Abstract
Drug-induced craving is thought to play an important role in relapse occasioned by a “slip”, or an isolated use of a previously abused drug after a period of abstinence. Clinical experience suggests that acute exposure to cocaine elicits craving (hereafter referred to as “priming”); however, this has received surprisingly little attention in the clinical literature.
Aims
The intentions of this review are to provide a qualitative review of the literature as well as a more stringent quantitative review of the existence and presence of cocaine-induced priming effects.
Methods
In order to determine whether priming effects occur following cocaine administration, we conducted qualitative and quantitative reviews of studies in which participants received cocaine under experimentally controlled conditions in the laboratory.
Results
The results of the qualitative review were equivocal, while the quantitative review revealed that cocaine administration was associated with a significant increase in craving for cocaine, and the effect size of this relationship was large.
Conclusion
A review of the individual studies revealed marked variability, suggesting that priming effects did not occur consistently and that there may be factors that mediate or moderate the intensity of the priming effects induced by cocaine. The implications of these findings are discussed.
Keywords: cocaine, priming, craving, reinstatement, relapse
Introduction
Several preclinical studies have shown that experimenter administered cocaine reinstated extinguished responding for animals trained to self-administer cocaine (de Wit & Stewart, 1981; Schenk & Partridge, 1999; Weissenborn, Yackey, Koob, & Weiss, 1995). It was found in the preceding studies that after receiving a “priming” dose of cocaine, rats increased cocaine self-administration, an occurrence known as “reinstatement”. Following this “priming” dose, the incident of reinstatement is a common and consistent happening across the preclinical literature.
In 1989, Jaffe and colleagues (Jaffe, Cascella, Kumor, & Sherer, 1989) reported that experimental administration of cocaine in humans was associated with increased “wanting” and “craving” for cocaine. The issue of increased “wanting” or “craving” following cocaine administration, which has also been characterized as “priming” (de Wit, 1996), has been the subject of follow-up investigations. Unlike the preclinical literature, the findings of these clinical investigations were mixed; whereas a subset of studies replicated the results reported by Jaffe and colleagues (Fischman, Foltin, Nestadt, & Pearlson, 1990; Jaffe et al., 1989; Kosten et al., 1992; Nann-Vernotica, Donny, Bigelow, & Walsh, 2001; Sofuoglu, Brown, Babb, Pentel, & Hatsukami, 2000a, b; Walsh, Haberny, & Bigelow, 2000; Ward, Haney, Fischman, & Foltin, 1997a, 1997b; Ward, Haney, Fischman, & Foltin, 1998), others did not (De La Garza, Newton, & Kalechstein, 2005; Foltin & Haney, 2000; Foltin et al., 2003; Romach et al., 1999; Sofuoglu, Pentel, Bliss, Goldman, & Hatsukami, 1999). To our knowledge, the clinical phenomenon of priming has not been addressed by a formal literature review. Because “priming” may be an important factor in relapse to dependence (de Wit, 1996), it seems essential to confirm that the association between cocaine administration and subsequent craving for the drug.
To address this, we conducted a qualitative review and a quantitative meta-analytic review of studies in which participants received cocaine under experimentally-controlled conditions. The qualitative review is intended to give an overview of previous studies that have reported cocaine-induced craving, regardless of whether actual statistics were provided. The quantitative review provides a statistical analysis, thus providing a more concrete answer to the debate. The meta-analytic approach provides a number of advantages over qualitative reviews. A primary advantage is this approach is that it provides the capacity to combine the results of studies utilizing different methodologies and statistical models in order to calculate a single effect size that quantifies the magnitude of the association between two variables, substantially increasing statistical power (Rosenthal, 1991). However, one limitation of the meta-analytic approach is that the review includes only those publications that include sufficient data to convert the statistical findings into effect sizes. As a result, studies that may be otherwise informative are generally excluded. To assure adequate coverage of all possibilities, the current report includes both a qualitative analysis and a quantitative meta-analytic review.
Methods
Qualitative Review
Pub-Med searches of studies published from 1966 through November 2006 were used to identify studies in which cocaine was administered in the laboratory and changes in self-reported craving or desire for cocaine were recorded. Keywords included “cocaine”, “administration”, “and “human” (as well as “cocaine craving”) with follow-up searches using the reference sections of the initial studies. The initial search identified 2,801 reports.
Studies were included in the qualitative review if cocaine was administered in the laboratory, craving was assessed, and there was an interpretable report of craving. The results may have been reported as statistics, or as a figure or table showing whether the priming effect was observed following cocaine administration. Based on these criteria, 38 studies were considered appropriate for inclusion in the qualitative review.
Quantitative Review
Studies were included in the quantitative, meta-analytic review if the sample was greater than or equal to four study participants (to enhance stability of the findings), if cocaine was administered via the intravenous (IV) or smoked routes, if there was a single dose of cocaine administered, and if cocaine was the only stimulant administered. Studies utilizing oral and nasal routes of administration were excluded from the quantitative review because cocaine taken by these routes is absorbed more slowly and this may alter the subjective effects produced (Kouri, Lundahl, Borden, McNeil, & Lukas, 2002). In addition, it has been found that the smoked and IV routes of administration have similar pharmacodynamic characteristics as well as similar times to reach the brain (Jeffcoat, 1989). Studies that did not provide the necessary statistics (e.g. means and standard deviations and/or standard errors) were not included in the quantitative review; however, these articles were still included in the qualitative review. Moreover, studies that did not use standard terms for assessing cocaine-induced craving, such as “crave”, “craving”, “desire”, “want,” were excluded. Based on these more stringent selection criteria, 12 studies were included in the quantitative review. Please see Table 1 for list of studies included in the quantitative review (designated with a # symbol next to the author line). Data were coded by both ADK and JJM to ensure accurate calculation during the analysis.
Table 1.
Studies included in the qualitative and quantitative reviews
First author | Year | Sample | ROUTE1 | Dose (mg) | RATE | Adjective | Measure to Assess Craving2 | Effect of Cocaine3 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Usual | Study | ||||||||
Collins | 1998 | 8M/0F | Smk | Smk | 12, 25, and 50 | * | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
De La Garza | 2005 | 7M/0F | Smk or IV | IV | 40 | 40 | Craving | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Donny | 2003 | 7M/1F | Smk or IV | IV | 12.5, 25, 50 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | + |
Donny# | 2004 | 8M/4F | Smk or IV | IV | 15 and 30 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Donny | 2006 | 8M/2F | Smk or IV | IV | 20 and 40 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Fischman# | 1990 | 6M/0F | NR | IV | 8, 16, and 32 | 60 | Want | 50 mm (line) | + |
Foltin | 2000 | 6M/2F | Smk | Smk | 25 | NR | Want | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Foltin | 2003 | 11M/3F | Smk | Smk | 12, 25, 50 | NR | Want | 100 mm (line) | − |
Foltin | 2004 | 10M/0F | Smk | Nasal | 4, 24, 48, 96 | 60 | Want | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Haney | 1998 | 7M/5F | Smk or IV | IV | 8, 16, 32 | 30 | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
Haney | 1999 | 8M/1/F | Smk | Smk | 12 and 50 | * | Want | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Haney | 2001 | 8M/2F | Smk | Smk | 12, 25, and 50 | * | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
Haney | 2005 | 8M/0F | Smk | Smk | 6, 12, 25, 50 | * | Want | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Haney | 2006 | 9M/1F | Smk | Smk | 12, 25, and 50 | * | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
Hart | 2004 | 6M/1F | Smk | Smk | 12, 25, and 50 | * | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
Houtsmuller | 2004 | 10M/2F | Smk or IV | IV | 20 and 40 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | + |
Jaffe# | 1989 | 9M/0F | IV | IV | 40 | 2 | Craving | 0 –5 | + |
Johnson# | 2004 | 12M/6F | IV | IV | 22.75 and 45.5 | NR | Craving | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Johanson | 2006 | 9M/1F | Smk or IV | IV | 10, 20, and 40 | 60 | CCQ-Now | NR | + |
Kosten | 1992 | 4M/1F | Smk or IV | IV | 8.75, 17.5, 35 | NR | Desire | 10 point | + |
Leyton | 2005 | 8M/0F | Nasal | Nasal | 42, 105, and 210 | ** | Multiple | 10 point | + |
Nann-Vernotica# | 2001 | 9M/1F | Smk or IV | IV | 25, 50 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | + |
Nelson | 2005 | 17M/F | Smk or IV | IV | 10, 25, 50 | 10, 30, 60 | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
Preston# | 1992 | 8M/0F | IV | IV | 12.5, 25, and 50 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | + |
Preston# | 1993 | 8M/0F | IV | IV | 12.5, 25, and 50 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | + |
Roache | 2005 | 11M/1F | IV | IV | 22.75 and 45.5 | 60 | Crave | 100 mm (line) | + |
Romach | 1999 | 11M/0F | IV | IV | 30 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Rush | 1999 | 8M/1F | Smk | Oral | 50, 100, 200, 300 | Immediate | Want | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Sofuoglu | 1999 | 6M/3F | Smk | Smk | 28 | 25 | Crave | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Sofuoglu# | 2000a | 9M/3F | NR | Smk | 28 | 25*** | Crave | 100 mm (line) | + |
Sofuoglu | 2000b | 9M/3F | NR | Smk | 28 | 25*** | Crave | 100 mm (line) | + |
Sofuoglu | 2003 | 25M/19F | Smk | Smk | 28 | 25*** | Desire | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Sofuoglu | 2005 | 5M/2F | Smk or IV | IV | 11 and 21 | 60 | Crave | 100 mm (line) | + |
Walsh# | 2000 | 7M/1F | IV | IV | 25, 50 | 60 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | + |
Ward# | 1997a | 7M/0F | IV | IV | 32 | 30 | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
Ward# | 1997b | 7M/0F | Smk | Smk | 50 | * | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
Ward# | 1998 | 7M/1F | Smk | Smk | 12 and 50 | * | Want | 100 mm (line) | + |
Winhusen | 2006 | 7M/F | IV | IV | 20, 40 | 120 | Desire | 100 mm (line) | 0 |
Abbreviations and Notes: M = female, F = female, IV =intravenous, NR = not recorded, Smk = smoked
– Usual = route of administration participant uses in their natural environment, Study = route of administration participant received in the laboratory study described.
– 0 – 100 mm (line) = VAS where participant marks along a 100-point line labeled “not at all” to “extremely” 0 – 5 = participant marks between 0 and 5
= participant told to inhale similarly to outside the laboratory and/or finish the smoke inside the pipe
= participant told to insufflate at own pace
= participant told to inhale for 10 second and then hold for 15 seconds
+ = increase in craving after cocaine administration (and/or stated as a significant increase within the paper)
0 = no change in craving after cocaine administration (and/or stated as a nonsignificant change within the paper)
− = decrease in craving after cocaine administration (and/or stated as a nonsignificant decrease within the paper)
= study also included in the quantitative, meta-analytic review
Meta-analytic methods
Effect Size
In order to determine the magnitude of the differences between the means of comparison groups, effect size estimates were calculated using standard techniques (Rosenthal, 1991). Estimates of the magnitude of the association between cocaine administration and change in craving were operationally defined as Pearson’s r and Cohen’s d
Combined Probabilities
To compute combined probabilities for the various subgroups of studies, we employed the method described by Rosenthal (Rosenthal, 1991). Specifically, we derived the meta-analytic Z (Zma) by converting the Pearson’s r for each correlation to a Z score, summing the Z scores for each subgroup of investigations, and dividing this sum by the square root of the number of studies included for each subgroup. The p-value associated with the Zma indicates the level of statistical significance for associations between drug administration and self-reported craving.
Fail-safe N
If Zma was significant, we computed the Fail-safe N (Nfs) to determine the number of studies with null findings that would be needed to invalidate the conclusion that a significant association existed between cocaine administration and craving (Rosenthal, 1991). Nfs was obtained by summing the Z scores for the particular subgroup, dividing this sum by the Z score associated with a particular probability value, squaring this new number, and finally, subtracting the number of studies in that subgroup. A larger Nfs indicates a more reliable association between cocaine administration and craving.
Estimating the magnitude of the effect size
According to previously published methods (Pedhazur & Schmelkin, 1991), Cohen constructed the most frequently used guidelines for the estimation of the magnitude of effect size using Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988). For the latter, Cohen proposed that a difference between means of .2 of a standard deviation be characterized as small, .5 as medium, and .8 as large. Another measure of estimating the magnitude of effect size is Pearson’s r. A measure of .1 is considered small, .25 is considered medium, and an effect size of .4 is considered large.
Moderating variables
Effect sizes were calculated separately for studies using the adjectives “crave”, “desire”, and “want” to characterize these responses separately. Similarly, effect sizes were calculated separately for studies employing smoked and IV route of administration, and for low (<48mg) and high (>49mg) cocaine doses (based on a median split of the sample).
Results
Qualitative Review
Cocaine administration increased craving for cocaine in 24 of 38 studies included in this review (Table 1). In contrast, 14 of 38 studies showed that cocaine administration did not affect, or reduced, craving for cocaine.
Quantitative Review
The results from the quantitative review (Table 3) indicate that cocaine administration is associated with a significant increase in craving (Zma = 5.29, p ≤ .0001). Moreover, the magnitude of the effect was large (Cohen’s d: 1.45±1.04; Pearson’s r: 0.53±0.18). The correlation was considered to be robust (Fail-safe n = 86). Table 2 lists the effect sizes for each individual study.
Table 3.
Variables that moderate the intensity of cocaine-induced craving
Domain | Pearson’s r Mean (SD) | Cohen’s d Mean (SD) | Number of Comparisons | Meta-Analytic Z-Score | p-value | Fail-safe Nfs |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Overall Effect of Priming | 0.53 (0.18) | 1.45 (1.04) | 12 | 5.29 | <0.0001 | 86.41 |
Adjectives Used to Define Priming Effect | ||||||
Craving | 0.48 (0.16) | 1.14 (0.44) | 4 | 1.92 | NS | 2.84 |
Desire | 0.51 (0.28) | 1.72 (1.8) | 4 | 2.05 | <0.005 | 3.38 |
Want | 0.58 (0.13) | 1.51 (0.58) | 4 | 2.32 | <0.01 | 4.60 |
Route of Administration | ||||||
Smoked | 0.49 (0.01) | 1.10 (0.02) | 3 | 1.46 | NS | 0 |
IV | 0.54 (0.21) | 1.57 (1.20) | 9 | 4.84 | <0.0001 | 53.89 |
Dose | ||||||
<48 | 0.54 (0.17) | 1.37 (0.64) | 5 | 2.68 | <0.005 | 8.35 |
>49 | 0.49 (0.19) | 1.23 (0.67) | 7 | 3.43 | <0.001 | 20.44 |
Table 2.
Effect sizes of individual studies included in the quantitative review
First author | Year | Sample | Effect Size | Effect on Craving | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pearson’s r | Cohen’s d | ||||
Donny | 2004 | 8M/4F | 0.28 | 0.58 | 0 |
Fischman | 1990 | 6M/0F | 0.76 | 2.34 | + |
Jaffe | 1989 | 9M/0F | 0.55 | 1.32 | + |
Johnson | 2004 | 12M/6F | 0.26 | 0.54 | 0 |
Nann-Vernotica | 2001 | 9M/1F | 0.62 | 1.58 | + |
Preston | 1992 | 8M/0F | 0.39 | 0.85 | + |
Preston | 1993 | 8M/0F | 0.47 | 1.06 | + |
Sofuoglu | 2000a | 9M/3F | 0.49 | 1.12 | + |
Walsh | 2000 | 7M/1F | 0.91 | 4.39 | + |
Ward | 1997a | 7M/0F | 0.6 | 1.5 | + |
Ward | 1997b | 7M/0F | 0.48 | 1.09 | + |
Ward | 1998 | 8M/0F | 0.49 | 1.12 | + |
In addition, table 3 shows the impact of potential moderator variables. Responses elicited by the adjectives “desire” and “want” were correlated with significant priming effects whereas “craving” was not correlated with a significant priming effect. Moreover, cocaine administered intravenously was associated with a significant priming effect whereas cocaine administered via smoking was not. Furthermore, priming effects did not appear to be strongly dose related, as doses smaller than 48 mg versus those larger than 49 mg (the median split for all doses specified) produced similar effects.
Discussion
The qualitative review was equivocal with respect to the probability that priming effects occurred subsequent to cocaine administration (24/38 or 63% in the affirmative). In contrast, the quantitative review revealed that cocaine administration was significantly associated with priming effects. It must be emphasized that 83% of the studies included in the meta-analysis demonstrated that cocaine-induced craving exists whereas only 62% of the studies in the qualitative review supported this conclusion. A potential limitation of the meta-analytic method is that studies that do not include statistics cannot be included in the analysis. This can be a cause for concern since details of statistics are very rarely reported when the findings do not reach significance. The quantitative review revealed that methodologies may moderate the outcomes of study (e.g., use of adjectives such as “craving”, “desire”, or “want”), though the sample was not large enough to permit follow up analyses that would have offered comparisons within a class of moderates (i.e., comparing the relative magnitude of the effect for each adjective). It may be that other factors moderate the relationship between cocaine administration and subsequent craving for the drug. These include, but are not limited to, the effects of rate of administration and the effects of administering multiple doses of cocaine. For example, it would be important to know if differences in priming effects occurred when cocaine was smoked or injected (Jeffcoat, Perez-Reyes, Hill, Sadler, & Cook, 1989). From the small number of available studies it appears that smoked administration may produce smaller priming effects than IV administration, but this conclusion is tentative given that only three studies used this route of administration (Table 3).
Furthermore, in studies, where cocaine was administered repeatedly, it was difficult to infer the exact amount administered; therefore, those studies did not meet the inclusion criteria and were omitted from the meta-analysis (however, several of the studies that involved self-administration were able to be included in the meta-analytic review because a “sample session” of cocaine was administered where all participants received a fixed amount of cocaine). The null findings using “craving” and employing smoked cocaine may be due in part to the small number of studies (N=3) using these design features.
The effects of rate of administration may be inferred in a preliminary way by comparing results obtained by Jaffe and colleagues (1989) to those obtained by other authors. In that study, 40 mg of cocaine was administered IV over 1–2 seconds and this produced an average increase in craving of 2.9 on a 0–10 scale (rate of administration for each individual study is reported in Table 1). That was a rapid rate of administration compared to the other studies shown in Table 1, in which the rate of administration ranged from 30 seconds to 2 minutes. The observation that a rapid rate of administration was associated with larger increases in craving is consistent with preclinical studies showing that rapid administration is associated with greater behavioral effects (Samaha et al, 2004; Samaha & Robinson, 2005). In addition, it has been reported that that rapidly administered cocaine preferentially engages mesocorticolimbic circuits, which more readily induces psychomotor sensitization (Samaha & Robinson, 2005). It has also been found that a more rapid rate of onset may enhance a drug’s reinforcing effects, but a drug with a slow onset can still maintain self-administration (Lile, 2006). Also, it has been demonstrated that the “rate hypothesis” (the faster the drug reaches the brain, the greater its reinforcing effects) of psychoactive drug action occurs after IV drug administration (Nelson et al., 2006). However, in the current report, there were no significant differences between rate of administration and no significant differences between groups that utilized the smoked route vs. the intravenous route.
Placebo administration has been reported to produce cocaine-like subjective effects (Muntaner et al., 1989). It is likely that expectancy can alter responses to active cocaine administration, as well. For example, in the study by Fischman and colleagues (Fischman et al., 1990), participants reported very high baseline craving (95 of 100), which limited the extent to which subsequent cocaine administration could increase craving. That research group had many active ongoing cocaine administration studies, and this may have produced an atmosphere of expectation in their participants. Similarly, the magnitude of priming effects reported in studies of Walsh and Ward (Walsh et al., 2000; Ward et al., 1997) may have been influenced by expectancies. Participants in these studies were aware that they would subsequently receive multiple doses of cocaine, and this expectancy may have contributed to the robust priming effects observed. The contrast between the low baseline levels of craving in those studies (mean baseline craving < 5 on a scale of 100) remains unclear when compared to that specified in the earlier study by Fischman and colleagues (Fischman et al., 1990).
The available literature does not allow examination of several other factors that may affect priming effects. For example, the role played by contingent compared to non-contingent cocaine administration may be important (Leri & Stewart, 2002), but has not been systematically evaluated in clinical studies. A recent investigation of priming effects utilized self-administration using patient-controlled analgesia (PCA) pumps. This study allowed non-treatment seeking cocaine dependent individuals to self-administer cocaine during a two-hour session (Sughondhabirom et al., 2005) . The authors reported that Visual Analog Scale (VAS) assessments of “want cocaine” did not differ as a function of dose. However, the response rate on the PCA device appeared as a classical “inverted U” dose-response function. Maximal response rates were supported by 8 mg cocaine, with placebo and higher doses of cocaine eliciting fewer responses. Moreover, study participants often administered approximately 50% of the cocaine available to them during a given study session. Further research is needed to determine if higher doses of cocaine suppress cocaine responding, as observed in preclinical investigations, though these initial data raise questions as to the relationship between cocaine dose, craving, and drug-seeking behavior. For example, one particular study found that higher doses of cocaine suppress cocaine responding (# injections/session), though total cocaine intake remains similar (mg/kg/session) (Edwards et al, 2006).
Other factors that could moderate priming effects include genetics, personal history of cocaine or polysubstance use, and comorbid psychiatric illnesses. For example, we recently reported that cocaine-induced craving was moderated by the level of reported symptoms of apathy, a distinct neuropsychiatric syndrome (Newton, Kalechstein, De La Garza, Cutting, & Ling, 2005). Recently, we have also noted that craving produced by non-contingent administration of cocaine is positively correlated with the number of days in which the drug was used in the month prior to the study (Newton and De La Garza, unpublished findings).
In interpreting these outcomes, several limitations should be noted. While most of the studies reported the years of cocaine use, most studies did not report the amount used nor the days used in the past month of previous cocaine exposure participants had outside of the laboratory, and this may affect the magnitude of cocaine priming effects observed in the laboratory. Paradoxically, repeated exposure to cocaine outside of the relevant laboratory context might reduce priming effects observed in the laboratory, a phenomenon referred to as the “Unconditioned Stimulus-pre-exposure effect” (Randich & LoLordo, 1979; Saladin, 1986). As specified previously, the simple visual analogue scales used to assess craving are unlikely to fully reflect the multifaceted and complex nature of craving in cocaine-addicted patients (Robbins, Ehrman, Childress, & O’Brien, 1997; Tiffany & Drobes, 1991; Tiffany, Singleton, Haertzen, & Henningfield, 1993). As such, the studies surveyed provide a limited assessment of factors impacting craving. Factors affecting craving for drugs other than cocaine (e.g. alcohol, nicotine, methamphetamine) may differ from those identified above. Finally, elucidation of the mechanisms by which drug administration alters both subjective mood states and behavior requires additional research.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health (DA-50038, DA-00388, DA-07272, DA-08804 and RR-00865).
Footnotes
An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Society for Neuroscience, November 2001, San Diego CA.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Literature Cited
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral science. 2. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Collins ED, Ward AS, McDowell DM, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. The effects of memantine on the subjective, reinforcing and cardiovascular effects of cocaine in humans. Behav Pharmacol. 1998;9(7):587–598. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199811000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De La Garza R, 2nd, Newton TF, Kalechstein AD. Risperidone diminishes cocaine-induced craving. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;178(2–3):347–350. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2010-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Stewart J. Reinstatement of cocaine-reinforced responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1981;75(2):134–143. doi: 10.1007/BF00432175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H. Priming effects with drugs and other reinforcers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1996;4(1):5–10. [Google Scholar]
- Donny EC, Bigelow GE, Walsh SL. Choosing to take cocaine in the human laboratory: Effects of cocaine dose, inter-choice interval, and magnitude of alternative reinforcement. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;69(3):289–301. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00327-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donny EC, Bigelow GE, Walsh SL. Assessing the initiation of cocaine self-administration in humans during abstinence: Effects of dose, alternative reinforcement, and priming. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;172(3):316–323. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1655-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donny EC, Bigelow GE, Walsh SL. Comparing the physiological and subjective effects of self-administered vs yoked cocaine in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;186(4):544–552. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0312-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards S, Whisler KN, Fuller DC, Orsulak PJ, Self DW. Addiction-related alterations in d(1) and d(2) dopamine receptor behavioral responses following chronic cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006 doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischman MW, Foltin RW, Nestadt G, Pearlson GD. Effects of desipramine maintenance on cocaine self-administration by humans. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1990;253(2):760–770. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foltin RW, Haney M. Conditioned effects of environmental stimuli paired with smoked cocaine in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2000;149(1):24–33. doi: 10.1007/s002139900340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foltin RW, Haney M. Intranasal cocaine in humans: Acute tolerance, cardiovascular and subjective effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;78(1):93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foltin RW, Ward AS, Collins ED, Haney M, Hart CL, Fischman MW. The effects of venlafaxine on the subjective, reinforcing, and cardiovascular effects of cocaine in opioid-dependent and non-opioid-dependent humans. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2003;11(2):123–130. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.11.2.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haney M, Collins ED, Ward AS, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Effect of a selective dopamine d1 agonist (abt-431) on smoked cocaine self-administration in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;143(1):102–110. doi: 10.1007/s002130050925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haney M, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Effects of pergolide on intravenous cocaine self-administration in men and women. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1998;137(1):15–24. doi: 10.1007/s002130050588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haney M, Hart C, Collins ED, Foltin RW. Smoked cocaine discrimination in humans: Effects of gabapentin. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;80(1):53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haney M, Hart CL, Foltin RW. Effects of baclofen on cocaine self-administration: Opioid- and nonopioid-dependent volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006 doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haney M, Ward AS, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Effects of ecopipam, a selective dopamine d1 antagonist, on smoked cocaine self-administration by humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;155(4):330–337. doi: 10.1007/s002130100725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart CL, Ward AS, Collins ED, Haney M, Foltin RW. Gabapentin maintenance decreases smoked cocaine-related subjective effects, but not self-administration by humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2004;73(3):279–287. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2003.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houtsmuller EJ, Notes LD, Newton T, van Sluis N, Chiang N, Elkashef A, et al. Transdermal selegiline and intravenous cocaine: Safety and interactions. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;172(1):31–40. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1616-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaffe JH, Cascella NG, Kumor KM, Sherer MA. Cocaine-induced cocaine craving. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989;97(1):59–64. doi: 10.1007/BF00443414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeffcoat AR, Perez-Reyes M, Hill JM, Sadler BM, Cook CE. Cocaine disposition in humans after intravenous injection, nasal insufflation (snorting), or smoking. Drug Metab Dispos. 1989;17(2):153–159. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johanson CE, Lundahl LH, Lockhart N, Schubiner H. Intravenous cocaine discrimination in humans. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2006;14(2):99–108. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.2.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BA, Roache JD, Ait-Daoud N, Wells LT, Mauldin JB. Effects of isradipine on cocaine-induced subjective mood. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2004;24(2):180–191. doi: 10.1097/01.jcp.0000115662.45074.c3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosten T, Gawin FH, Silverman DG, Fleming J, Compton M, Jatlow P, et al. Intravenous cocaine challenges during desipramine maintenance. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1992;7(3):169–176. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kouri EM, Lundahl LH, Borden KN, McNeil JF, Lukas SE. Effects of oral contraceptives on acute cocaine response in female volunteers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;74(1):173–180. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00992-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leri F, Stewart J. The consequences of different “lapses” on relapse to heroin seeking in rats. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2002;10(4):339–349. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.10.4.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leyton M, Casey KF, Delaney JS, Kolivakis T, Benkelfat C. Cocaine craving, euphoria, and self-administration: A preliminary study of the effect of catecholamine precursor depletion. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119(6):1619–1627. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lile JA. Pharmacological determinants of the reinforcing effects of psychostimulants: Relation to agonist substitution treatment. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2006;14(1):20–33. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muntaner C, Cascella NG, Kumor KM, Nagoshi C, Herning R, Jaffe J. Placebo responses to cocaine administration in humans: Effects of prior administrations and verbal instructions. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989;99(2):282–286. doi: 10.1007/BF00442823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nann-Vernotica E, Donny EC, Bigelow GE, Walsh SL. Repeated administration of the d1/5 antagonist ecopipam fails to attenuate the subjective effects of cocaine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;155(4):338–347. doi: 10.1007/s002130100724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson RA, Boyd SJ, Ziegelstein RC, Herning R, Cadet JL, Henningfield JE, et al. Effect of rate of administration on subjective and physiological effects of intravenous cocaine in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006;82(1):19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newton TF, Kalechstein AD, De La Garza R, 2nd, Cutting DJ, Ling W. Apathy predicts hedonic but not craving response to cocaine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;82(1):236–240. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedhazur EJ, Schmelkin LP. Measurement, design, and analysis: An integrated approach. student. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Preston KL, Sullivan JT, Berger P, Bigelow GE. Effects of cocaine alone and in combination with mazindol in human cocaine abusers. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1993;267(1):296–307. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preston KL, Sullivan JT, Strain EC, Bigelow GE. Effects of cocaine alone and in combination with bromocriptine in human cocaine abusers. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1992;262(1):279–291. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Randich A, LoLordo VM. Associative and nonassociative theories of the ucs preexposure phenomenon: Implications for pavlovian conditioning. Psychol Bull. 1979;86(3):523–548. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roache JD, Johnson BA, Ait-Daoud N, Mauldin JB, Thornton JE, Wells LT, et al. Effects of repeated-dose isradipine on the abuse liability of cocaine. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2005;13(4):319–326. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.4.319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins SJ, Ehrman RN, Childress AR, O’Brien CP. Relationships among physiological and self-report responses produced by cocaine-related cues. Addict Behav. 1997;22(2):157–167. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(96)00007-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romach MK, Glue P, Kampman K, Kaplan HL, Somer GR, Poole S, et al. Attenuation of the euphoric effects of cocaine by the dopamine d1/d5 antagonist ecopipam (sch 39166) Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(12):1101–1106. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Rev. Newbury Park: Sage publications; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Rush CR, Baker RW, Wright K. Acute physiological and behavioral effects of oral cocaine in humans: A dose-response analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999;55(1–2):1–12. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00164-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saladin ME. Us preexposures retard excitatory and facilitate inhibitory conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Animal Learning & Behavior. 1986;14(2):121–132. [Google Scholar]
- Samaha AN, Mallet N, Ferguson SM, Gonon F, Robinson TE. The rate of cocaine administration alters gene regulation and behavioral plasticity: Implications for addiction. J Neurosci. 2004;24(28):6362–6370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1205-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samaha AN, Robinson TE. Why does the rapid delivery of drugs to the brain promote addiction? Trends Pharmacol Sic. 2005;26(2):82–87. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2004.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schenk S, Partridge B. Cocaine-seeking produced by experimenter-administered drug injections: Dose-effect relationships in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;147(3):285–290. doi: 10.1007/s002130051169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sofuoglu M, Brown S, Babb DA, Pentel PR, Hatsukami DK. Carvedilol affects the physiological and behavioral response to smoked cocaine in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2000a;60(1):69–76. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00143-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sofuoglu M, Brown S, Babb DA, Pentel PR, Hatsukami DK. Effects of labetalol treatment on the physiological and subjective response to smoked cocaine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000b;65(2):255–259. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00201-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sofuoglu M, Dudish-Poulsen S, Brown SB, Hatsukami DK. Association of cocaine withdrawal symptoms with more severe dependence and enhanced subjective response to cocaine. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;69(3):273–282. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00328-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sofuoglu M, Poling J, Mitchell E, Kosten TR. Tiagabine affects the subjective responses to cocaine in humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;82(3):569–573. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sofuoglu M, Pentel PR, Bliss RL, Goldman AI, Hatsukami DK. Effects of phenytoin on cocaine self-administration in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999;53(3):273–275. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00140-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sughondhabirom A, Jain D, Gueorguieva R, Coric V, Berman R, Lynch WJ, et al. A paradigm to investigate the self-regulation of cocaine administration in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;180(3):436–446. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2192-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiffany ST, Drobes DJ. The development and initial validation of a questionnaire on smoking urges. Br J Addict. 1991;86(11):1467–1476. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01732.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiffany ST, Singleton E, Haertzen CA, Henningfield JE. The development of a cocaine craving questionnaire. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1993;34(1):19–28. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(93)90042-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh SL, Haberny KA, Bigelow GE. Modulation of intravenous cocaine effects by chronic oral cocaine in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2000;150(4):361–373. doi: 10.1007/s002130000439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward AS, Collins ED, Haney M, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Ketoconazole attenuates the cortisol response but not the subjective effects of smoked cocaine in humans. Behav Pharmacol. 1998;9(7):577–586. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199811000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward AS, Haney M, Fischman MW, Foltin RW. Binge cocaine self-administration by humans: Smoked cocaine. Behav Pharmacol. 1997b;8(8):736–744. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199712000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward AS, Haney M, Fischman MW, Foltin RW. Binge cocaine self-administration in humans: Intravenous cocaine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997a;132(4):375–381. doi: 10.1007/s002130050358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winhusen T, Somoza E, Singal BM, Harrer J, Apparaju S, Mezinskis J, et al. Methylphenidate and cocaine: A placebo-controlled drug interaction study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006 doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.06.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissenborn R, Yackey M, Koob GF, Weiss F. Measures of cocaine-seeking behavior using a multiple schedule of food and drug self-administration in rats. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1995;38(3):237–246. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(95)01107-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]