Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addict Behav. 2007 Mar 7;32(10):2351–2357. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.02.004

Increases in Impulsivity Following Smoking Abstinence are Related to Baseline Nicotine Intake and Boredom Susceptibility

Jasmine Pettiford 1, Rachel V Kozink 1, Avery M Lutz 1, Scott H Kollins 1, Jed E Rose 1, F Joseph McClernon 1,*
PMCID: PMC2040068  NIHMSID: NIHMS29380  PMID: 17399907

Abstract

Trait impulsivity and response inhibition have been shown to be related to smoking behavior. One measure of response inhibition—antisaccade performance, or the ability to inhibit looking at a novel stimulus—has been shown to be worsened by smoking abstinence, improved by nicotine administration and predictive of smoking cessation outcomes. However, relations between antisaccade performance and measures of trait impulsivity have not been extensively evaluated in smokers. In the present study, twelve dependent smokers (n = 12) completed an eye tracking task following smoking as usual and overnight abstinence; and they completed baseline measures of trait impulsivity, smoking history and provided biological samples. As expected, overnight abstinence significantly increased antisaccade errors (p < .002) while having no effect on prosaccade performance. Abstinence-induced increases in antisaccade errors were positively correlated with baseline plasma cotinine and Sensation Seeking Scale Boredom Susceptibility, and negatively correlated with IQ. These results suggest that smoking abstinence significantly increases errors of response inhibition and that the magnitude of this increase is related to trait impulsivity and nicotine intake variables.

Keywords: smoking, impulsivity, antisaccade, sensation seeking, IQ, nicotine

INTRODCUTION

Impulsivity, which has been conceptualized as a dysregulation of inhibitory processes (Evenden, 1999), has been shown to play a role in smoking (Mitchell, 2004) and smoking relapse (Doran, Spring, McChargue, Pergadia, & Richmond, 2004; Powell, Pickering, Dawkins, West, & Powell, 2004). For instance, Doran et al. (2004) found that among participants asked to quit smoking for 48 hrs, those who were higher in trait impulsivity relapsed more quickly than other participants. This relation was not mediated by other potentially important variables (e.g., craving).

One performance based measure of inhibitory processes—the antisaccade task—has been used to study relations between smoking and impulsivity. The antisaccade task measures response inhibition by requiring subjects to inhibit looking at a novel stimulus (Pierrot-Deseilligny, Muri, Ploner, Gaymard, & Rivaud-Pechoux, 2003). Performance on this task has been shown to reliably differentiate healthy controls from individuals who are higher in trait impulsivity, such as individuals with ADHD (Feifel, Farber, Clementz, Perry, & Anllo-Vento, 2004; Munoz, Armstrong, Hampton, & Moore, 2003) and drug dependence (Rosse, McCarthy, Alim, & Deutsch, 1994). Overnight abstinence from smoking has been shown to result in worse antisaccade performance (Powell, Dawkins, & Davis, 2002). Moreover, the administration of 4 or 6 mg nicotine gum was shown to significantly decrease antisaccade error rates compared to placebo gum (Larrison-Faucher, Matorin, & Sereno, 2004). Additionally, Powell et al (2004) found that the degree to which nicotine (4 mg lozenge) improved antisaccade performance in overnight deprived smokers was correlated with self-reported nicotine dependence and predicted relapse during the first week after quitting (Powell, Pickering, Dawkins, West, & Powell, 2004).

Despite studies showing relations between measures of trait impulsivity and smoking, and antisaccade performance and smoking; relations between trait impulsivity and antisaccade performance among smokers has not been thoroughly evaluated. Therefore, the goals of the present study were to address this question by evaluating relations between abstinence-induced changes in antisaccade performance and measures of smoking behavior and trait impulsivity.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were 14 adult smokers recruited from the community. These participants were part of a larger study examining individual differences in tobacco withdrawal for which antisaccade data was available. Potential participants were required to smoke ≥ 15 cigarettes per day and have an afternoon CO level ≥ 15 ppm. Two participants' data were not analyzed due to missing or incomplete data. The study was approved by the Duke University Institutional Review Board.

Procedure

Following a baseline session, participants completed two 2.5 h experimental sessions—each scheduled to start between 7 and 9 a.m. Prior to one of the sessions, participants were required to maintain abstinence starting at 10 p.m. the previous night (abstinent session). Prior to the other session, participants could smoke freely and they were also allowed two smoke breaks during the session (satiated session). Otherwise, the experimental sessions were identical. The order of sessions (abstinent first vs. satiated first) was randomly assigned.

Smoking and Personality Measures

Expired air carbon monoxide (CO)

Verification of smoking status and overnight abstinence was obtained using expired air CO concentrations (BreathCo, Vitalograph, Lenexa, Kan. USA). The criteria for CO following overnight abstinence was individually determined using a previously validated formula (Rose & Behm, 2004) based on each smoker's baseline afternoon CO level.

Baseline cotinine

Plasma cotinine concentrations were determined from samples taken at screening. Cotinine concentrations were assayed using gas chromatography (Jacob, Wilson, & Benowitz, 1981).

Smoking history

Smoking history was assessed using a 14-item questionnaire.

Nicotine dependence

The 6-item Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) was administered (Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker, & Fagerstrom, 1991). FTND scores ranged from 0 to 10.

Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (KBIT)

General cognitive functioning was assessed using the KBIT (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1990) which yields estimates of verbal, nonverbal and overall functioning.

Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS)

The Barratt Impulsivity Scale version 11 (BIS-11; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995) is a 30-item measure of three domains of trait-impulsivity: attentional, motor and non-planning impulsiveness. The reliability of the scale in clinical and community samples is adequate (Cronbach's α = .79 to .83; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995).

Sensation Seeking Scale Form V (SSS-V)

The SSS-V (Zuckerman, 1993) contains 40 forced-choice items yielding a Total Score and four subscales: Thrill and Adventure Seeking (TAS), Experience Seeking (ES), Disinhibition (DIS), and Boredom Susceptibility (BS).

The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ)

The TPQ (Cloninger, 1986) is a 100-item true-false questionnaire. The self-report personality inventory measures three major personality dimensions: Novelty Seeking, Harm Avoidance, and Reward Dependence.

Eye Tracking Measures

Eye Tracking

Eye movements were measured using an ASL EYE-TRAC 6000 eye-tracking system (Applied Science Laboratories, MA). A near-infrared light was used to illuminate the pupil and corneal reflection, from which pupil diameter and line of gaze was extracted (sampling rate = 60 Hz). Visual stimuli were displayed on a computer screen 77 cm from the participants' eyes. At the beginning of each trial, a central fixation circle that was either gray or white was presented on a black background for a random duration between 1250 and 1750 ms. The fixation circle disappeared 200 ms prior to the presentation of a white target asterisk. The target was displayed for 1750 ms, 6.8° from center. If the fixation circle was gray, participants were instructed to look in the opposite direction of the target (antisaccade). If the circle was white, they were instructed to look towards the target (prosaccade). Each task consisted of 110 trials—30 prosaccade and 80 antisaccade—presented in random order.

Analysis

Saccades were classified as artifactual, correct, or incorrect. Trials were marked artifactual if the direction of the saccade initiation could not be distinguished due to excessive noise, loss of pupil-corneal reflection, excessive head movement. Both pro- and anti-saccades were classified as incorrect if all of the following conditions were met: 1) saccade initiation was in the wrong direction, 2) the incorrect saccade generated was more than 2.3° (one-third) in the wrong direction, and 3) the incorrect saccade was not corrected by an eye movement in the correct direction within 150 ms. All other saccades were classified as correct.

RESULTS

Sample Characteristics

The sample was 58.3% female (n = 7). Reported racial/ethnic group membership was 83.3% Caucasian (n = 10), 8.3% African American (n = 1), and 8.3% Other/Unspecified (n = 1). Mean age was 32.08 years (SD = 6.10). Two participants (16.6%) completed high school or equivalent (n = 2), 8 had some college/technical education (n = 66.6%), and 2 were college graduates/post-grads (16.6%). Mean FTND score was 4.40 (SD = 1.4).

Smoking behavior

Participants smoked a mean of 18.75 (SD = 4.33) cigarettes per day, and reported smoking a mean of 13.17 (SD = 6.62) years. Six (n = 6; 50.0%) reported smoking menthol cigarettes. Mean cotinine level at screening was 187.57 ng/mL (SD = 66.61). Pre-session CO levels were indicative of satiety and overnight abstinence (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Carbon monoxide levels and percent incorrect on prosaccade and antisaccade trials in the satiated and abstinent conditions.

Satiated Abstinent

Mean SD Mean SD
CO (ppm) 28.25 12.64 9.58 5.66
Prosaccade 2.96 2.85 2.12 1.66
Antisaccade 19.24 10.61 27.50 12.04

Task performance

Participants exhibited a significantly higher percentage of antisaccade errors on the abstinent day, F(1,11) = 16.20, p < .002, Table 1. In contrast, prosaccade performance was unaffected (p > .05). Abstinence did not affect percentage of artifactual trials (p's > .05).

Correlates of antisaccade performance

A change score reflecting the effects of abstinence on antisaccade performance was calculated (% incorrect on abstinent day - % incorrect on satiated day). As can be seen in Table 2, this change score was significantly positively correlated with baseline plasma cotinine and SSS Boredom Susceptibility and negatively correlated with IQ. Of these three variables, only IQ and SSS Boredom Susceptibility were correlated with each other (r = −.705, p = .01).

Table 2.

Correlations between IQ, smoking behavior and personality variables with change in % antisaccade errors in the Abstinent – Satiated conditions.

Abstinent –
Satiated
% Antisaccade
Errors
IQ
 KBIT Score −.630*
Smoking Behavior
 Cigarettes per day −.037
 Baseline expired CO −.081
 Baseline plasma cotinine  .656*
 FTND  .099
Personality Measures
 BIS
  Motivational Impulsiveness −.316
  Non Planning Impulsiveness −.221
  Attentional Impulsiveness  .047
  Total −.225
 SSS
  Total Sensation Seeking  .310
  Thrill And Adventure Seeking  .272
  Experience Seeking −.265
  Disinhibition  .118
  Boredom Susceptibility  .583*
 TPQ
  Reward Dependence −.101
  Novelty Seeking −.352
  Harm Avoidance −.230
*

p < 0.05 (2-tailed)

All n's = 12 except FTND (n = 10)

DISCUSSION

The results of the present study show that smoking abstinence results in significantly increased impulsive responses as measured by the antisaccade task, while not affecting prosaccade performance. Anti-saccade change scores were significantly correlated with higher baseline plasma cotinine levels, Boredom Susceptibility scores from the SSS, and lower IQ estimates.

Consistent with earlier research (Powell, Dawkins, & Davis, 2002), our results show that smoking abstinence increases response inhibition errors among smokers—a finding with important implications for understanding smoking withdrawal. Smoking has been viewed as a behavior that becomes highly automated over time (Field, Mogg, & Bradley, 2006), and that can be triggered by any number of both internal (e.g., withdrawal; stress) and external triggers (e.g., conditioned smoking cues)(Beckham et al., 2005; Krukowski, Solomon, & Naud, 2005; Shiffman et al., 2002). From this viewpoint, maintaining abstinence would require the inhibition of a prepotent reponse (i.e. smoking) in the face of cues that previously automatically led to smoking behavior. The findings of the present study, along with others (e.g., Powell, Pickering, Dawkins, West, & Powell, 2004), suggest that smoking abstinence results in poorer response inhibition that likely exacerbates attempts to remain abstinent.

We also observed a significant positive relation between abstinence-induced decrements in response inhibition and baseline plasma cotinine levels, an index of nicotine intake. This suggests that smokers who administer higher levels of nicotine experience greater disturbances of response inhibition during abstinence. This finding may account for previous observations that smokers higher in nicotine dependence exhibit greater improvement in antisaccade performance by nicotine following abstinence (Powell, Pickering, Dawkins, West, & Powell, 2004).

Lower IQ and greater Boredom Susceptibility were also correlated with worsening of response inhibition by overnight abstinence. Individuals who are high in Boredom Susceptibility and have lower levels of intellectual functioning may require higher levels of environmental stimulation to achieve the same levels of arousal as individuals who do not share these traits. As such, smoking may serve to increase arousal in these individuals and smoking abstinence may therefore cause greater disruptions in this process. Very little work has been done examining the relationships among Boredom Susceptibility, IQ, and response inhibition, so interpretation of this interesting finding will require additional research. In any case, given the high correlation between IQ and Boredom Susceptibility, these are likely to be measuring a comparable underlying construct.

