Abstract
The objective of this review was to evaluate whether systematic visual training leads to (1) a restitution of the visual field (restoration), (2) an increase in the visual search field size or an improvement in scanning strategies (compensation) and (3) a transfer of training‐related improvements in activities of daily living such as reading. To retrieve relevant publications, computer‐aided searches of databases (Medline, Embase, Cinahl, Cochrane Central Registers of Controlled Trials) and extensive reference tracing and hand searching were performed. Subsequently, all retrieved and blinded studies were scored on methodological quality. 14 studies were included, 2 randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and 12 within‐subject repeated‐measures designs (RMD). One of the two RCT studies had good quality. The internal validity of the RMD studies varied from poor to good. Five studies reported a significant effect of the vision restoration therapy (VRT), whereas two studies reported no effect using scanning laser ophthalmoscopy or Goldmann perimetry as outcome measure. All authors of the studies on scanning compensatory therapy (SCT) found a significant effect of up to 30° visual search field, a significant increase in reading speed or decrease in reading errors. It is unclear to what extent patients benefit from restoration therapy in relation to a more efficient scanning strategy which enables them to read faster or to avoid obstacles in a better way. No study has given a satisfactory answer. SCT seems to provide a more successful rehabilitation with more simple and user‐friendly training techniques. Validated questionnaires provide the most reliable subjective data to assess the transfer of the relevance of training procedures to activities of daily living of the patient. Hence, SCT is recommended until the effect of the VRT is defined.
The largest group of visual disorders after acquired brain injury are homonymous visual field defects (HVFDs). Homonymous hemianopia refers to a loss of perception over half the field of vision, affecting both eyes, due to a deficient cortical representation of parts of the visual field or deficient transmission of information from the chiasma towards the visual cortex. Approximately 20–30% of all patients with cerebrovascular infarction requiring treatment in a rehabilitation centre have HVFDs.1 Some 70% of patients with HVFDs show a spatially disorganised visual search strategy.2 Such patients have particular difficulties with reading and visual exploration, which have far‐reaching, disabling repercussions on their domestic and vocational lives. These percentages indicate the impact of the HVFDs, and how important structured rehabilitation efforts for this group of patients can be.
Since the beginning of the 20th century, efforts have been made to train patients with homonymous hemianopia systematically.3 Over the past decades, many authors have found evidence that patients may successfully adapt to their HVFDs by training. Some authors claim that their rehabilitation methods lead to restitution of part of the HVFDs.4,5,6,7,8 According to this view, training reactivates surviving neurons of the partially damaged brain structure itself—that is, the border region (transition zone) or islands of residual vision that exist in some patients with cortical damage. This is also called border shift.4 Other authors have found evidence that patients may successfully adapt to their HVFDs by compensatory oculomotor strategies—that is, by learning to make large eye movements into the blind hemifield, thereby enlarging the field of search and improving visually guided activities of daily living.1,2,9,10,11 Recently, Pambakian et al10 found that patients with lesions <6 months old with HVFDs adapt themselves to the loss of HVFDs (eg, by orienting) in the absence of training. This does not tend to occur in the presence of unilateral spatial neglect.
It is not yet clear whether and, if so, which of these two methods is more effective. Neither is it clear whether the patients benefit from both methods in activities of daily living. There is a debate among authors about what instruments might most accurately measure increase in visual field.7,12,13,14 Despite many years of research, there is no consensus on how to determine a border shift. In general, the compensatory strategy method is accepted, but there is criticism about its effect due to a lack of controlled studies.
Hence, a systematic review was conducted of all relevant studies in order to evaluate the effects of visual training for patients with HVFDs. The three main objectives were to review whether systematic visual training can lead to (1) a restitution of the visual field (restoration), (2) an increase in the visual search field size or an improvement of scanning strategies (compensation) and (3) a transfer of training‐related improvements in activities of daily living such as reading.
Methods
Literature search
Relevant publications were identified by means of computerised searches and citation tracking (box 1).
The search strategy included Medline (Winspirs), Embase (Winspirs), Cinahl (Winspirs) and the Cochrane Central Registers of Controlled Trials for the period 1966–2005/07. Furthermore, references of conference reports, references of most relevant studies, citations of most relevant studies and related articles were checked for relevant materials. Vocational reintegration was not included in the search because this term did not provide any useful hits.
Box 1 Search strategy used for computerised searches identifying the design of randomised controlled trials, of controlled clinical trials, of retrospective studies or of repeated‐measures design studies
1 “Hemianopia”/all subheadings
2 Homonymous hemianop*
3 Hemineglect
4 Hemianopic dyslexia
5 Hemianopic alexia
6 Hemianopic reading
7 Cerebral blindness
#8 #1 OR #2 OR #3 OR #4 OR #5 OR #6 OR #7
9 Rehabilitation AND hemianop*
10 Treatment and hemianop*
11 Visual training
12 Vision restoration therapy
13 “Saccades”/all subheadings
14 Eye movement and hemianop*
15 Oculomotor rehabilitation
#16 #9 OR #10 OR #11 OR #12 OR #13 OR #14 OR #15
#17 #8 AND #16
18 Activity of daily living
#19 #17 AND #18
21 Quality of life
#22 #17 AND #21
Study selection
Studies were selected if they met the following entry criteria: (1) inclusion of only patients with HVFDs due to post‐chiasmatic lesions of the visual system after brain injury, documented by CT/MRI scans, and patients with left or right field defects ranging from homonymous quadrantanopia to complete homonymous hemianopia, with and without macular sparing; (2) applying the intervention of vision restoration therapy (VRT) or of compensatory saccadic eye movements and visual search strategies—that is, scanning compensatory therapy (SCT); (3) using the outcome measures of visual field size, visual search field, reading time and reading error, and subjective measures of questionnaires; (4) using the design of RCT, of controlled clinical trial (CCT), of retrospective studies or of RMD studies; (5) using only publications written in English, German or Dutch. The assessment of studies potentially eligible for meeting the entry criteria was done independently by two of the authors (LB and JH). Disagreements were solved by discussion. If disagreement persisted, the judgement of a third reviewer (CL) was decisive. Inter‐rater agreement was expressed using Cohen's κ.
Assessment of methodological quality of the trials
It is known that patients with visual deficits due to acquired brain damage are not a homogeneous group.1,10 Because of the heterogeneity of underlying brain lesions, it is difficult to ensure that control and experimental groups are comparable.1 RCTs are therefore scarce, and hence RMD studies were included, in which patients act as their own controls. All studies were scored on methodological quality. Two authors (LB and JH) independently assessed the publications with the Cochrane checklist for RCT and with the developed checklist for RMD studies (supplementary appendix 1 available online at http://jnnp.bmj.com/supplemental). Thirteen criteria were used to evaluate the internal validity and clinical relevance of RCT. Each criterion was scored as good, moderate or poor. A validated list of criteria for assessing the methodological quality of the RMD studies was not available, hence a list of 11 criteria was developed for assessing study quality (box 2).
Box 2 Criteria list for assessing the methodological quality of repeated‐measures design studies
Was a representative sample of participants used?
Was the size of visual field defect sufficiently specified?
Was macular sparing/splitting specified in terms of measurement of macular sparing/splitting?
Was restitution of visual field adequately measured?
Was visual search field adequately measured?
Was reading performance adequately measured?
Was functional outcome adequately measured?
Were stimuli of outcome measures derived from the stimuli of the training programme or vice versa?
Was comorbidity identified as a confounding factor and controlled for?
Was spontaneous recovery identified as a confounding factor and controlled for?
Were examiners blinded to clinical information from participants?
Criteria were designed to tap domains of external validity (items 1–3) and internal validity (items 4–11). Three of the criteria described the external validity. The period of time after onset lesion, location of the lesion and aetiology of the lesion were considered important factors for external validity of the studies. Scores on items 4–11 were assumed to be of decisive importance for internal validity. Four outcome measures were included: size of visual field, size of visual search field (the term visual search field is defined as the area that a patient can actively scan by eye movements but without head movement), reading performance and subjective measures (items 4–7). Fixation control was defined as the criterion to assess whether the restitution of visual field was adequately measured. The aim of the VRT is to increase visual field size by shifting the absolute visual field border and improving detection ability in areas of residual vision. Stimulation in this area could provoke saccadic eye movements towards the stimulus, which can be misinterpreted as a visual field recovery.14 Four confounding factors which could cause bias in the studies were analysed: stimuli of outcome measures derived from stimuli of the training programme or vice versa, comorbidities such as unilateral spatial neglect, period of time after lesion onset and blinding of examiners (item 8–11). Items were scored as good, moderate or poor. Items were equally weighted.
Disagreements with respect to methodological quality were identified and resolved in a consensus discussion. If consensus could not be reached, the third reviewer (CL) made the final decision. The final quality score for each study was based on full consensus between the reviewers.
Data extraction and data analysis
From the original studies, we extracted data on participants (number, gender, age, time after onset, specification of visual field defect, aetiology and location of lesion), pathology from MRI/CT, confounding factors (comorbidity and spontaneous recovery), intervention (visual restoration therapy, compensation visual search therapy), outcome measures and transfer of treatment gains to functional outcome measures. The studies may not be sufficiently similar with respect to outcome measures to allow summarising data statistically. Hence, these studies are described here.
Results
Literature search and study selection
The systematic literature search yielded 315 publications. Of these, 26 possible relevant studies were retrieved as full articles. As the review deals with a broad question, a sensitive search was performed in order not to miss possibly relevant studies. Consequently, 289 of the 315 publications did not meet the inclusion criteria. Of the 289 papers that were rejected, one third, for example, did not have the proper design as they were case studies. Furthermore, in the majority of the publications, hemianopia was only mentioned in the abstract or keywords as a sign of a particular disorder, and treatment was, as such, not the focus. This information could in all cases be retrieved from the abstracts.
Assessment of these studies with regard to their potential eligibility for meeting the entry criteria reduced the number of studies to 12. Reference tracing and hand searching yielded four more possibly relevant articles. In total, 14 studies were selected, of which 2 were RCT and 12 were RMD studies. There were 15 disagreements between reviewers on selection of the studies and extraction data, resulting in a moderate inter‐reviewer agreement Cohen's κ of 0.54. All disagreements were resolved by discussion; consequently, there was no need to consult the third reviewer for a final decision.
Two RCT studies4,5 and five RMD studies6,7,8,12,13 described the effect of VRT. Seven studies with RMD1,2,9,10,11,15,16 described the effect of SCT, of which two were focused on reading problems. A total of 420 patients who fulfilled the above‐mentioned inclusion criteria were taken into account for this study. In all, 70 out of 420 subjects participated in the RCT (34 in the experimental group and 36 in the control group) and 350 in RMD studies; 64 patients were trained using the VRT and 286 patients using the SCT.
Assessment of methodological quality of the trials
Table 1 gives a detailed description of the included studies. The studies are listed according to the type of training and year of publication.
Table 1 Characteristics of included studies.
Study | Method | Participants | Intervention | Outcome measures | Effect | Remarks |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
VRT | ||||||
Reinhard et al (2005)13 | RMD study | 17 patients, time after onset: >1 year, lesion location in central visual pathways | VRT: Nova Vision, 6 days a week, 60 min sessions, length of intervention 6 months | Objective | Objective | Pathology heterogeneous |
rm: 2 | Mean age: 49 (range 24–72) years | Fixation control: no | SLO difference of ratio before and after training = E, 0.14E = 1 degree | SLO: no increase in border shift | Outcome not comparable with other studies, no comparison between SLO and HRP/TAP | |
Before training | Head and eye movements: no | ADL: text presented in the SLO, reading speed, wpm | ADL: reading speed: 6% improvement: not relevant | No correlation of objective results with subjective reports, no clear data | ||
After training | Subjective | Good internal validity | ||||
No follow‐up | Subjective | 2/3 satisfied with training | ||||
Reports of patients | 6 satisfied with reading; 4 not satisfied, 4 no reading problem before training | |||||
Sabel et al (2004)14 | RMD study | 16 patients, post hoc 2 groups, COM (n = 9) and INC (n = 7), time after onset: >1 year, lesion location | VRT: Nova Vision, 6 days, twice a week, 30 min sessions, length of intervention 6 months | Objective | Objective | Pathology: heterogeneous |
rm: 2 | Post‐chiasmatic | Fixation control: no | SLO difference of ratio before after training = E, 0.14E = 1 degree | SLO: no increase in border shift | Efficacy of VRT depends on which outcome measure is used | |
Before training | Mean age: 49.3 (range 24–72) years | Head and eye movements: no | HRP, number of hits | HRP: 5.28–7.01° (0.20°) increase in border shift | Subjective: reading: no clear data | |
After training | TAP, number of misses | HRP and TAP: OD 4.56–6.05° (0.20°); OS 4.49–5.47° (0.21°) increase in border shift | No relationship between improvement in perimetric procedures and subjective vision | |||
No follow‐up | Subjective: | |||||
Standardised vision state questionnaire | Subjective: | Good internal validity | ||||
Vision change questionnaire | 87.5% satisfied with VRT | |||||
Reports of patients | Vision state questionnaire: 10% improvement | |||||
Vision change questionnaire: 14/15 patients improved | ||||||
Julkunen et al (2003)6 | RMD study | 5 patients, time after onset: >1 year, lesion location | VRT: developed computer program training, 3 times a week, 60 min sessions | Objective | Objective: | Pathology: homogeneous |
rm: 3 | Left occipital inf 1 | Length of intervention: 33–47 h (3–4 months) | Kinetic: Goldmann, degrees | Perimetry: yes, VEP: yes | Detailed description of patients | |
Before training | Left occipital ich 1 | Fixation control: yes | Static: Octopus101, degrees | No clear data of objective outcome measures and subjective data of reading | ||
After training | Right temporal ich 1 | Head and eye movements: no | VEP, latency in ms | Subjective improvement 2, decline 2, no change 1 | No general conclusions due to small group | |
After follow‐up | Left multiple ich 1 | Subjective questionnaire | Moderate internal validity | |||
Follow‐up period: 3 months | Right temperooccipital inf extending to thalamus:1 | |||||
Age: 18–70 years | ||||||
Kasten et al (2001)5 | Randomised trial double blind | 22 patients, exp. group (n = 16) | VRT: exp. group: Visure, Seetrain, plac. group: Fixtra, daily, 60 min sessions, length intervention 150 h within 6 months | Objective | Objective | Pathology: heterogeneous |
Follow‐up study RCT 1998 | Plac group (n = 6), time after onset: >1 year, lesion location | Fixation control: yes | PeriMa, number of stimuli; PeriForm, number of forms; PeriColor, number of colours; TAP, number of hits and misses | Mean (SD) exp.group increase 0.4 (0.9°), plac. group increase 0.13° (0.6°) | Different outcome measures and VRT in comparison with pretreatment and post‐treatment periods (Kasten, 1998); hence incomparable with data before‐ and after training | |
Follow‐up period: | Post‐chiasmatic‐optic nerve | Head and eye movements: no | Subjective | No blinding of participants | ||
Mean (SD) 23.5 (2.3) months | Mean age | Questionnaire | Subjective | No subjective data from this study | ||
Exp. group 47.7 (12.9), plac. group 55.3 (range 16.2) | From study 1998 | < poor internal validity > | ||||
Kasten et al (1998)4 | Two randomised controlled trials | 46 patients | VRT exp. group: Nova Vision, plac. group: fixation training programme, | Objective | Objective | Pathology heterogeneous |
1. Optic nerve | Exp. group (n = 18) | daily, 60 min sessions, length intervention 6 months. | Primary: HRP, | Primary: improvement | Subjective measures: no separate outcome measures optic nerve lesion and postchiasmatic | |
2. Postchiasmatic | Plac. group (n = 30), time after onset: >1 year, lesion location | Fixation control; no | Secondary: TAP | Exp HRP: 29.4% border shift 4.9° (1.7), placebo HRP: 7.7% | < good internal validity> | |
included: postchiasmatic trial | Post‐chiasmatic and | Head and eye movements: no | Subjective | Border shift −0.9° (±0.8) | ||
No follow‐up | Optic nerve | Pre‐trial: history interview | Secondary: improvement | |||
Mean (SD) age | Post‐trial: questionnaire | Exp TAP border shift 0.43° (0.34) | ||||
Exp group 47.7 (12.9), plac. group 55.3 (16.2) | Placebo TAP −0.51° (0.34) | |||||
Subjective improvements: | ||||||
Exp 72.2%, placebo: 16.6% | ||||||
Kasten et al (1995)8 | RMD study | 14 patients, time after onset: 0.5–240 months, lesion location: | VRT Visure, Seetrain, Formtrain, daily, 60 min sessions, length intervention 80–300 hours. | Objective | Objective | Pathology heterogeneous |
rm 2 | Post‐chiasmatic. | Fixation control: no | Static perimetry: | Improvement: | Specific training effect light, form and colour: modality specific | |
Before training | Mean age 48.5 | Head and eye movements: no | Perimat | Perimat: 41.6% in 9 of 11 patients | Within first 20 hours no training effect, increase effect from 30 hours | |
After training | Periform | Periform: 37.4%, depending on hours of training | < moderate to poor internal validity> | |||
No follow‐up | Pericolor: | Pericolor: 25.7% | ||||
Dynamic: TAP | TAP: unclear | |||||
Subjective | ||||||
None | ||||||
Balliet et al (1985)12 | RMD study | 12 patients, time after onset: 5–36 months, lesion location: | VRT Goldmann perimeter, compensation training for patients who failed VRT: Goldmann, | Objective | Objective: | Pathology homogeneous |
rm 2 | Occipital. | 2–5 days weekly, 60 min sessions, length intervention | Goldmann perimeter | Restitution: Goldmann perimeter: 1° | Used same instrument for test and training (Goldmann), bias on training effect | |
Before training | Age: 56–66 | 2–11 months. | Subjective: | Compensation: 0° | Emphasis on discussing measurement error caused by compensation eccentric fixation and on good fixation control | |
After training | Fixation control: yes | Reports of patients | Subjective: | < moderate to poor internal validity> | ||
No follow‐up | Head and eye movements: no | report of patients: no changes in visual field | ||||
SCT | ||||||
Pambakian et al (2004)10 | RMD study | 31 patients, time after onset: 3 to >12 months, lesion location: | Compensation training | Objective | Objective | Pathology: heterogeneous |
rm 4 | Postchiasmatic | Developed home training on a monitor, daily 40 min sessions, length intervention 1 month | Humphrey | Restitution: 0° | Used same instrument for test and training, bias on training effect | |
2 before training | Mean age: 49.7 (range 24–75) | Response time, error rates | Compensation:76% improvement | Controlled for age: elderly patients benefited more from training | ||
2 after training | ADL: response time | ADL: 31 patients 25% improvement | < moderate internal validity > | |||
Follow‐up: 1 month | Subjective | |||||
Standardised questionnaire Kerkhoff | Subjective: | |||||
Improvement of 27 patients | ||||||
S (p = <0.0002) | ||||||
Nelles et al (2001)9 | RMD stduy | 21 patients, mean time after onset: | VRT and compensation training on a board (CVFT), two daily, 30 min sessions, length intervention 1 month | Objective | Objective | Pathology: no description |
rm: 2 | 1. 5 months (range 0.5–24), | TAP | Restition: not mentioned | Outcome measure and intervention are alike: bias on training effect | ||
Before training | no description lesion location. | CVFT | Compensation: improvement | Follow‐up effect: no outcome measures | ||
After training | Mean age: 59.2 (±3.5) | Reponse time, error rates | Group A: NS | <poor internal validity > | ||
follow‐up period | Subgroup | Subjective | Group B: S (p⩽0.02) | |||
8 months from 15 patients | A: eyes fixating B:exploratory eye movements | Standardised questionnaire Kerkhoff with item reading added | Subjective | |||
Improvement S(p⩽0.05) | ||||||
Zihl (1995)2 | RMD study | 14 patients, mean time after onset: | Compensation training | Objective | Objective: | Pathology: homogeneous |
rm 2 | 11 weeks (range 6–18), lesion location: | Saccadic eye movements with TAP, searching task with slides, 30 min sessions, length intervention 16 (range 8–23). | TAP | Restorative: no | Emphasis study oculomotor scanning | |
Before training | Striate cortex, thalamus, occipito‐parietal | Visual scanning: Pupil‐corneal‐reflection method, search time, length of scanpath,number of fixations. | Visual scanning: improvement three variables S (p⩽0.001) | Damage in occipito‐parietal cortex, optic radioation, striate cortex more training sessions necessary than occipital lesions | ||
After training | Mean age: 44 (range 23–74) | Subjective | Subjective: reduction of complaints | No clear subjective measures | ||
No follow‐up | Complaints before and after training | <moderate internal validity > | ||||
Kerkhoff et al (1994)1 | RMD study | 22 patients, mean time after onset: | Compensation training | Objective: | Objective: | Pathology: homogeneous |
rm: 3 | 7.5 months (range 1–37), lesion location: | Training in 3 steps: | TAP | Restitution: mean increase | Left vs right hemianopia, early vs late training no significant differences | |
Before training | occipital: 12 | large saccades on large screen, visual search with slides, transfer to ADL, | Visual search field: TAP, slides, table test | 6.6° (range 2° to 24°) | All patients returned to previous job | |
After training | Occipito‐temporal: 3 | 5 days a week, 30 min sessions, length intervention 1–3 months | Subjective | visual search field increase: mean 30° | <good internal validity > | |
After follow‐up | Temporal: 17 | Standardised questionnaire Kerkhoff | search time decrease: S (p = 0.01), reduction of errors: 50% | |||
Follow‐up period: | Mean age: 46 (range 16–77) | Subjective | ||||
3 months (1–12) | significant improvements p‐value 0.01 | |||||
Kerkhoff et al (1992)11 | RMD study | 122 patients, | Compensation training | Objectieve | Objective: | Pathology: heterogeneous |
rm 3 | VFD group (n = 92), | Training in 3 steps: | TAP | Restitution: mean increase: 1.6° (range 1.0° to 30°) | More training sessions necessary during training with head movements. | |
Before training | VFD+ group (n = 30), | Large saccades on large screen, visual search with slides, transfer to ADL | Visual search field increase: mean 30° | Time after lesion, etiology, type of visual field defect, visual field sparing and age irrelevant for treatment success | ||
After training | included VFD group, | 5 days a week, 30 | Subjective | Subjective | <moderate internal validity > | |
After follow‐up | mean time after onset: 32.8(range 1–220), lesion location | min sessions, length intervention 6 weeks | None | No data | ||
Follow‐up period: | Postchiasmatic. | |||||
Mean 22 months | Mean age 49 (range 117–74) | |||||
SCT focused on reading | ||||||
Kerkhoff et al (1992)15 | RMD study | 56 patients, mean time after onset: 40.2 weeks (range 3–220), no description lesion location. | Compensation training | Objective | Restitution mean increase: 1.6° (range 1.0° to 20°) | Pathology: heterogeneous |
rm 3 | Mean age: 46.8 (range 13–74) | Computer‐based method with moving text, 5 days a week, 40 min sessions, length intervention 14 sessions in 4–6 weeks. | TAP | Reading time reduction: | Most severely disturbed patients benefited most during training | |
Before training | Reading time | S (p⩽0.02) | Reduction reading time irrespective initial reading time before training | |||
After training | Reading errors | Reading errors reduction: | <good internal validity > | |||
After follow‐up | Subjective data | before treatment: 4.97 (8.1) | ||||
Follow‐up period | Standardised questionnaire Kerkhoff | after follow‐up: 1.48(1.66) | ||||
mean 22 months | Subjective data | |||||
(6 months –5 years) | not reported | |||||
Zihl (1995)16 | RMD study | 20 patients, | Compensation training | Objective | Restitution: no | Pathology: heterogeneous |
rm: 2 | LH group (n = 10) | Computer based with moving text, 5 days a week. 40 min sessions, mean length intervention LH 11 (range 8–16) sessions, RH 22 (range 9–29). | TAP | Reading speed: wpm | Clear relationship between improvements in reading performance and changes of eye movements parameters | |
Before training | RH group (n = 10), time after onset: | Pupil‐corneal reflection method | LH 76→113 | < moderate to good internal validity > | ||
After training | LH 3–12 weeks (5.8), RH 4–9 weeks(5.9), | RH 53→96 | ||||
No follow‐up | No description lesion location. Mean age LH group 39 (range 21–53), RH group 37 (range 19–54) | Subjective data | Perceptual span | |||
None | LH 3.75°→4.03° | |||||
RH 2.79°→3.74° | ||||||
Subjective data | ||||||
None |
exp, experimental; HRP, High‐ resolution perimetry; Ich, intracerebral haemorrhage; Inf, infarction; LH, left‐sided hemianopia; OD, right eye; OS, left eye; VRT, vision restoration therapy; VEP, Visual evoked potential; plac.,placebo; RCT, randomised controlled trial; RH, right‐sided hemianopia; rm, repeated measures; RMD, repeated‐measures design; S, significant; SLO, scanning laser ophthalmoscope; TAP, Tübinger automatic perimeter.
The agreement of the methodological quality assessment of the two authors (LB and JH) was high and after discussion full consensus was reached. Methodological quality scores of included studies are presented in table 2.
Table 2 Methodological quality scores of included studies of repeated‐measures design (in alphabetical order on restoration and compensation therapy).
First author (year) | External validity | Internal validity | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | |
VRT | |||||||||||
Balliet et al (1985)12 | G | G | G | M | M | — | P | P | M | M | P |
Kasten et al (1995)8 | M | G | M | M | — | — | P | M | P | M | P |
Julkunen et al (2003)6 | G | M | P | M | — | — | M | G | M | G | P |
Sabel et al (2004)7 | G | M | M | G | — | — | G | G | M | G | P |
Reinhard et al (2005)13 | G | M | M | G | — | P | P | G | M | G | P |
SCT | |||||||||||
Kerkhoff et al (1992)11 | G | M | P | M | M | — | P | G | G | M | P |
Kerkhoff et al (1994)1 | G | M | P | M | G | — | G | G | G | M | P |
Zihl (1995)2 | G | G | M | M | M | — | M | G | M | M | P |
Nelles et al (2001)9 | G | M | P | — | M | — | G | P | P | P | P |
Pambakian et al (2004)10 | G | M | P | M | G | — | G | M | M | M | P |
SCT focused on reading | |||||||||||
Kerkhoff et al (1992)15 | G | M | G | M | — | G | P | G | G | M | P |
Zihl (1995)16 | G | G | G | — | — | M | P | G | G | M | P |
–, not measured; G, good; M, moderate; P, poor.
Numbers correspond to questions in checklist for assessing methodological quality of subject within repeated‐measures design.
Of the studies that reported the effect of VRT, the RCT of Kasten et al4 had good internal and external validity, but the follow‐up study of Kasten et al5 had poor internal validity. Of the RMD studies that reported the effect of the VRT, the study of Sabel et al7 and Reinhard et al13 had good internal validity. The study of Julkunen et al6 had moderate internal validity. Two studies8,12 had moderate to poor internal validity. Both studies were included in this systematic review because they contributed to the development of the rehabilitation of the patients with homonymous hemianopia. Balliet12 was the first author to discuss the issue of an adequate fixation control, but did not have access to the developed instrumental measurements of the later studies. The study of Kasten et al8 was an open pilot trial, which was followed in 1998 by the RCT study.4 Five studies4,5,6,7,8 reported a significant effect of VRT, whereas two studies12,13 reported no effect of VRT.
Of the studies focused on SCT, the study of Kerkhoff et al1,15 had good internal validity, whereas the study of Zihl16 had moderate to good internal validity; the studies of Zihl,2 Pambakian et al10 and Kerkhoff et al11 had moderate internal validity, and the study of Nelles et al9 had poor internal validity. All authors found a significant effect of up to 30° visual search field, or a significant increase in reading speed or decrease in reading errors.
Data extraction and data analyses
Two RCT publications4,5 were analysed. The first study4 describes the pretreatment and post‐treatment effects of restorative therapy, the second study5 describes the follow‐up. The RCT of Kasten et al4 assessed in two independent trials the effect of VRT in patients with optic nerve lesion or post‐chiasmatic brain injury. This review included only the trial of the post‐chiasmatic lesions, which had a good methodological score on randomisation, blinding and comparability of the groups. Kasten found a border shift of 4.9° using high‐resolution perimetry (HRP), but a border shift of 0.43° using Tuebinger automatic perimeter (TAP). In the follow‐up study of Kasten et al,5 the patients were recruited from the original population of the study of Kasten et al.4 This study had poor internal validity, since the placebo group in the follow‐up study was not blinded. All patients treated with placebo had been offered VRT after completion of the previous trial. Also, the number of patients treated with placebo was small.
Out of 10 patients in the placebo group, only 6 were re‐examined. Different types of restoration therapy and outcome measures were used in comparison with the pretreatment and post‐treatment periods.4,5 Consequently, the outcome measures of the follow‐up period were incomparable with the data before and after training.
Analysis of the RMD publications
All studies gave a good description of the characteristics of the population. Only Kasten et al8 did not mention explicitly the inclusion and exclusion criteria. However, lesion location was specified in only two of the VRT studies6,12 and in two of the compensatory therapy studies.1,2 All other studies only described the lesions as post‐chiasmatic and therefore, in many studies, it remained unclear to what extent the outcome was influenced by comorbidity. Two of the VRT studies8,12 and two of the compensatory therapy studies2,16 scored good on defining the size of the visual field defect. All other studies only described left or right, complete or incomplete HVFDs, and were therefore rated as moderate. Macular sparing/splitting was not mentioned in the majority of the compensation therapy studies,1,9,10,11 whereas in the majority of the VRT studies the macular sparing/splitting was adequately measured.
Of the five VRT studies, restitution of the visual field was adequately measured in two studies.7,13 The method used in these studies provides a simultaneous assessment of the retinal image and the stimulus in the central 10° visual field, thus allowing an absolute fixation control. Although all authors used the enlargement of the visual field as an outcome measure of the VRT, the instruments determining visual field were very diverse and made an overall effect estimate impossible. Sabel et al7 found no effect on border shift using SLO, whereas he found an absolute border shift of 1.73° using HRP. Reinhard's et al's study13 used SLO and found no change in the absolute field defect border after training. Julkunen6 used two perimetric methods and is the only author who applied pattern reversal visual evoked potential as an outcome measure. After training, there was a significant change of 5° in visual angle using the dynamic Goldmann perimeter and static Octopus101 perimeter. In 9 of the 11 patients Kasten et al8 found a visual field enlargement using the Perimat test, with an average of 41.6%, and did not mention data regarding border shift of the visual field. No clear data were found using TAP. Balliet et al12 found <1° of apparent visual field change using the dynamic Goldmann perimeter. In five SCT studies1,2,10,11,15 the measurement of the restoration of visual field was not the focus of the study and was considered a byproduct of the visual training. These studies were defined as moderate. Zihl2 and Pambakian et al10 found no effect. Kerkhoff et al1 found a mean increase of 6.6° (range 2° to 24°), Kerkhoff et al15 a mean increase of 1.6° (range 1.0° to 20°) and Kerkhoff et al11 a mean increase of 1.6° (range 1.0° to 30°). The clinical significance of the effect of an intervention depends on how large the treatment effects are in clinical practice. The VRT studies reported an effect of up to 5° increase in visual field size. This small effect could be clinically significant for reading, for fluent reading the visual span has to be extended up to 5°, whereas for scanning scenes this effect is too small to be clinically significant. An effect of up to 40° would be clinically significant enough for the subject to be able to explore the world as reported by the SCT studies.
Among the SCT studies, visual search field was adequately measured in the studies by Kerkhoff et al1 and Pambakian et al,10 since different methods for measuring VSF were used. The studies of Zihl,2 Nelles et al9 and Kerkhoff et al11 were judged moderate using only one outcome measure. All studies showed an improvement in scanning strategies of up to 30° in the 46° VSF of the hemianopic visual field. As they used different instruments to measure the VSF, an overall effect estimate was not possible. Among the VRT studies, only the study of Balliet et al12 trained and measured VSF as an effect on restitution. He found very small changes in VSF. The study was judged as moderate.
Kerkhoff et al15 scored good on the retest reliability of the reading test. Zihl16 used a standardised reading test and his study was judged moderate. In both studies, reading time and reading errors improved significantly. Of the VRT studies, only the study by Reinhard et al13 measured the reading performance as the effect of VRT training. The study scored poor on the measurement of reading performance. The increase of 6% in reading performance after VRT is hence doubtful.
With regard to the subjective measures of improvement, a difference was found between restorative studies and compensatory studies. A total of four studies of compensatory therapy used validated or standardised questionnaires.1,2,9,10 Only one study of restorative therapy7 used a validated questionnaire.
In the studies by Balliet et al12 and Nelles et al,9 most of the stimuli that were used in training were also used in the evaluation of improvement, and therefore this criterion was judged as poor.
Only the studies of Kerkhoff et al1,11,15 and Zihl16 described the use of neuropsychological tests to exclude comorbidities such as visuospatial disorders, and visual agnosia and alexia as confounding factors.
Three recent VRT studies controlled for spontaneous recovery and only trained patients who were >1 year post‐lesion.6,7,13 In none of the RMD studies were the examiners blinded to clinical information from participants.
Discussion
The methodological quality of the studies ranged from poor to good. Only two RCT studies could be selected from the literature search and therefore RMD studies were included in the search.
However, a repeated‐measures design implicates less control, and internal validity is by definition lower compared with RCT studies. In order to assure internal validity of the RMD studies, it is necessary to ensure that no factors other than the intervention itself determine the outcome measure. Therefore, quality assessment was performed using a developed criteria list focused on the internal validity and in particular on information bias and confounding factors.
Strikingly, none of the RMD studies reported whether examiners were blinded to clinical information from participants. Hence we cannot exclude the possibility that examiners could have been influenced by the results of the training or by seeing previous perimetry results before each measurement. As a consequence, bias of outcome measures cannot be excluded. To resolve this issue, blinding of examiners should be pursued in future studies.
Only a few studies controlled for visual neglect and visual agnosia with neuropsychological tests.1,11,15,16 Most studies did not pay much attention to the possibility of higher visual disorders. In our experience, cases of “pure” hemianopia are relatively rare because in most cases parts of the occipital pole (BA 17) as well as other, more anterior brain regions are damaged. The chances of higher order disorders supplementary to the hemianopia are quite high when the occipitotemporal or occipitoparietal regions are involved. Since in most studies patients were selected on the basis of having post‐chiasmatic lesions, it is unlikely that lesions of these patients were limited to the occipital pole.
The size of the visual field defect and the presence or absence of macular sparing or splitting depends on the location of the brain lesion and varies between patients. In general, macular sparing in hemianopia occurs only when the lesion is limited to the occipital pole. Hence, it should be analysed carefully and expressed in the outcome measures. The majority of the studies did not specify these factors, which might be of importance for the chances of successful rehabilitation. Except for the studies of Kerkhoff et al1 post hoc, Kasten et al5 and Sabel et al,7 in none of the studies was the size of the HVFD taken into account as a weighed measure in the analysis of improvement after treatment.
Among the five studies that met the entry criteria of the restoration intervention, a certain line of research can be distinguished starting from the study of Balliet et al12 to the recent study of Reinhard.13 Zihl and Von Cramon were the first to evaluate systematically the effects of specific perimetry training on visual field size in visual field defects.17 Since the 1980s, there has been considerable development in the quality of methods and instruments to assess visual fields and fixation control. Our review shows no consensus between different authors about which methods should be used to measure the exact size of the visual field and improvements in the transition zone. Some authors12,13 claim that studies using perimetric or campimetric methods do not control sufficiently for eye movements or para‐central fixation. There is a discussion about a mismatch of border position between SLO on the one hand and HRP/TAP on the other hand. In most patients the SLO is noticeably closer to the midline than the HRP/TAP border.7 In all studies, the original size of the visual field defect and also the efficacy of VRT depended on the method of the perimetric measurements and on the fixation control that was used. Thus, the apparent visual field defect is greater when measured with SLO than when measured with HRP or TAP. After VRT, the mismatch is even more pronounced. To our knowledge, this matter has not been resolved.
Potential confounders are the effect of practice in detection or discrimination tasks, and measurement errors due to improper fixation, which can cause eccentric fixation. Improper alignment in the baseline measurement can cause a mismatch border position.
It would have been very useful to evaluate whether an improvement in the transition zone also leads to an improvement in the VSF. We found no strong evidence that the possible gain of a few degrees of visual field results in better oculomotor scanning strategies and leads to a better performance of activities of daily living.
If visual search field and relevant activities of daily living such as reading would indeed improve as a result of a small border shift, one would perhaps also expect that the initial size of the VFD before the training would correlate to the level of impairment. To our knowledge, there is no strong evidence that points to this. On the other hand, it seems that in VRT studies there is a basic assumption that patients benifit from reducing the HVFD by a few degrees, which makes it even more necessary to use the size of the initial visual field deficit as a weighed factor in the analysis.
SCT seems to provide a more successful rehabilitation, with more simple and user‐friendly training techniques. The data of the studies show that patients performed significantly faster in search strategies after compensatory therapy. Scanning strategies are applied to and trained in real life scenes. However, none of these studies compare the results with those of an untreated control group.
The evidence of the transfer of training‐related improvements in activities of daily living of both VRT and SCT is limited. Validated questionnaires seem to provide the most reliable subjective data to assess the translation of the relevance of training procedures to activities of daily living of the patient.
Conclusion
It is unclear to what extent patients benefit from restoration therapy in relation to a more efficient scanning strategy that enables them to read faster or to avoid obstacles in a better way.
No study has given a satisfactory answer. The discrepancy between the positive results of the restoration by perimetric measurements and the null‐SLO finding diminishes the chances of restoration after VRT. The latest discussions prove that restorative therapy requires further study of residual vision.
Transfer of visual search performance in activities of daily living is not sufficiently proven. There is a need for more validated instruments that can measure therapy outcome objectively.
Until the effect of the restoration therapy is further evaluated, visual search therapy is recommended.
Supplementary file containing the appendix is available online at http://jnnp.bmj.com/supplemental
Copyright © 2007 BMJ Publishing Group Ltd
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Professor Dr J Zihl for his comments and advise as an expert in the field. We also thank Jos W Snoek, MD, PhD, neurologist, Anja van de Wege, PhD, Marion Priebe, MSc, and Emiel van Trijffel, MSc, for critically reviewing the draft version of this paper.
Abbreviations
HRP - high‐resolution perimetry
HVFD - homonymous visual field defect
RCT - randomised controlled trial
RMD - repeated‐measures design
SCT - scanning compensatory therapy
SLO - scanning laser ophthalmoscope
TAP - Tuebinger automatic perimeter
VRT - vision restoration therapy
Footnotes
Competing interests: None declared.
Supplementary file containing the appendix is available online at http://jnnp.bmj.com/supplemental
References
- 1.Kerkhoff G, Munssinger U, Meier K. Neurovisual rehabilitation in cerebral blindness. Arch Neurol 199451474–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Zihl J. Visual scanning behavior in patients with homonymous hemianopia. Neuropsychologia 199533287–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Poppelreuter W.Disturbances of lower and higher visual capacities caused by occipital damage. Zihl J, Weiskrantz L. (Translators). Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1917/1990
- 4.Kasten E, Wuest S, Behrens‐Baumann W.et al Computer‐based training for the treatment of partial blindness. Nat Med 199841083–1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kasten E, Muller‐Oehring E, Sabel B A. Stability of visual field enlargements following computer‐based restitution training‐results of a follow‐up. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 20013297–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Julkunen L, Tenovuo O, Jaaskelainen S.et al Computer‐assisted training: a clinical and neurophysiological study. Restor Neurol Neurosci 20002119–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sabel B A, Kenkel S, Kasten E. Vision restoration therapy (VRT) efficacy as assessed by comparative perimetric analysis and subjective questionnaires. Restor Neurol Neurosci 200422399–420. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kasten E, Sabel B A. Visual field enlargement after computer training in brain‐damaged patients with homonymous deficits. Restor Neurol Neurosci 19958113–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Nelles G, Esser J, Eckstein A.et al Compensatory visual field training for patients with hemianopia after stroke. Neurosci Lett 2001306189–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pambakian A L, Mannan S K, Hodgson T L.et al Saccadic visual search training: a treatment for patients with homonymous hemianopia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004751443–1448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kerkhoff G, Munssinger U, Haaf E.et al Rehabilitation of homonymous scotomata in patients with postgeniculate damage of the visual system: Saccadic compensation training. Restor Neurol Neurosci 19924245–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Balliet R, Blood K M, Bach‐y‐Rita P. Visual field rehabilitation in the cortically blind? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1985481113–1124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Reinhard J, Schreiber A, Schiefer U.et al Does visual restitution training change absolute homonymous visual field defects? A fundus controlled study. Br J Ophthalmol 20058930–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sabel B A, Trauzettel‐Klosinski S. Improving vision in a patient with homonymous hemianopia. J Neuro‐Ophthalmol 200525143–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kerkhoff G, Munssinger U, Eberle‐Strauss G.et al Rehabilitation of hemianopic alexia in patients with postgeniculate visual field disorders. Neuropsychol Rehabil 1992221–42. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Zihl J. Eye movement patterns in hemianopic dyslexia. Brain 1995118891–912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Zihl J, Von Cramon D. Visual field recovery from scotoma in patients with postgeniculate damage. Brain 1985108335–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.