Abstract
Previous studies have demonstrated that attempts to suppress thoughts about using substances may actually lead to increases in substance use. Vipassana, a mindfulness meditation practice, emphasizes acceptance, rather than suppression, of unwanted thoughts. Bowen and colleagues (2006) studied the effects of a Vipassana course on substance use and in an incarcerated population, showing significant reductions in substance use among the Vipassana group as compared to a treatment as usual control condition (Bowen, Witkiewitz, Dillworth, Chawla, Simpson, Ostafin, et al., 2006). The current study further examines the mediating effects of thought suppression in the relationship between participation in the course and subsequent alcohol use in an incarcerated population. Those who participated in the course reported significant decreases in avoidance of thoughts when compared to controls. The decrease in avoidance partially mediated effects of the course on post-release alcohol use and consequences.
Keywords: Vipassana, meditation, mindfulness, thought suppression, alcohol, substance use, incarcerated
1. Introduction
Several studies suggest thought suppression often results in an increase, rather than decrease, in unwanted thoughts (Wegner, 1997; Wegner, Schneider, Carter & White, 1987). Research in addictive behaviors has found that thought suppression impedes attempts to quit smoking (Toll, Sobell, Wagner & Sobell, 2001; Salkovskis & Reynolds, 1994; Haaga & Allsion, 1994), and that heavy social drinkers given instructions to suppress alcohol-related thoughts and urges, when compared to controls, demonstrated stronger expectancies after alcohol cue exposure (Palfai, Monti, Colby & Rosenhow, 1997).
Contrary to thought suppression, mindfulness-based strategies teach acceptance, non-judgment and non-reaction to thoughts, feelings and sensations. Several studies have incorporated meditation and mindfulness techniques in treatment of substance use, with promising results (Linehan, Schmidt, Dimeff, Craft, Kanter & Comtois, 1999; Gifford, Kohlenberg, Hayes, Antonuccio, Piasecki, Rasmussen-Hall, et al., 2004). However, little is know about the mechanisms by which mindfulness leads to changes in substance use.
A study of a 10-day intensive Vipassana mindfulness meditation course held in a minimum security jail in Seattle (Bowen, Witkiewitz, Dillworth, Chawla, Simpson, Ostafin, et al., 2006) found that course participants, when compared to a treatment as usual control (TAU) group, showed significant decreases in substance use three months following release from jail (Bowen et al., 2006). The current study is a secondary data analysis examining thought suppression as a mediator of the relationship between meditation and post-release alcohol use.
2. Methods
2.1 Participants
The 173 study participants (57 meditation course, 116 TAU) in the current analyses completed both baseline and post-course assessments. Participants ranged in age from 19 to 58 (M=37.4, SD= 8.6) and were 79% male. Sixty one percent identified as Caucasian, 13% African American, 8% Latino/a, 8% American Indian, and 1–2% as either Pacific Islander, Alaskan Native, Asian American or other. A full description of the sample can be found in Bowen, et al. (2006).
2.2 Procedure
Participants practiced sitting meditation for approximately 8 to 10 hours daily throughout the 10-day course. Meditation instructions focused on observation of breath, body sensations, and observation and acceptance of internal experiences, rather than reaction to or avoidance of them. Aside from questions to the instructor, participants refrained from speaking, reading, or writing.
2.3 Measures
All participants were given a battery of self-report questionnaires prior to the start of the course (baseline assessment), a post-course assessment immediately following the course, and follow-up assessments three and six months after release from jail. Thought Suppression was measured using the White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI, Wegner & Zanakos, 1994). Quantity and frequency of peak weeks of alcohol use was assessed with the Daily Drinking Questionnaire (DDQ; Collins, Parks, & Marlatt, 1985). Alcohol-related negative consequences were measured using the Short Inventory of Problems (SIP; Miller, Tonigan & Longabaugh, 1995). Means and standard deviations for all measures are listed in Table 1.
Table 1.
TAU | VP | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | Follow-up | Baseline | Follow-up | |
WBSI: Avoidance | 3.28 (.99) | 3.20 (1.03) | 3.54 (.91) | 3.07 (.87) |
WBSI: Intrusion | 3.24 (1.12) | 3.17 (1.07) | 3.31 (1.03) | 3.14 (.96) |
Drinks* | 43.98 (55.61) | 27.77 (46.37) | 64.83 (73.01) | 8.38 (13.37) |
SIP** | 17.95 (15.50) | .13.94 (15.33) | 20.98 (14.86) | 8.46 (12.25) |
Total number of drinks per peak week
Scores from 19 to 22 are considered mid-level severity
3. Results
Similar to previous findings (Rassin, 2003), a Principal Components Analysis of the WBSI provided support for a two-factor model from which two subscales were created: Thought Avoidance (e.g., “I always try to put problems out of mind,” α = .83) and Intrusive Thoughts (e.g., “I have thoughts that I cannot stop,” α = .90). Change scores were calculated such that a positive score on the difference between pre- and post-course indicated decreased (improved) thought suppression and a negative score indicated increased (worse) thought suppression.
Multiple regression analyses of complete case data from the three-month follow-up (n=81) tested whether changes in the thought suppression subscales following the Vipassana course vs. TAU mediated the relationship between pre-treatment drinking and drinking at three-month follow-up. Multiple regression analyses were conducted (Baron and Kenny, 1986) for each of the drinking outcome variables (total drinks per peak week and average SIP scores). First, post-treatment drinking was regressed on baseline drinking and treatment condition. Second, change in WBSI subscale scores was regressed on treatment condition. Third, post-treatment drinking was regressed on baseline drinking, change in WBSI subscale scores, and treatment condition.
For the Avoidance subscale, partial mediation was supported. As shown in Table 2, all three of Baron and Kenny’s (1986) criteria for partial mediation were supported and the product of coefficients method outlined by MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets (2002) provided further evidence of mediation (Drinks: z′ = 1.94, p < .05; SIP: z′ = 1.74, p < .05). For the Intrusion subscale, the first two analyses, both necessary for mediation, resulted in non-significant regression weights [WBSI-Intrusion on treatment condition (β=−.10, p = .38) and drinking outcomes on WBSI-Intrusion (drinks: β=.20, p = .07; SIP: β=.17, p = .07)].
Table 2.
Model | B | σB | β | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
(a) Change in WBSI-avoidance regressed on treatment condition | .66 | .24 | −.287 | .007 |
(b) 3-month drinking regressed on change in WBSI | 10.69 | 3.92 | .29 | .008 |
(c) 3-month drinking regressed on treatment condition | −.26 | .02 | ||
(c′) 3-month drinking regressed on change in WBSI (mediator) and treatment condition | .20 | .07 | ||
(a) Change in WBSI-avoidance regressed on treatment condition | .66 | .24 | −.287 | .007 |
| ||||
(b) 3-month SIP regressed on change in WBSI | .19 | .09 | .20 | .03 |
| ||||
(c) 3-month SIP regressed on treatment condition | −.21 | .02 | ||
| ||||
(c′) 3-month SIP regressed on change in WBSI (mediator) and treatment condition | −.16 | .08 |
4. Discussion
Individuals who participated in the 10-day Vipassana meditation course reported greater decreases in attempts to avoid unwanted thoughts than individuals who did not take the course. Change in levels of avoidance partially mediated the relationship between Vipassana course participation and alcohol use and consequences three months following release from jail.
These results provide support for the hypothesis that thought avoidance may be an important component in the relationship between meditation and alcohol use (Witkiewitz, Marlatt & Walker, 2005). Vipassana course participants did not report a significant decrease in intrusive thoughts; thus frequency of unwanted thoughts may not be as important as the manner in which an individual copes with those thoughts. Future studies and treatments may benefit from addressing attempts to avoid unwanted negative thoughts, as well as exploring what other factors mediate this relationship.
While this study shows promising results, there are several limitations, the primary of which is the absence of random assignment to condition. Although there were no significant differences between course participants and TAU controls at baseline, it is important to replicate these findings in a randomized trial. Additionally, this sample represents a unique and specific population of incarcerated adults who were not necessarily seeking treatment. Future investigations would benefit from assessing mindfulness-based treatments in a treatment-seeking community sample. Moreover, the high attrition rate, (approximately 47% at 3 months) although common in this population, (Farrington, Petrosino & Welsh, 2001) limits the power to detect differences. Although there were no significant differences between completers and non-completers greater retention would add strength to the study.
Despite the limitations, the current study provides preliminary findings that might prove useful in furthering our understanding of effects of Vipassana meditation on alcohol use and negative consequences. While future research examining the role of attempts to control unwanted thoughts is clearly warranted, the current findings present a starting place for further attempts to better our understanding of the power and potential of mindfulness techniques.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator mediator distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowen S, Witkiewitz K, Dillworth TM, Chawla N, Simpson TL, Ostafin BD, et al. Mindfulness Meditation and Substance Use in an Incarcerated Population. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2006 doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.3.343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins RL, Parks GA, Marlatt GA. Social Determinants of Alcohol Consumption: The Effects of Social Interaction and Model Status on the Self-Administration of Alcohol. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1985;53:189–200. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.53.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrington DP, Petrosino A, Welsh BC. Systematic reviews and cost-benefit analyses of correctional interventions. The Prison Journal. 2001;81:339–359. [Google Scholar]
- Gifford EV, Kohlenberg BS, Hayes SC, Antonuccio DO, Piasecki MM, Rasmussen-Hall ML, et al. Acceptance-Based Treatment for Smoking Cessation. Behavior Therapy. 2004;35:689–705. [Google Scholar]
- Haaga DAF, Allison ML. Thought suppression and smoking relapse: a secondary analysis of Haaga 1989. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1994;33:327 – 331. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1994.tb01128.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linehan MM, Schmidt H, Dimeff LA, Craft JC, Kanter J, Comtois KA. Dialectical behavior therapy for patients with borderline personality disorder and drug-dependence. American Journal on Addictions. 1999;8(4):279–292. doi: 10.1080/105504999305686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR, Tonigan JS, Longabaugh R. In: The Drinker Inventory of Consequences (DrInC), Project MATCH Monograph Series. Mattson Margaret E., editor. Vol. 4. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Palfai TP, Monti PM, Colby SM, Rohsenow DJ. Effects of suppressing the urge to drink on the accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies. Behavior Research and Therapy. 1997;35(1):59–65. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(96)00079-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rassin E. The White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI) focuses on failing suppression attempts. European Journal of Personality. 2003;17:285–298. [Google Scholar]
- Salkovskis PM, Reynolds M. Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behavior Research and Therapy. 1994;32:193–201. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90112-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toll BA, Sobell MB, Wagner EF, Sobell LC. The relationship between thought suppression and smoking cessation. Addictive Behaviors. 2001;26:509–515. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(00)00140-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wegner DM. When the antidote is the poison: Ironic mental control processes. Psychological Science. 1997;8:148–150. [Google Scholar]
- Wegner DM, Schneider DJ, Carter S, White T. Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;53:5–13. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.53.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wegner DM, Zanakos S. Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality. 1994;62:615–640. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00311.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witkiewitz K, Marlatt GA, Walker DD. Mindfulness-based relapse prevention for alcohol and substance use disorders. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2005;19:221–228. [Google Scholar]