The present study provides additional evidence of the effects of smoking abstinence vs. smoking satiety and relates abstinence-induced worsening of response inhibition to important individual difference variables. Future studies will examine the effects of nicotine replacement and treatments designed specifically to reduce abstinence-induced disinhibition on antisaccade performance and smoking cessation outcomes.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Mr. Jim Liu for his development of a custom MATLAB graphical user interface for scoring the eye-tracking data. This research was funded by an unrestricted grant to study adult smoking cessation from Philip Morris USA, Inc. (JER) and by NIDA grant K23DA017261 (FJM).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Beckham JC, Feldman ME, Vrana SR, Mozley SL, Erkanli A, Clancy CP, et al. Immediate antecedents of cigarette smoking in smokers with and without posttraumatic stress disorder: a preliminary study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2005;13(3):219–228. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.3.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Cloninger CR. A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatr Dev. 1986;4(3):167–226. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Doran N, Spring B, McChargue D, Pergadia M, Richmond M. Impulsivity and smoking relapse. Nicotine Tob Res. 2004;6(4):641–647. doi: 10.1080/14622200410001727939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Feifel D, Farber RH, Clementz BA, Perry W, Anllo-Vento L. Inhibitory deficits in ocular motor behavior in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56(5):333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Field M, Mogg K, Bradley BP. Automaticity of smoking behaviour: the relationship between dual-task performance, daily cigarette intake and subjective nicotine effects. J Psychopharmacol. 2006 doi: 10.1177/0269881106063997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO. The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict. 1991;86(9):1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Jacob P, 3rd, Wilson M, Benowitz NL. Improved gas chromatographic method for the determination of nicotine and cotinine in biologic fluids. J Chromatogr. 1981;222(1):61–70. doi: 10.1016/s0378-4347(00)81033-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. American Guidance Services, Inc.; Circle Pines, MN: 1990. [Google Scholar]
  9. Krukowski RA, Solomon LJ, Naud S. Triggers of heavier and lighter cigarette smoking in college students. J Behav Med. 2005;28(4):335–345. doi: 10.1007/s10865-005-9003-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Larrison-Faucher AL, Matorin AA, Sereno AB. Nicotine reduces antisaccade errors in task impaired schizophrenic subjects. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2004;28(3):505–516. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Mitchell SH. Measuring impulsivity and modeling its association with cigarette smoking. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004;3(4):261–275. doi: 10.1177/1534582305276838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Munoz DP, Armstrong IT, Hampton KA, Moore KD. Altered control of visual fixation and saccadic eye movements in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90(1):503–514. doi: 10.1152/jn.00192.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51(6):768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Muri RM, Ploner CJ, Gaymard B, Rivaud-Pechoux S. Cortical control of ocular saccades in humans: a model for motricity. Prog Brain Res. 2003;142:3–17. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)42003-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Powell J, Dawkins L, Davis RE. Smoking, reward responsiveness, and response inhibition: tests of an incentive motivational model. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51(2):151–163. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01208-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Powell JH, Pickering AD, Dawkins L, West R, Powell JF. Cognitive and psychological correlates of smoking abstinence, and predictors of successful cessation. Addict Behav. 2004;29(7):1407–1426. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Rose J, Behm F. Extinguishing the rewarding value of smoke cues: Pharmacological and behavioral treatments. Nicotine Tob Res. 2004;6(3):523–532. doi: 10.1080/14622200410001696501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Rosse RB, McCarthy MF, Alim TN, Deutsch SI. Saccadic distractibility in cocaine dependent patients: a preliminary laboratory exploration of the cocaine-OCD hypothesis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1994;35(1):25–30. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(94)90106-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Shiffman S, Gwaltney CJ, Balabanis MH, Liu KS, Paty JA, Kassel JD, et al. Immediate antecedents of cigarette smoking: an analysis from ecological momentary assessment. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111(4):531–545. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.4.531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Zuckerman M. P-impulsive sensation seeking and its behavioral, psychophysiological and biochemical correlates. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(12):30–36. doi: 10.1159/000118996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES