Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2007 May 14.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatr Res. 2006 Sep 11;60(5):513–517. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000242848.45999.7b

Iron Deficiency in Infancy Predicts Altered Serum Prolactin Response 10 Years Later

BARBARA FELT 1, ELIAS JIMENEZ 1, JULIA SMITH 1, AGUSTIN CALATRONI 1, NIKO KACIROTI 1, GLORIA WHEATCROFT 1, BETSY LOZOFF 1
PMCID: PMC1868574  NIHMSID: NIHMS21604  PMID: 16966351

Abstract

Serum prolactin may reflect CNS dopaminergic function. Because iron deficiency (ID) alters brain dopamine in rats, serum prolactin levels were previously investigated in infants with varied iron status. High serum prolactin levels correlated with behaviors typical of chronic ID. The objective of this study was to determine the effect of infant iron status on serum prolactin levels after a stressor in early adolescence. One hundred fifty-nine of 191 children enrolled in infancy (chronic ID, n = 46; good iron comparison group, n = 113) had serum prolactin measurements after catheter placement at 11–14 y of age. Serum prolactin levels were compared by sex, pubertal status and infant iron status and the pattern of change over time was compared by infant iron status controlling for pubertal stage and background factors. Males and less mature adolescents had lower serum prolactin concentrations than females and more mature adolescents. Controlling for these factors, the serum prolactin response pattern differed significantly by infant iron status. Serum prolactin declined earlier for the chronic ID group. In conclusion, an altered serum prolactin response pattern was observed 10 y after chronic ID in infancy and may suggest a long-lasting effect of ID on the regulation of prolactin.

The hormone prolactin has typically been associated with processes of lactation. However, prolactin has also been shown to have a broad range of functions (1-3). An area of particular attention has been prolactin as a stress-response hormone. Studies in biologic psychiatry have documented a rise in serum prolactin levels after physical and psychological stress (4-8).

Serum prolactin has been considered a peripheral indicator of central dopaminergic function, because the neurotransmitter dopamine provides the primary tonic inhibitory influence on prolactin release from the anterior pituitary (9,10). Increased dopamine release inhibits prolactin secretion, as can reduced dopamine reuptake through decreased dopamine transporter (DAT) number or function. Among the family of dopamine receptors, D2 is the primary one associated with the inhibition of prolactin release (9). A complex system has been demonstrated to regulate prolactin and includes other neuro-transmitter systems (e.g., serotonin) (10,11).

We became interested in prolactin in the course of research on iron deficiency, a common nutritional problem that may affect as many as half of the world's infants (12). Of the neurotransmitter systems shown to be altered by iron deficiency in animal studies, dopamine has been studied the most. There is substantial evidence that several aspects of dopaminergic function are altered by iron deficiency (13,14). Early studies showed that iron deficient rats had reduced D2 receptor number and function in the striatum and a behavioral profile similar to that observed with the use of dopamine receptor blocking agents (15). As predicted by these changes, serum prolactin levels and liver prolactin-binding sites were increased in iron deficient rats (16,17). Recent animal studies have suggested more complex dopamine system alterations, including dopamine transporter down-regulation and increased extracellular dopamine levels in the striatum (13).

We previously explored the question of dopaminergic alterations in human iron deficiency by assessing serum prolactin levels in 12- to 23-mo-old infants who participated in a study of the effects of iron deficiency anemia on behavior and development (7,18). Serum was available from the blood obtained for iron status measures before and after three months of iron treatment. Developmental testing preceded venipuncture at both time points. Therefore, serum prolactin levels may have reflected the infant's response to both events. We did not find a significant relationship between infant iron status and serum prolactin levels in that study. However, we found that a high serum prolactin level was associated with the behavioral profile of infants with iron deficiency anemia – wary and hesitant behavior during developmental testing. In addition, serum prolactin levels (high or normal) were surprisingly stable over the two assessments.

In the present study, we obtained serum prolactin levels in these same children in early adolescence. While the heightened serum prolactin levels in infancy were related to the wary and hesitant behavioral profile typical of infants with chronic iron deficiency and not iron status per se (5), we believed that a relationship between iron status and altered serum prolactin remained possible. One reason was that affective differences such as wary and hesitant behavior have been observed in every human study of iron deficiency that included such measures in infancy (14). In addition, animal studies have consistently documented dopaminergic alterations with iron deficiency anemia and have documented hesitant behavior after early iron deficiency as well (13,14,19). Based on the association of elevated serum prolactin with the behavioral profile typical of iron deficiency in infancy and the information available from animal studies, we predicted that having chronic iron deficiency in infancy would be associated with elevated serum prolactin levels after a stressor in early adolescence.

METHODS AND MATERIALS

Subjects

The original cohort consisted of 191 healthy, full-term infants who were enrolled at 12–23 mo of age (between 1983 and 1985) for a study of the behavioral and developmental effects of iron deficiency (18). In infancy, the children resided in a peri-urban, lower-middle-class community in Costa Rica. Infant iron status ranged from moderate iron deficiency anemia to iron sufficiency. Moderate anemia was defined as Hb ≤ 100 g/L and mild anemia was defined as Hb 101–105 g/L. Iron deficiency was defined as serum ferritin ≤ 12 μg/L, and either erythrocyte protoporphyrin > 100 μg/dL packed red blood cells or transferrin saturation ≤ 10%. Iron sufficiency was defined as Hb ≥ 120 g/L and no abnormal iron measures. Infants with Hb < 120 g/L and any degree of iron deficiency (defined above) were given either IM iron (calculated to increase Hb to 125 g/L), or two daily oral doses of ferrous sulfate (3 mg/kg, one dose by project personnel) for a period of three months. Infants who had Hb ≥ 120 g/L and were either iron deficient or iron depleted (serum ferritin < 12 μg/L being the only abnormality) also received the oral iron treatment.

All infants with moderate iron deficiency anemia corrected their anemia with 3 mo of iron treatment. However, 64% of the infants with moderate IDA still had altered iron status measures after iron treatment (e.g., erythrocyte protoporphyrin > 100 μg/dL packed red blood cells or transferrin saturation ≤ 10%). In addition, 17 infants with hemoglobins >100 g/L also had altered iron status measures after iron treatment. Previous analyses have shown that the severity of iron deficiency and response to iron treatment combined are related to CNS outcomes. The entire group of subjects who had moderate iron deficiency anemia in infancy and those who had higher Hb levels but persisting abnormalities of iron status measures after iron treatment were similar in having poorer long-term behavioral and developmental outcomes (20,21). Both groups of infants had evidence of chronic iron deficiency. Infants with moderate IDA had chronic, severe iron deficiency since anemia is a late manifestation of iron deficiency. The infants who had persisting alterations of iron measures had lower hemoglobins and higher erythrocyte protoporphyrin levels at study entry than those infants whose iron measures completely responded to iron treatment. It is important to note that unmodified cow milk was typically introduced in the first few months of life in this population and that subjects were identified as ID at 12–23 mo in the original study. Thus, the iron deficiency was likely to have been chronic. Previous studies have compared the children with evidence of chronic ID in infancy to children who had good iron status in infancy. The good iron status comparison group consisted of infants who were iron sufficient (as described above) at study entry and/or after iron treatment (other than those with moderate IDA) (20,21). The present study uses the same grouping approach.

Of the original infant cohort, 167 (87%) participated in a re-evaluation of behavioral and developmental status in early adolescence (11 to 14 y of age) between 1994 and 1995 (21). Serum samples for prolactin were available for 159 of these children. The average age at follow up was 12.3 ± 0.7 y (range 10.9–13.7 y). There were 70 females and 89 males. Iron status during infancy did not differ significantly for children who participated in this follow-up and those who did not. The project pediatrician obtained signed informed consent from parents and assent from the child. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Michigan, the Hospital Nacional de Ninos, Costa Rica, and the Office of Protection from Research Risks, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD.

Procedures

Sequential blood samples for prolactin levels were collected in conjunction with a venipuncture to determine iron status in early adolescence. To facilitate repeated blood sampling, an angio-catheter was placed at venipuncture by the project pediatrician who was blind to infant iron status group. Venipuncture and catheter placement occurred between 7:30 and 8:00 h to minimize variation of serum iron and prolactin levels related to normal diurnal patterns (22,23). The timing of the subsequent samples was guided by the physiology of serum prolactin response to stress. Previous studies in adults have documented an increase in serum prolactin levels within 5–10 min of a stressful event, a peak between 20–30 min, then a decline by 45– 60 min (24). We collected the first sample at venipuncture for catheter placement (the stressor), and sequential samples were planned at 15 min, 30 min, and 45 min thereafter. Between the initial and second samples, all children had a complete physical examination that included an assessment of pubertal stage by standard protocol (25). After the second sample, the children had a break during which they rested, read and had a snack. The children had no other activities between the third and fourth samples. All blood samples for prolactin measurement were immediately centrifuged and the sera separated. Sera were stored at −20°C until assay in duplicate for prolactin concentration (mIU/L) using standard double-antibody RIA kits from ICN Pharmaceuticals, Costa Mesa, California. The assays were completed within 3–4 y of collection. Inter-assay variability was 15% and the intra-assay variability was 13%.

Our overall measure of pubertal development was pubic hair Tanner stage (1-5). This measure correlated highly with all other ratings of pubertal development (axillary hair, breast or genital development, and menstruation status (all values r > 0.70)), and thus, could be applied to both boys and girls. Children were categorized as follows: Tanner stage 1—less mature; Tanner stage 2 and 3—intermediate mature; and Tanner stage 4 and 5—more mature.

Statistical analysis

The children in the chronic ID group (n = 46) were compared with the children who had had good iron status in infancy (the comparison group, n = 113) for age, sex, pubertal development, time of serum prolactin samples, and serum prolactin concentrations using t-test for continuous and normally distributed variables, the Mantel-Haenzael χ2 test for ordered categorical variables, and the Fisher's exact test for dichotomous variables. Serum prolactin levels at each time point were then compared by infant iron status controlling for propensity score, pubertal status and first serum sample prolactin concentration using linear mixed models. We used a propensity score to reduce bias due to group differences in background characteristics and other factors. Propensity scores have been increasingly used in epidemiologic and medical research as an index of the risk (propensity) for the condition of interest (26). The propensity score, consisting of relevant risk factors, is used as a control variable in analyses of outcome. In this study, the condition of interest was iron status in infancy (chronic ID versus the comparison, good iron status group). Based on our previous work, factors that could influence the probability of iron status group were included in the propensity score using multivariate logistic regression. The factors included sex, birth weight, infant feeding history, age at initial enrollment, mother's IQ, socioeconomic status, family structure, and quality of the home environment. The area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (c-statistic) for the propensity score model in this study was 0.84, indicating excellent discrimination between iron status groups. The propensity score was divided into quintiles and incorporated as a stratified control variable. A linear mixed effects model growth curve analysis was used to account for correlations among observations due to sequential measurements taken on the same individual. The initial level of prolactin after a stressor affects the range of the response curve thereafter (e.g., lower initial levels constrain the amount of possible decline) (27). Therefore, this was controlled in this analysis. We assumed a spatial autocorrelation structure for the covariance matrix where the strength of the correlation between observations was a function of the time interval between measurements. Separate covariance structures were estimated for the chronic ID and good iron status groups due to unequal variances. All statistical tests were two-tailed with alpha levels set at 0.05. Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS Version 9.1 (28).

RESULTS

In early adolescence, no child was anemic and only 2% (n = 3) had biochemical evidence of iron deficiency (21). The characteristics of the children who had chronic ID in infancy and those who had good iron status before and/or after iron therapy (comparison group) are described in Table 1. There were significantly more males in the chronic ID group and, at the time of assessment, these adolescents were less mature than the comparison group. The actual times of the serum prolactin samples were recorded and found to vary somewhat by individual. However, there was no significant difference for the mean sample times when compared by iron-status group (Table 1).

Table 1.

Subject characteristics and sample collection times by infant iron status

Chronic Iron
Deficiency Group
(n = 46)
Comparison
Group
(n = 113)
p-value
Gender (female)* 13 (28%)   57 (50%)   <0.02
Age (years)** 12.1 (0.6) 12.4 (0.6)  <0.01
Pubertal development*
  Less mature (Tanner I) 23 (50%)  34 (30%) 
  Intermediate mature (Tanner II–III) 18 (39%)  45 (40%) 
  More mature (Tanner IV–V)  5 (11%) 34 (30%)   0.01
Time of serum samples (min)**
  First sample 0 0
  Second sample 15.8 (6.6) 15.6 (6.3)   0.84
  Third sample 30.2 (7.2) 30.7 (7.1)   0.74
  Fourth sample 52.6 (9.8) 52.6 (10.3)  0.97
*

Number of participants (percent within infant iron status group).

**

Mean (SD).

Serum prolactin levels were compared by sex and pubertal status. In this early adolescent period, males had significantly lower serum prolactin levels than females at each sample time (Table 2). In addition, the less mature adolescents had significantly lower serum prolactin levels at each sample time than those who were more mature (Table 3).

Table 2.

Serum prolactin concentrations at each sample time by sex

Sample Female
(n = 70)
Male
(n = 89)
p-value
1 391.2 (194.5) 331.1 (177.6)  0.04
2 366.3 (202.1) 294.6 (166.1)  0.02
3 323.0 (169.2) 256.0 (131.6)  0.01
4 294.9 (147.7) 229.3 (108.1) <0.01

Serum prolactin concentrations (mIU/L) at each sample time (0, 15, 30, 50 minutes) after venipuncture and catheter placement. Participants are compared by Student's t- test according to sex. Values are expressed as mean (SD).

Table 3.

Serum prolactin concentrations by pubertal status

Pubertal Development
Sample Less Mature
(n = 57)
Intermediate
Mature
(n = 63)
More Mature
(n = 39)
p-value
1 319.1 (163.7)* 351.3 (193.4)** 428.1 (196.7)  0.02
2 296.1 (167.7)* 317.2 (199.8)   383.1 (180.6)  0.07
3 255.6 (130.1)* 285.9 (175.0)   328.1 (138.7) <0.01
4 227.1 (104.5)* 244.9 (117.1)** 323.7 (161.1) <0.01

Serum prolactin concentrations (mIU/L) at each sample time (0, 15, 30, 50 minutes) after venipuncture and catheter placement. Participants are compared by Student's t -test according to pubertal status and values are expressed as mean (SD).

Pubertal Development (Pubic Hair): Tanner stage I, less mature; Tanner stage II–III, intermediately mature; Tanner stage IV–V, more mature.

Pair-wise comparisons p < 0.05:

*

Less mature versus more mature

**

Intermediate mature versus more mature.

Comparing serum prolactin concentrations at each time point by infant iron status, the chronic ID group had significantly lower levels at 15 min after venipuncture (unadjusted values, Table 4). After controlling for propensity score, pubertal status and the first prolactin sample concentration in the linear effects model, group differences were not significant (adjusted values, Table 4).

Table 4.

Unadjusted and adjusted serum prolactin levels by infant iron status group

Sample Value Chronic ID
Group
Comparison
Group
p-value
1 unadjusted 332.0 (155.5) 368.3 (198.3) 0.27
2 unadjusted 281.8 (139.8) 343.8 (198.9) 0.03
adjusted 312.3 (97.39) 348.8 (133.3) 0.08
3 unadjusted 257.6 (124.5) 295.8 (161.1) 0.16
adjusted 293.4 (97.00) 297.5 (133.9) 0.84
4 unadjusted 228.9 (119.0) 269.2 (133.5) 0.08
adjusted 265.9 (96.8)  271.7 (133.6) 0.78

Unadjusted and adjusted serum prolactin concentrations (mIU/L) at each sample time: 1, at venipuncture and catheter placement; 2, 15 minutes; 3, 30 minutes; 4, 50 minutes after venipuncture and catheter placement. Values are expressed as mean (SD). Unadjusted values are compared by Student's t-test according to infant iron status group. Adjusted values are compared by infant iron status group controlling for propensity score, pubertal stage, and the first prolactin sample concentration.

The change in serum prolactin between samples 2 (15 min) and 3 (30 min), and between samples 3 and 4 (50 min) were compared within and between infant iron status groups using the linear effects model (Table 5). The pattern of serum prolactin response differed for chronic ID versus the good iron status comparison group. The adolescents that had chronic ID in infancy had significantly less decline between 15 and 30 min than the good iron status comparison group. The iron status groups did not differ for amount of serum prolactin change between 30 to 50 min after venipuncture.

Table 5.

Change in serum prolactin concentrations by infant iron status group

Mean prolactin
change between
times 2–3 and 3–4
Iron Status
Group
Sample 2–3 p-value* Sample 3–4 p-value*
Chronic ID group 18.8(8.9)  0.04  27.5(10.6) <0.01
Comparison group 51.3(8.6) <0.01 25.8(9.9)   <0.01
p-value** <0.01    0.91  

Mean difference for adjusted serum prolactin concentrations controlling for propensity score, pubertal stage, and first prolactin sample concentrations. Sample times after venipuncture and catheter placement: 2, 15 minutes; 3, 30 minutes; 4, 50 minutes.

*

p -value for difference in change for a given time interval within iron status group.

**

p -value for difference in change for a given time interval between iron status groups.

DISCUSSION

This follow-up of early adolescents who had participated in a study of iron deficiency in infancy provided a unique opportunity to assess long-term alterations in the response of serum prolactin to stress in relation to infant iron status. Based on the association between high serum prolactin levels and the behavioral profile of iron deficiency in infancy and the effects of iron deficiency on the dopamine system in animal studies, we had hypothesized that children who had chronic ID in infancy would show elevated serum prolactin levels 10 y later. The adolescents who had chronic ID in infancy did not show the predicted pattern. Instead, they demonstrated a more prompt decline of serum prolactin shortly after venipuncture, compared with those who had good iron status in infancy.

Dopamine, a central factor in the production, release and reuptake of prolactin, is the primary direct tonic inhibitor of prolactin secretion (9,10). Of three major hypothalamic neuronal populations, tuberoinfundibular dopminergic neurons (TIDA) are considered the primary source of dopamine for inhibiting prolactin secretion (10). Iron deficiency early in life in rats has been shown to alter striatal dopamine regulation, (13,14) but the TIDA system has not been directly studied in the context of iron deficiency. The hypothalamic-TIDA dopamine system is thought to differ from dopaminergic systems in other brain areas and there are species-specific differences (10). However, increased serum prolactin levels and liver prolactin-binding sites have been observed in iron deficient rats (16,17).

The potential biologic mechanisms that underlie our findings of altered prolactin regulation years after a period of chronic iron deficiency are unknown and must be considered cautiously. If early life iron deficiency affected the TIDA dopamine system, one might expect decreased dopaminergic inhibition and higher prolactin levels while iron deficient. Although we did not demonstrate this in infancy, (7) recent findings that infants of anemic mothers with or without malnutrition had elevated cord blood prolactin levels provides support for this mechanism early in life (29). In the present study, we observed an altered serum prolactin pattern in response to stressor more than 10 y after ID in infancy. The pattern is consistent with a swifter response of dopaminergic inhibition. Decreased DAT number or function, as recently observed in the striatal system of iron-deficient rats, could allow extracellular dopamine to accumulate and inhibit prolactin release (14). In addition, chronically high prolactin levels in infancy might lead to fundamental alterations that increased the number of TIDA neurons, with a corresponding increase in prolactin inhibition over time (30). Alterations in other aspects of prolactin regulation are also possible, (e.g., serotonin) (31).

Recent research indicates that early life experiences can result in fundamental neurobiologic changes. The development of CNS regulatory mechanisms for prolactin has been demonstrated to be dynamic, (32-34) and in one rodent study, early postnatal enrichment of maternal care (handling) was associated with lowered serum prolactin response to fearful situations later in life (35). Studies in humans with mental health conditions, such as bipolar disorder and bullemia nervosa, have noted blunted prolactin responses after pharmacologic challenge and serum prolactin responses correlated with the degree of early life trauma. In these conditions, altered serotonin biology was proposed as the indirect mechanism leading to altered dopamine and serum prolactin levels (31,36). Investigations of other neurohormonal systems are also relevant. Rodent studies of the developing hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the pattern of corticosterone response to stress have shown that the timing and duration of early stressful experiences affects the direction of the stress-response system long-term, resulting in exaggerated or blunted corticosterone response patterns (37,38).

There were limitations in evaluating serum prolactin responses in the children in this study. In infancy, serum prolactin levels did not differ significantly by sex or age (7). However, as these children entered adolescence, sex and pubertal stage became important factors as observed by others (5,39). We statistically controlled for propensity score (which included sex) and pubertal status, but a stronger design would be to assess children with more equal distribution by group for sex and pubertal status. Another important limitation in this study was that the venipuncture (the stressor of interest) and the first serum sample occurred simultaneously. Both groups showed declining prolactin concentrations thereafter indicating that the peak response in these young adolescents may have occurred in anticipation of, and during the venipuncture, in contrast to the initial rise post-venipuncture observed in adults. It is possible that the chronic ID group subjects had more anticipatory distress that resulted in a peak serum prolactin response even earlier than the good iron status comparison group. An earlier peak before catheter placement could explain the differences in decline of serum prolactin over time. In addition, it is important to recall that more of the chronic ID group adolescents had been rated as wary and hesitant during developmental testing in infancy. This raises the possibility of enduring behavioral differences that might underlie the difference in serum prolactin response. Future studies could explore this further by a repeated sampling approach with a stress after reaching baseline prolactin levels and behavioral measures. Finally, other factors such as dietary tryptophan intake were not monitored in these children and might have affected serum prolactin levels (40).

In summary, serum prolactin levels have not previously been assessed years after a period of iron deficiency in infancy. Our results show an altered serum prolactin response pattern in adolescents who had chronic ID in infancy. We suggest this finding reflects fundamental alterations in the regulation of serum prolactin due to effects of early iron deficiency on the dopamine system.

Abbreviations

ID

iron deficiency

TIDA

tuberoinfundibular dopminergic neurons

Footnotes

This study was supported by NIH HD36106 and a MERIT Award to B. Lozoff, R37HD31606.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Ben-Jonathon N, Mershon JL, Allen DL, Steinmetz RW. Extrapituitary prolactin: distribution, regulation, functions, and clinical aspects. Endocr Rev. 1996;17:639–669. doi: 10.1210/edrv-17-6-639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bole-Feysot C, Goffin V, Edery M, Binart N, Kelly P. Prolactin (PRL) and its receptor: actions, signal transduction pathways and phenotypes observed in PRL receptor knockout mice. Endocr Rev. 1998;19:225–268. doi: 10.1210/edrv.19.3.0334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Chikanza IC. Prolactin and neuroimmunomodulation: in vitro and in vivo observations. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;876:119–130. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb07629.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Biondi M, Peronti M, Pacitti F, Pancheri P, Pacifici R, Altieri I, Paris L, Zuccaro P. Personality, endocrine and immune changes after eight months in healthy individuals under normal daily stress. Psychother Psychosom. 1994;62:176–184. doi: 10.1159/000288920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gerra G, Zaimovic A, Giucastro G, Folli F, Maestri D, Tessoni A, Avanzini P, Caccavari R, Bernasconi S, Brambilla F. Neurotransmitter-hormonal responses to psychological stress in peripubertal subjects: relationship to aggressive behavior. Life Sci. 1998;62:617–625. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(97)01157-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lindh A, Carlstrom K, Eklund J, Wilking N. Serum steroids and prolactin during and after major surgical trauma. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 1992;36:119–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-6576.1992.tb03436.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lozoff B, Felt BT, Nelson EC, Wolf AW, Meltzer HW, Jimenez E. Serum prolactin levels and behavior in infants. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37:4–12. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00148-V. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sobrinho LG. Emotional aspects of hyperprolactinemia. Psychother Psychosom. 1998;67:133–139. doi: 10.1159/000012273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Benker G, Jaspers C, Hausler G, Reinwein D. Control of prolactin secretion. Klin Wochenschr. 1990;68:1157–1167. doi: 10.1007/BF01815271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ben-Jonathan N, Hnasko R. Dopamine as a prolactin (PRL) inhibitor. Endocr Rev. 2001;22:724–763. doi: 10.1210/edrv.22.6.0451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Albinsson A, Palazidou E, Stephenson J, Andersson G. Involvement of the 5-HT2 receptor in the 5-HT receptor-mediated stimulation of prolactin release. Eur J Pharmacol. 1994;251:157–161. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(94)90396-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.deMaeyer E, Adiels-Tegman M. The prevalence of anaemia in the world. World Health Stat Q. 1985;38:302–316. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Beard JL, Connor JR. Iron status and neural functioning. Annu Rev Nutr. 2003;23:41–58. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.23.020102.075739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lozoff B, Beard J, Connor J, Felt B, Georgieff M, Schallert T. Long-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy. Nutr Rev. 2006;64:S34–S43. doi: 10.1301/nr.2006.may.S34-S43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Youdim MB. Brain iron metabolism biochemical and behavioral aspects in relation to dopaminergic neurotransmission. In: Lagoth A, editor. Handbook of Neurochemistry. Plenum Press; New York: 1985. pp. 731–755. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Barkey RJ, Ben-Shachar D, Amit T, Youdim MB. Increased hepatic and reduced prostatic prolactin (PRL) binding in iron deficiency and during neuroleptic treatment: correlation with changes in serum PRL and testosterone. Eur J Pharmacol. 1985;109:193–200. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(85)90420-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Barkey RJ, Amit T, Ben-Shachar D, Youdim MB. Characterization of the hepatic prolactin receptors induced by chronic iron deficiency and neuroleptics. Eur J Pharmacol. 1986;122:259–267. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(86)90111-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lozoff B, Brittenham GM, Wolf AW, McClish DK, Kuhnert PM, Jimenez E, Jimenez R, Mora LA, Gomez I, Krauskopf D. Iron deficiency anemia and iron therapy: effects on infant developmental test performance. Pediatrics. 1987;79:981–995. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Felt BT, Beard JL, Schallert T, Shao J, Aldridge JW, Connor JR, Georgieff MK, Lozoff B. Persistent neurochemical and behavioral abnormalities in adulthood despite early iron supplementation for perinatal iron deficiency anemia in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006;171:261–270. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Wolf AW. Long-term developmental outcome of infants with iron deficiency. N Engl J Med. 1991;325:687–694. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199109053251004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, Mollen E, Wolf AW. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105:E51. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.4.e51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Patterson JC, Marrack D, Wiggins HS. The diurnal variation of the serum iron level in erythropoietic disorders. J Clin Pathol. 1953;6:105–109. doi: 10.1136/jcp.6.2.105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sassin JF, Frantz AG, Weitzman ED, Kapen S. Human prolactin: 24 hour pattern with increase release during sleep. Science. 1972;177:1205–1207. doi: 10.1126/science.177.4055.1205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Richter SD, Schurmeyer TH, Schedlowski M, Hadicke A, Tewes U, Schmidt RE, Wagner TO. Time kinetics of the endocrine response to acute psychological stress. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;81:1956–1960. doi: 10.1210/jcem.81.5.8626864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tanner JM. Growth at Adolescence. Blackwell Scientific; Oxford: 1962. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.D'Agostino RB., Jr Propensity score methods for bias reduction in the comparison of a treatment to a non-randomized control group. Stat Med. 1998;17:2265–2281. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0258(19981015)17:19<2265::aid-sim918>3.0.co;2-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wilder J. The law of initial value in neurology and psychiatry: Facts and problems. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1957;125:73–86. doi: 10.1097/00005053-195701000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Littell RC, Milliken GA, Stroup WW, Wolfinger RD. SAS System for Mixed Models. SAS Institute; Cary: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mahajan SD, Singh S, Shah P, Gupta N, Kochupillai N. Effect of maternal malnutrition and anemia on the endocrine regulation of fetal growth. Endocr Res. 2004;30:189–203. doi: 10.1081/erc-200027380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Phelps CJ. Pituitary hormones as neurotrophic signals: anomalous hypophysiotrophic neuron differentiation in hypopituitary dwarf mice. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med. 1994;206:6–23. doi: 10.3181/00379727-206-43719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Steiger H, Gauvin L, Israel M, Koerner N, Ng Ying Kin NM, Paris J, Young SN. Association of serotonin and cortisol indices with childhood abuse in bulimia nervosa. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:837–843. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.9.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Freemark M, Nagano M, Edery M, Kelly P. Prolactin receptor gene expression in the fetal rat. J Endocrinol. 1995;144:285–292. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1440285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Freemark M, Driscoll P, Andrews J, Kelly P, Royster M. Ontogenesis of prolactin receptor gene expression in the rat olfactory system: potential roles for lactogenic hormones in olfactory development. Endocrinology. 1996;137:934–942. doi: 10.1210/endo.137.3.8603606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Shah GV, Shyr SW, Grosvenor CE, Crowley WR. Hyperprolactinenmia after neonatal prolactin (PRL) deficiency in rats: evidence for altered anterior pituitary regulation of PRL secretion. Endocrinology. 1988;122:1883–1889. doi: 10.1210/endo-122-5-1883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rots NY, deJong J, Workel JO, Levine S, Cools AR, deKloet R. Neonatal maternally deprived rats have as adults elevated basal pituitary-adrenal activity and enhanced susceptibility to apomorphine. J Neuroendocrinol. 1996;8:501–506. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1996.04843.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rinne T, Westenberg HG, den Boer JA, van den Brink W. Serotonergic blunting to meta-chlorophenylpiperazine (m-CPP) highly correlates with sustained childhood abuse in impulsive and autoaggressive female borderline patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:548–556. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00181-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-nduced release in adult rats. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1993;18:195–200. doi: 10.1016/0169-328x(93)90189-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.van Oers HJ, de Kloet ER, Levine S. Early vs. late maternal deprivation differentially alters the endocrine and hypothalamic responses to stress. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1998;111:245–252. doi: 10.1016/s0165-3806(98)00143-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Waterman GS, Dahl RE, Birmaher B, Ambrosini P, Rabinovich H, Williamson D, Novacenko H, Nelson B, Puig-Antich J, Ryan ND. The 24-hour pattern of prolactin secretion in depressed and normal adolescents. Biol Psychiatry. 1994;35:440–445. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)90041-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Attenburrow MJ, Williams C, Odontiadis J, Powell J, Van de Ouderaa F, Williams M, Cowen PJ. The effect of a nutritional source of tryptophan on dieting-induced changes in brain 5-HT function. Psychol Med. 2003;33:1381–1386. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703008547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES