Z. l. nuttalli (25.92 ± 2.81 g) ( P = 0.26). Telencephalon volume relative to body mass also differed significantly between the three subspecies (F2, 42 = 9.7, P < 0.001; Table 1). Planned comparisons analyses showed that compared to both migratory Z. l. gambelii and Z. l. oriantha, nonmigratory Z. l. nuttalli had significantly larger relative telencephalon volume (P < 0.01), whereas relative telencephalon volumes of migratory Z. l. gambelii and Z. l. oriantha were statistically indistinguishable (P = 0.5; Table 1). Body mass was not a significant predictor of the telencephalon volume (F1, 42 = 0.01, P = 0.92), and results of analyses of absolute telencephalon volume were similar to those of relative telencephalon volume: nonmigratory Z. l. nuttalli had significantly larger absolute telencephalon volume compared to migratory Z. l. gambelii and Z. l. oriantha (P < 0.01), whereas there were no significant difference in absolute telencephalon volume between the two migratory subspecies (P = 0.41; Table 1).

Table 1.

Telencephalone volume (absolute and relative to body mass) in three subspecies of the white-crowned sparrow. Results presented as least square means from General Linear Models with standard errors.

Subspecies Absolute volume, mm3 Volume relative to body mass, mm3 n
Z. l. gambelii 513.4 ± 18.0 512.7 ± 19.6 13
Z. l. oriantha 494.0 ± 14.5 494.7 ± 16.0 20
Z. l. nuttalli 598.1 ± 18.0 597.8 ± 18.4 13

DISCUSSION

Our results showed that the sedentary behaviour of Z. l. nuttalli derived from migratory ancestors was associated with enlarged brain size, suggesting that brain enlargement might have occurred in a subspecies with migratory ancestry. These results contradict the behavioural flexibility (and enlarged brain)–migratory precursor hypothesis (Sol et al. 2005a) and suggest that brain size might have evolved as a consequence of evolutionary changes in behavioural strategies in these birds. Irrespective of whether sedentary habits of Z. l. nuttalli are simply a result of migratory suppression caused by ecological conditions (e.g. Rappole 2003) or have indeed evolved from a migratory ancestor, the relatively larger brain size of these nonmigratory birds suggests that selection pressures associated with a specific migratory or nonmigratory strategy have influenced the evolution of brain size in white-crowned sparrows. The presence of nocturnal migratory restlessness in Z. l. nuttalli, which is strictly sedentary, only strengthens our conclusion that brain size is affected by current behavioural strategies and that large brain size was not a precursor for the development of a sedentary strategy.

Previously, Pravosudov et al. (2006) reported that compared to nonmigratory Z. l. gambelii, migratory Z. l. nuttalli have larger hippocampal volume relative to the remainder of the telencephalon and more hippocampal neurons, suggesting that migratory behaviour might provide high selection pressure on spatial memory and the hippocampus. Such results, combined with the findings of the current study, suggest that migratory strategies might provide opposite selection pressures on different parts of the brain responsible for different functions (Pravosudov et al. 2006). While migratory species might benefit from enhanced spatial memory associated with an enlarged hippocampus, the costs of maintaining larger hippocampus might outweigh such benefits in nonmigratory species. Selection pressures on total brain size, however, show the opposite pattern and appear to favour reduced brain size in migratory species and/or increased brain size in nonmigratory species.

Our comparison concerns only three subspecies, and therefore, it is quite limited in scope. However, even one example should be sufficient to falsify a general hypothesis such as the behavioural flexibility–migratory precursor hypothesis. This hypothesis clearly states that brain size was a precursor to the evolution of migratory/sedentary strategies (Sol et al. 2005a). In contrast, our results demonstrate that larger brain size appears to have evolved in a formerly migratory subspecies, suggesting that brain size was not a precursor to the evolution of sedentary behaviour in Z. l. nuttalli. Thus, the strength of our comparison mainly concerns falsifying the behavioural flexibility-migratory precursor hypothesis rather than establishing a specific alternative hypothesis.

Unfortunately, there are no data on differences in behavioural flexibility between the compared subspecies and so our comparison rests only with brain size data. However, because our study involved different subspecies of the same species, differences in behaviours between these subspecies (other than migratory behaviour) appear to be minimal (Chilton et al. 1995). In particular, social and mating tactics, which might also affect the evolution of brain size, appear to be extremely similar in all three subspecies (Chilton et al. 1995). While it is impossible to rule out all differences between these well-studied subspecies, difference in migratory tactics appears to be the major one (Chilton et al. 1995).

Evidence from another species, dark-eyed junco, Junco hyemalis, which has migratory and nonmigratory populations, also seem to support our conclusions. Absolute brain measurements presented in Cristol et al. (2003) suggest that nonmigratory J. h. carolinensis have significantly larger telencephalon volumes than do migratory J. h. hyemalis. Furthermore, nonmigratory J. h. carolinensis that are truly sedentary have significantly larger telencephalon volumes than do J. h. carolinensis that move short distances during winter (Cristol et al. 2003). Phylogeny data suggest that ancestors of dark-eyed juncos were migratory, because species that diverged basal to dark-eyed juncos (Melospiza spp., Pipilo spp., Passerella spp.) are all migratory (Sibley & Ahlquist 1990; Zink & Blackwell 1996). Thus, dark-eyed juncos might present another example in which secondarily derived sedentary behaviour appear to correlate with larger brain size, suggesting that larger brains might have evolved to support sedentary habits rather than brain size being a precursor to evolution of migratory behaviour.

Thus, our results suggest that the evolution of sedentary behaviour in Nuttall’s white-crowned sparrows was likely shaped by ecological factors independent of pre-existing brain size. Different behavioural strategies, on the other hand, might have provided different selection pressure on behavioural flexibility and brain size, resulting in their gradual evolution following the divergence in such strategies.

Maintaining a large brain appears to be energetically expensive (Armstrong & Bergeron 1985), so selection might favour a decrease in brain size when the cognitive skills associated with large brain size do not increase fitness. Alternatively, the lower energetic costs associated with reductions in overall brain size or relative brain size to body mass may have been favoured by natural selection in migratory species (Winkler et al. 2004). Consequently, migrants may have gradually lost their behavioural innovation capabilities as a result of a gradual decrease in brain size. A sedentary strategy, on the other hand, appears to favour behavioural and foraging innovations, and thus, selection pressure for these traits might result in a gradual increase in brain size in nonmigrants. While our results are consistent with this hypothesis, they do not allow us to rule out other alternative hypotheses. For example, migratory species may maintain a relatively smaller brain size because of the high selection pressure associated with migratory behaviour, but once such pressure has been removed (e.g. as a result of a switch to sedentary life style), these species might evolve larger brains. Thus, many potential alternative hypotheses remain viable, and our results do not allow us to differentiate between them. Irrespective of the causes for the differences in brain size and migratory strategy of migratory and nonmigratory subspecies, our results suggest that the changes in brain size occurred following the changes in behaviour. Our results contradict the behavioural flexibility-migratory precursor hypothesis, which states that the evolution of behaviour followed differences in brain size.

Our results also argue against the hypothesis that main differences in brain size between the lineages have evolved early in evolutionary history, which preceded formation of current migratory systems (Sol et al. 2005a). In our study, a sedentary subspecies appears to have evolved a larger brain size fairly recently compared to the evolution of migratory behaviour in its ancestral species/subspecies which suggests that the evolution of brain size might potentially occur at any time under prevailing selection pressures. More comparisons of species and subspecies with known migratory ancestry are necessary to elucidate general mechanisms of the evolution of brain size in birds.

Acknowledgments

We thank Alicia Omanska for technical assistance, Marty Morton for help collecting birds, Alessandro Graziano, Edward Jones and David Amaral for allowing us to use their laboratories during brain analyses. Comments by Jim Ha and two anonymous reviewers greatly improved the manuscript. This work was partially supported by NIH/NIMH career award to VVP. Birds were collected under California State and Federal Scientific Collecting Permit 801064-04 and a permit from California Department of Parks and Recreation. Work reported here was performed in accordance with University of California Davis animal care protocol 10804.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Armstrong E, Bergeron R. Relative brain size and metabolism in birds. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 1985;53:271–276. doi: 10.1159/000118782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Chilton G, Baker MC, Barrentine CD, Cunningham MA. White-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys) In: Poole A, Gill F, editors. The Birds of North America, No. 183. The Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphi, and The American Ornithologist’s Union; Washington, D. C.: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  3. Cristol DA, Reynolds EB, Leclerc JE, Donner AH, Farabaugh CS, Ziegenfus WS. Migratory dark-eyed juncos, Junco hyemalis, have better spatial memory and denser hippocampal neurons than nonmigratory conspecifics. Animal Behaviour. 2003;66:317–328. [Google Scholar]
  4. Gunderson HJG, Jensen EB. The efficiency of systematic sampling in stereology and its predictions. Journal of Microscopy. 1987;147:229–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2818.1987.tb02837.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Lee DW, Smith GT, Tramontin AD, Soma KK, Brenowitz EA, Clayton NS. Hippocampal volume does not change seasonally in a non food-storing songbird. Neuroreport. 2001;12:1925–1928. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107030-00031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Lefebre L, Whittle P, Lascaris E, Finkelstein A. Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Animal Behaviour. 1997;53:549–560. [Google Scholar]
  7. Lefebre L, Reader SM, Sol D. Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and primates. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2004;63:233–246. doi: 10.1159/000076784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Mewaldt LR, Kibby SS, Morton ML. Comparative biology of pacific coastal white-crowned sparrows. Condor. 1968;70:14–30. [Google Scholar]
  9. Pravosudov VV, Clayton NS. A test of the adaptive specialization hypothesis: population differences in caching, memory and the hippocampus in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla) Behavioral Neuroscience. 2002;116:515–522. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Pravosudov VV, Omanska A. Prolonged moderate elevation of corticosterone does not affect hippocampal anatomy or cell proliferation rates in mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli) Journal of Neurobiology. 2005a;62:82–91. doi: 10.1002/neu.20069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Pravosudov VV, Omanska A. Dominance-related changes in spatial memory are associated with changes in hippocampal cell proliferation rates in mountain chickadees. Journal of Neurobiology. 2005b;62:31–41. doi: 10.1002/neu.20065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Pravosudov VV, Lavenex P, Clayton NS. Changes in spatial memory mediated by experimental variation in food supply do not affect hippocampal anatomy in mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli) Journal of Neurobiology. 2002;51:142–148. doi: 10.1002/neu.10045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Pravosudov VV, Kitaysky AS, Omanska A. The relationship between migratory behavior, memory and the hippocampus – an intraspecific comparison. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences. 2006;273:2641–2649. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Rappole JH. An integrative framework for understanding the origin and evolution of avian migration. Journal of Avian Biology. 2003;34:124–128. [Google Scholar]
  15. Sibley CG, Ahlquist JE. Phylogeny and classification of birds. Yale Univ. Press; New Heaven: 1990. [Google Scholar]
  16. Smith RW, Brown IL, Mewaldt LR. Annual activity patterns of caged non-migratory white-crowned sparrows. Condor. 1969;81:419–440. [Google Scholar]
  17. Sol D, Lefebre L, Rodriguez-Teijero JD. Brain size, innovative propensity and migratory behavior in temperate Palearctic birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences. 2005a;272:1433–1441. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Sol D, Duncan RP, Blackburn TM, Cassey P, Lefebre L. Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2005b;102:5460–5465. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408145102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Weskstein JD, Zink RM, Blackwell-Rago RC, Nelson DA. Anomalous variation in mitochondrial genomes of white-crowned (Zonotrichia leucophrys) and Golden-crowned (Z. atricapilla) sparrows: pseudogenes, hybridization, or incomplete lineage sorting? Auk. 2001;118:231–236. [Google Scholar]
  20. Winkler H, Leisler B, Bernroider G. Ecological constraints on the evolution of avian brains. Journal of Ornithology. 2004;145:238–244. [Google Scholar]
  21. Zink RM. Towards a framework for understanding the evolution of avian migration. Journal of Avian Biology. 2002;33:433–436. [Google Scholar]
  22. Zink RM, Blackwell RC. Patterns of allozyme, mitochondrial DNA, and morphometric variation in four sparrow genera. Auk. 1996;113:59–67. [Google Scholar]
  23. Zink RM, Dittmann DL, Rootes WL. Mitochondrial DNA variation and the phylogeny of Zonotrichia. Auk. 1991;108:578–584. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES