Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2007 Nov 21.
Published in final edited form as: Mol Psychiatry. 2007 Mar 13;12(6):544–555. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001978

Serotonin Transporter Polymorphism, Memory, and Hippocampal Volume in the Elderly: Association and Interaction with Cortisol

R O’Hara 1,2, CM Schröder 1, R Mahadevan 1, AF Schatzberg 1, S Lindley 1,3, S Fox 4,5, M Weiner 4,5, HC Kraemer 1, A Noda 1,2, X Lin 1, HL Gray 1, JF Hallmayer 1
PMCID: PMC2084475  NIHMSID: NIHMS17078  PMID: 17353910

Abstract

The s allele variant of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTT) has recently been observed to moderate the relationship of stress to depression and anxiety. To date no study has considered interactive effects of 5-HTT genotype, stress and HPA function on cognition in healthy, older adults, which may reflect developmental, functional, or neurodegenerative effects of the serotonin transporter polymorphism.

We investigated whether 5-HTT genotype interacts with cumulative life stress and HPA-axis measures of waking and diurnal cortisol slope to impact cognition in 154 non-depressed, older adults. Structural images of hippocampal volume were acquired on a subsample of 56 participants.

The 5-HTT s allele was associated with both significantly lower delayed recall and higher waking cortisol levels. Presence of the s allele interacted with higher waking cortisol to negatively impact memory. We also observed a significant interaction of higher waking cortisol and the s allele on lower hippocampal volume. Smaller hippocampi and higher cortisol were associated with lower delayed recall only in s allele carriers. No impact or interactions of cumulative life stress with 5-HTT or cortisol were observed.

This is the first investigation to identify an association of the 5-HTT s allele with poorer memory function in older adults. The interactive effects of the s allele and waking cortisol levels on reduced hippocampal volume and lower memory suggest that the negative effect of the serotonin polymorphism on memory is mediated by the HPA axis. Further, given the significant association of the s allele with higher waking cortisol in our investigation, future studies may want to evaluate the impact of the serotonin transporter polymorphism on any neuropsychiatric or behavioral outcome which is influenced by HPA axis function in older adults.

Keywords: Serotonin transporter, 5-HTT, Stress, HPA, Memory, Hippocampus, Aging

Introduction

Current research has focused on the role of genetic variations in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) in depressive symptomatology, anxiety, and personality traits.13 This polymorphism is a 44-base pair (bp) allelic insertion (long or l allele) or deletion (short or s allele) variant regulating the transcription of the 5-HTT.4 The s allele is associated with lower levels of serotonin uptake and lower transcriptional efficiency of the serotonin transporter.5

A growing literature implicates the serotonin transporter gene in the development and integrity of neural systems which subserve complex emotional and cognitive processing.6 Serotonergic activity is implicated in cognitive function, and is proposed to interact with the cholinergic system to impact memory and attention.79 Schultz et al.10 observed an association between increased phobic symptoms and lower global cognitive function in older adults, with a trend for this association to be exacerbated in those with the 5-HTT s allele. In healthy, nondepressed adults, Canli et al.11 identified a range of functional and structural regional brain differences between s allele carriers and non-carriers, suggesting 5-HTT genotype affects neural systems controlling affective, motor, and cognitive processes.

In addition to impacting cognition, the serotonergic system is involved in the regulation of Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis function.12, 13 Animal studies indicate serotonin involvement in both activation and feedback control of the HPA axis, as well as a direct effect of serotonin on hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors.14, 15 HPA axis dysfunction, particularly elevated levels of endogenous cortisol, are consistently associated with impairments in cognition.16, 17 Exposure to higher levels of cortisol results in hippocampal damage by inhibiting synaptic transmission, decreasing dendritic branching, and causing neuronal injury and death.1820 These negative effects are proposed as potential physiological mechanisms underlying the documented association of higher cortisol levels with age-related cognitive decline and onset of dementia.2125

Stressful life events have long been hypothesized to impair neuroendocrine function, with elevated cortisol levels associated with acute stress, and impaired diurnal rhythms of cortisol associated with chronic stress.16, 26 Recent studies observe an interactive effect of the s allele variant of 5-HTT and stress on neuropsychiatric outcomes.27, 28 Caspi et al. 27 were the first to report that the 5-HTT genotype moderates the influence of stressful life events on depression in a dose-dependent fashion, with young adults homozygous for the s allele being more sensitive to the depressogenic effect of stressful life events than those homozygous for the l allele. Kendler 28 similarly observed this relationship in a population-based sample of mostly middle-aged adult twins. But not all studies have replicated these findings. In an investigation of over 1000 Australian twins, Gillespie et al.29 found a main effect of stressful life events on major depression but no main or interactive effect of 5-HTT genotype. This investigation differed from those with positive findings by virtue of a broader age range and a more limited reporting period for stressful life events (past 12 months). Other studies have observed gender differences in terms of the interactive effects of 5-HTT s allele and environmental factors on depression, with female s allele carriers being more prone to developing depression and male s allele carriers protected from this disorder.30, 31 Although developmental stage, gender, and timing of stressful life events likely modify any impact of 5-HTT genotype, a significant literature is cumulating to support the perspective that the s allele interacts with stress to negatively impact a variety of behavioral outcomes,32 including depression,8,33, 34 alcohol and drug use35 and psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer’s Disease.36

While many studies have focused on the vulnerability of the 5-HTT s allele to psychological stressors, fewer have investigated the relationship of the 5-HTT gene to physiological markers of stress such as cortisol. We are aware of no study that has considered an interactive effect of 5-HTT genotype and measures of HPA function on cognition.

To address whether 5-HTT genotype interacts with either physiological or psychological stress to impact cognitive function and hippocampal volume, we examined this issue in an ongoing investigation of the relationships among these variables in non-depressed, community dwelling older adults.

Methods

Subjects were 154 community-dwelling older adults participating in an investigation of cognitive decline. They were recruited through advertisements and local senior centers. Recruitment advertisements were general in nature, asking for individuals who were interested in participating in “A Study of Stress and Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults.” All subjects provided informed consent in accordance with Stanford University IRB regulations.

Subjects were 103 females and 51 males, between 60 and 100 years of age (M = 71.13; SD = 8.8), with a mean 16.17 years of education (SD = 2.8). All subjects were Caucasian. An initial evaluation included demographics, self-reported current and past medical status, the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) 37 and Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR. Inclusion criteria were 60 years of age or over, capable of giving informed consent and sufficient visual and auditory acuity for the cognitive testing. Individuals were excluded if they had a MMSE less than 26, a diagnosis of possible or probable dementia, any serious medical illness or any Axis I disorders currently, or within the last two years. Participants were also excluded if they were currently using any systemic corticosteroids, psychotropic medication, short-acting anxiolytics, sedative hypnotics, or medications with significant cholinergic or anticholinergic side effects, or any FDA-approved medications for Alzheimer's disease (AD). Upon entry into the study, whole blood was drawn and transported immediately to the genetics laboratory, and DNA extracted upon arrival. The following measures were assessed, in order.

Cognitive Battery

As part of ongoing assessments at our laboratory, subjects are administered an extensive cognitive battery. For analytic purposes we identify a priori those cognitive domains most pertinent to the issues under investigation. We insure the measures are independent, and reduce those which are significantly correlated to yield one independent measure of the domain of interest. In the current investigation, we selected, a priori, four cognitive domains which we considered most relevant to the issues under investigation. The cognitive domains selected were 1) delayed recall from verbal memory (assessed by the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)38); 2) information processing speed and attention (assessed by the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCW)39), 3) verbal naming (assessed by the Boston Naming Test (BNT)40), and 4) visuospatial ability (Judgement of Line Orientation (JLO)41). Prior work from our and other groups has consistently found lower performance on measures of delayed recall to be associated with increased age, elevated cortisol response, and reduced hippocampal volume.4244 Deficits in the other three domains are common in older adults and are associated with an increased risk of cognitive decline and onset of dementia.45 One outcome measure from each test was selected. For the RAVLT we choose the delayed recall component, i.e. RAVLT 7. For Stroop Color and Word test we employed the time taken to complete the color/word component. Total number answered correctly served as the outcome measure for the Boston Naming Test, and also for the Judgment of Line Orientation. The MMSE37 was included as a brief mental status examination to quantify global cognitive functioning. The selected measures are widely used and have proven reliability (further detail on the cognitive tests is provided in supplementary materials).

Assessment of Mood and Cumulative Life Stress

The Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS)46 is a widely-used depression screening device specifically designed for the elderly.

The Life Stressor Checklist—Revised (LSC-R)47, a 30-item questionnaire, assesses the number of traumatic stressors and negative events occurring over one’s lifetime, the age at which the event occurred, the duration and perceived impact of these event. It was designed as a screen for life events that meet DSM-IV criteria for trauma. Total score for the perceived impact of stressful events across the lifespan was employed (further detail on our assessments of mood and cumulative life stress are provided in supplementary materials).

Cortisol measures

Directly following the cognitive assessment, each subject completed the following cortisol protocol on two consecutive days in their own home. Subjects collected saliva in salivette devices and samples were obtained upon waking, 30 minutes later, and at 1200h, 1700h, and 2100h. Samples were refrigerated immediately after collection. Our prior work comparing actual times recorded by participants at each saliva collection with the actual intervals between collection times recorded by a Medication Event Monitoring Units indicated that older adults were highly compliant with the protocol.48 Full details of our protocol are provided in Kraemer et al..48 Samples were stored at −80C prior to laboratory centrifugation and assays for salivary cortisol using luminescence immunoassay (LIA) reagents provided by Immuno-Biological Laboratories, Inc. Hamburg, Germany. Assay sensitivity was 0.015 μg/dl. Intra-assay variation on three saliva pools of the low, medium, and high controls were averaged 2.78%, 10.45%, and 4.79% respectively. The mean values of the low, medium, and high controls were .054 μg/dl, .228 μg/dl, and .863 μg/dl. The inter-assay coefficients of variation for the low, medium, and high controls were 10.9%, 10.5%, and 5.5%, respectively.

Two measures of cortisol response were identified a priori for investigation in the current study; namely waking cortisol level and diurnal slope. Waking cortisol has been associated with memory deficits,49 and recent work has also suggested that the slope of diurnal cortisol may be an important measure of stress responsiveness.26 Waking cortisol was calculated as the average of the waking cortisol values from each of the two days of collection. The slope of diurnal cortisol variation was calculated with log-transformed regression using the average of each cortisol collection time point from the two days of collection.

Genotyping

DNA was extracted from 200nl of frozen blood using the Qiagen DNeasy Kit (Cat.#69506). Oligonucleotide primers flanking the 5-HTT-linked polymorphic region4 and corresponding to the nucleotide positions −1416 to −1397 (stpr5, 5'-GGC GTT GCC GCT CTG AAT GC) and −910 to −888 (stpr3, 5'-GAG GGA CTG AGC TGG ACA ACC AC) of the 5-HTT gene 5'-flanking regulatory region were used to generate 484bp or 528bp fragments. Polymerase Chain Reaction (PCR) amplification was carried out in a final volume of 30nl consisting of 50ng of genomic DNA, 50ng each of sense and antisense primers, 15nl of Taq PCR Master mix (Qiagen, Cat.#201445), 10% DMSO and 1M Betaine. Annealing was carried out at 61°C for 30s, extension at 72°C for 1 min, and denaturation at 95°C for 30s for a total of 35 cycles. The PCR products were electrophoresed through 5% Polyacrylamide gel (Acrylamide/bis-Acrylamide ratio 19:1) at 120 V for 60 min. A 100bp marker was used to measure the PCR product size for l and s allele. Two independent observers, blind to any information pertaining to the participants, assigned the alleles and genotypes.

Neuroimaging Measures

Neuroimaging was conducted on a subset of our participants. To avoid selection bias, the first 60 subjects enrolled in this investigation were also approached for participation in the neuroimaging component. Fifty-six subjects fulfilled the eligibility criteria for participating in the MRI (e.g. no internal metal, not claustrophobic).

Following the cognitive and cortisol assessments, MRI scans were performed on a GE Signa 1.5 Tesla clinical scanner with a standard GE head coil. The pulse sequences were: (a) Spin-echo, sagittal localizer 20, 3 mm thick slices (acquisition time = 1 min 44 s); (b) Proton density and T2-weighted spin-echo MRI, TR/TE1/TE2 = 5000/30/80 ms, 51 contiguous axial slices (3 mm) covering the entire brain and angulated parallel to the long axis of the hippocampal formation, .976 x .976 mm2 in-plane resolution (acquisition time = 17 min); and (c) Three-dimensional spoiled GRASS (SPGR) MRI of entire brain, TR/TE = 9/2ms, 15o flip angle, perpendicular to the long axis of the hippocampi, .976 x .976 mm2 in-plane resolution, 1.5 mm slice thickness, no skip (acquisition time = 7 min 27 s). The double spin echo scan was axial, parallel to the long axis of the hippocampal formation. The T1/SPGR images were collected in the coronal plane, perpendicular to the long axis of the hippocampus.

Hippocampal volumes were evaluated using a semi-automatic, commercially available, high dimensional brain-mapping tool (Medtronic Surgical Navigation Technologies, Louisville, CO). First a rater manually places 22 reference points as local landmarks around the hippocampus. A landmark is placed at the hippocampal head and at the tail. Then the program divides the length of the hippocampus into five equally-spaced coronal images perpendicular to the long axis of the hippocampus. The rater completes the landmark placement with four landmarks per coronal image; i.e. at the superior, inferior, medial, and lateral boundaries. A fluid image transformation is used to register the individual MRI scans to a reference anatomy template which has the hippocampus carefully labeled. Pixels corresponding to the hippocampus are counted to obtain volume measurements. This method has been independently validated by comparing it with manual hippocampal tracing in AD patients and controls.50, 51 Manual and semi-automated hippocampal volume measurements generally correlated better than 0.952 but semi-automated methods depend less on rater judgment, reducing potential rater bias. To adjust for differences in head size, all hippocampal volumes were normalized to total intracranial volume (TIV) by dividing total hippocampal volume by TIV. TIV was identified using a mask containing the brain as identified on the T2-weighted image, as described previously.53

Statistical Analysis

We employed a multiple regression analysis to test the impact of 5-HTT, HPA function, and life stress on cognition. 5-HTT status, waking cortisol, slope of diurnal cortisol, cumulative life stress, and all possible interactions served as independent variables, with performance on the four cognitive measures serving as the dependent measures. None of the outcome measures were significantly correlated, thus we considered these independent tests for the purposes of analyses. Since interactive effects were included, all independent variables were centered at the mean to insure interpretability of the regression coefficients. Further discussion of centering in regression analysis can be found in Kraemer and Blasey.54 The distributions of the raw waking cortisol values and log-transformations for these values were sufficiently skewed that it was not clear they would fulfill the linearity assumptions necessary for conducting a multiple regression analysis. We divided the cortisol values into a 5-point scale giving 1 to those in the lowest quintile, 2 to those in the second quintile, etc., bringing the linearity assumptions closer in line with those required for employing a parametric procedure, yielding the same effect whether raw or log-transformed values for cortisol are employed. Residual analyses were conducted as the diagnostic procedure to ascertain whether there were any deviations from either the linearity or the equal variance assumptions central to linear regression. No compromises were detected. Kruskal-Wallis analysis was used to compare the three genotypes with respect to HPA function. The same analytic approach was employed to consider the impact of 5-HTT genotype and cortisol on hippocampal volume in the 56 subjects who participated in the neuroimaging component of the study. All statistical tests were conducted at the two-tailed 5% level. Only 8 of the 56 subjects who underwent neuroimaging were homozygous for the s allele. This is not a sufficiently large sample to yield enough power to meaningfully consider the 5-HTT genotype groups separately for the neuroimaging component. Thus, in this analysis, s allele carriers were compared with l allele homozygotes. In secondary analyses, we included depressive symptomatology and all interactions in the multiple regression model. We also employed the same regression model using additional imaging measures as secondary outcome measures. These included Total Intracranial Volume, Total Gray Matter Volume, Total White Matter Volume, and CSF.

Results

Table 1 displays the distribution of 5-HTT genotype. The frequencies of the s and the l allele are .442 and .558, respectively, which is in line with frequencies observed in other Caucasian samples.55 Genotypes do not deviate from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium.

Table 1.

5-HTT Genotype Distribution and Mean Values for Demographics and Predictor Variables

ll (n=45) ls (n=82) ss (n=27) P Values

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p

Age, y 70.1 (9.09) 71.9 (8.9) 70.4 (8.2) .38
Years of Education, y 16.3 (3.6) 16.0 (2.44) 16.4 (2.82) .67
GDS 6.2 (6.0) 6.8 (5.2) 6.3 (6.0) .42
Life Stress 11.9 (10.36) 9.7 (7.21) 11.7 (10.04) .55
Waking Cortisol, μg/dl 0.49 (0.24) 0.64 (0.39) 0.72 (0.35) .01
Cortisol Slope −0.12 (0.07) −0.13 (0.07) −0.15 (0.05) .20

Abbreviations: l, long allele; s, short allele; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale.

No significant differences were observed with respect to the 5-HTT genotype groups for any of the demographic variables. Kruskal-Wallis analysis revealed a significant difference among the 3 allele groups with respect to waking cortisol, but not slope of diurnal cortisol, with individuals with the s allele having significantly higher waking cortisol values (see Figure 1 and Table 1). No differences were observed among the allele groups on the GDS or the LSC-R.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Main effect of 5-HTT genotype on average waking cortisol (ng/dl).

Abbreviations: 5-HTT, serotonin transporter gene; s, short allele; l, long allele.

Multiple regression analysis revealed a statistically significant association of 5-HTT genotype with lower delayed recall scores on the RAVLT 7 (t= −3.00; p<0.003), with s allele carriers having significantly lower memory scores (see Figure 2 and Table 2). The mean delayed recall performance of those homozygous for the s allele was two-thirds of a standard deviation below the performance of those homozygous for the l allele. Also, the ss genotype group performed approximately one standard deviation lower than age and education appropriate normative values for the RAVLT 7 in the literature.56 Performance greater or equal to one to one and a half standard deviations below age and education-matched normative values on a cognitive test can be indicative of impairment.57 There was a main effect of waking cortisol (t= −2.47; p<0.01) and slope of diurnal cortisol (t= −3.27; p<0.001) with higher waking cortisol and steeper diurnal rhythms associated with lower delayed recall. 5-HTT genotype interacted with waking cortisol, but not slope of diurnal cortisol, on memory (t=−1.98; p<.05). Elevated waking cortisol was most strongly associated with lower memory in s allele carriers. Pearson product moment correlations between waking cortisol and delayed recall were r=.12, r=−.21, and r= −.36, for the ll, ls, and ss groups respectively (Figure 3). No main effect or interactions of the LSC-R with 5-HTT or cortisol were observed, and there were no associations of our predictor variables with any of the additional cognitive measures. Although a list learning measure of delayed recall from episodic memory and the Wechsler Memory Scale-II were not included in this study since we had identified the RAVLT 7 to serve as our primary measure of delayed recall, poorer performance on the delayed recall components of these tests were similarly negatively associated with the s allele and the s allele interacted with higher waking cortisol to result in lower performance on these tasks (data not shown).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Main effect of 5-HTT genotype on delayed recall performance as assessed by the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test- Delayed Recall.38

Abbreviations: 5-HTT, serotonin transporter gene; s, short allele; l, long allele.

Table 2.

Means and Standard Deviations for Cognitive Tests by 5-HTT Allele Group

ll (n=45) ls (n=82) ss (n=27) P Values

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p

RAVLT Delayed 10.1 (3.68) 8.8 (4.29) 7.1 (4.08) .003
Recall
SCW 145.45 (43.36) 155.96 (42.38) 166.741 (96.24) .30
JLO 24.1 (3.82) 23.6 (3.73) 24.3 (3.88) .51
BNT 56.7 (3.94) 56.5 (3.94) 57.3 (3.89) .35

Abbreviations: 5-HTT, serotonin transporter; l, long allele; s, short allele; RAVLT, Rey Verbal Learning Test; SCW, Stroop Color and Word; JLO, Judgement of Line Orientation; Boston Naming Test.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Association of waking cortisol and delayed recall performance as assessed by the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test 38 as a function of 5-HTT genotype (ll vs. ls vs. ss).

Abbreviations: 5-HTT, serotonin transporter gene; s, short allele; l, long allele.

With respect to hippocampal volume, there was no main effect of 5-HTT s allele on hippocampal volume. There was a significant main effect of waking cortisol (t= −2.91; p<.01), with higher levels associated with lower hippocampal volumes. A significant interaction of the s allele and higher waking cortisol on lower hippocampal volume (t= −1.97; p<.05) was observed. This relationship was stronger among s allele carriers than non-carriers, with Pearson product moment correlations of r= −.49, and r= −.25, respectively (Figure 4). In an additional multiple regression with the RAVLT 7 as the outcome measure, lower hippocampal volume and higher waking cortisol were associated with lower delayed recall performance only in those with the s allele (t= −1.99; p<.05).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Association of waking cortisol and hippocampal volume as determined by structural MRI (total hippocampal volume corrected for total intracranial volume) as a function of 5-HTT genotype (ll vs. ls/ss).

In secondary analyses, we included the GDS in the original multiple regression model and observed no interactive effect of depressive symptomatology, with 5-HTT genotype, stress, and cortisol on any cognitive measure. There were no differences in our findings when we covaried for age, gender or household income. Only 5 participants smoked, and only 17 consumed more than 2 drinks a week. Neither current nor past history of smoking or drinking impacted any of the observed relationships. Using the same multiple regression model approach, we examined for any contribution of the subcomponents of the LSC-R, including type of stressful event, age at which the event occurred and its associated impact. No significant independent or interactive effects were observed for any of these variables. We observed no significant impact or interaction of 5-HTT genotype, cortisol and cumulative life stress on any of the secondary neuroimaging measures (Table 3 and 4).

Table 3.

Mean Values for structural MRI variables dependent on s allele carrier status.

ll (n=16) ls and ss (n=40) P Values

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p

Total Hippocampal Volume (cc) 4.356(.587) 4.316 (.571) .82
Total Intracranial Volume (cc) 1414.312 (110.031) 1387.135 (128.985) .46
Gray Matter Volume (cc) 628.311 (52.408) 628.159 (66.176) .99
White Matter Volume (cc) 427.780 (35.581) 424.417 (49.415) .81
Cerebrospinal Fluid (cc) 286.106 (48.260) 271.358 (50.128) .34

Abbreviations: l, long allele; s, short allele; MRI, Magnetic Resonance Imaging.

Table 4.

Multiple Regression with RAVLT Delayed Recall as the Dependent Variable

Coefficient Std. Error t p
Waking Cortisol −0.699 0.282 −2.48 0.01
Cortisol Slope −20.565 6.285 −3.27 0.001
5-HTT genotype −1.910 0.637 −3.00 0.003
Life Stress 0.012 0.048 0.24 0.81
Waking Cortisol X Cortisol Slope −3.977 4.056 −0.98 0.33
Waking Cortisol X 5-HTT genotype −0.937 0.474 −1.98 0.05
Waking Cortisol X Life Stress −0.041 0.038 −1.07 0.29

Abbreviations: RAVLT: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; 5-HTT, serotonin transporter.

Discussion

In the current study, the 5-HTT s allele was significantly associated with lower memory performance in non-depressed, community-dwelling older adults. The poorest memory was exhibited by subjects homozygous for the s allele. Contrary to prior observations of interactive effects of psychosocial stress and 5-HTT on neurobehavioral outcomes,27 we did not observe an interaction of 5-HTT and life stress on any cognitive domain. However, the 5-HTT s allele was associated with higher levels of waking cortisol, a physiological marker of stress. In addition to an independent association with both lower memory and higher waking cortisol, the s allele interacted with higher cortisol levels to result in lower delayed recall performance. Although we observed no impact of the 5-HTT s allele on hippocampal volume, higher waking cortisol significantly interacted with the s allele on lower hippocampal volumes. Higher waking cortisol and lower hippocampal volumes were associated with lower memory performance only among s allele carriers. Taken together, these findings go beyond the current perspective that the s allele variant of 5-HTT increases the risk for neuropsychiatric disorders in the presence of stress by implicating the 5-HTT s allele in cognitive function, specifically poorer delayed recall performance.

We are not the first to suggest a role for 5-HTT genotype in cognitive processing. In their fMRI investigation of healthy, non-depressed adults, Canli et al.11 observed a range of functional and structural regional brain differences between s allele carriers and non-carriers during an emotional stroop paradigm leading the authors to suggest that 5-HTT genotype affects neural systems controlling affective, cognitive, and motor processes. Yet, they observed no performance differences according to s allele status on any condition of the emotional Stroop task, which draws on both affective and non-affective cognitive processes. Similarly, Hariri et al.58 found in an fMRI investigation of healthy normal volunteers that s allele carriers and l allele homozygotes did not differ with respect to either reaction time or accuracy on an “n-back task”, measuring working memory and attention. Also, performance was not associated with any brain regional activation differences according to genotype group.58 The performance findings from these studies are consistent with our observed lack of association of 5-HTT genotype with the Stroop measure of attention, but contrary to our investigation they did not consider measures of delayed recall, and their findings regarding attention were secondary to a primary focus on emotion processing. Our observation of a significant negative impact of the 5-HTTLPR s allele on delayed recall and the observed interaction of waking cortisol and the s allele on hippocampal volume speaks to a potentially important role of serotonin function in memory processing in older adults.

The involvement of 5-HTT genotype in modulating serotonin neuronal function is complex. The s allele has been associated with less transcriptional efficiency, reduced serotonin reuptake and less 5-HTT binding in the brain. However, recent neuroimaging assessments of 5-HTT binding potential observe no differences between l allele homozygotes and s allele carriers, producing a less consistent picture of 5HTT function in the in vivo brain.59 5-HTT knockout mice exhibit impaired clearance and marked elevation of extracellular levels of 5-HT in forebrain and hippocampal regions.60, 61 While 5-HT is crucial for brain development, excess extracellular 5-HT has deleterious effects on neurogenesis and synaptogenesis, preventing or partially interfering with the development of the cortical projection areas during critical periods of brain development.6264 Lesch et al.5 propose that this mechanism paves the way for impaired synaptic plasticity in adulthood, and sets the stage for later brain dysfunction. Others argue that s allele status confers increased vulnerability to a range of behavioral symptoms secondary to age-related neurodegenerative processes, particularly in response to stress.65 Thus, the phenotype associated with the 5-HTT s/l alleles may result from developmental, functional, or neurodegenerative effects of the serotonin transporter polymorphism or a combination of these effects.

The higher cortisol levels and lower memory function observed in the s allele carriers in our study may independently reflect 5-HTT-associated impairments in neuronal development. If impairments in neuronal development were to contribute to smaller hippocampal volume this in turn could contribute independently or sequentially to HPA dysregulation and lower memory function. However, we did not observe a main effect of the s allele on hippocampal volume but we did observe a significant interaction of the s allele and higher cortisol on lower hippocampal volume. Thus, another interpretation is that neuronal development in s allele carriers increases vulnerability to dysregulation of HPA function with a subsequent negative effect on hippocampal volume and memory. Alternatively, given the overall correlation of elevated waking cortisol and lower memory function and reduced hippocampal volume, s allele carrier status may simply increase vulnerability to negative effects of higher cortisol levels on hippocampal volume and function, independent of the source of HPA dysregulation.

The serotonergic system is implicated in hippocampal regulation of HPA axis activity,66 increasing hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, specifically during certain developmental periods.67 Developmental modification of the ascending serotonin system has been suggested to result in long-term dysregulation of HPA function.12 Hippocampal regions are vulnerable to neurotoxic effects of dysregulation of the HPA system, specifically chronic elevated levels of glucocorticoids. Sapolsky et al.25 proposed that the hippocampal damage caused by elevated cortisol levels alters the normal inhibitory control of corticosteroids on the hippocampal control of HPA activity, resulting in a forward cascade in which the elevated cortisol levels it causes further damage the hippocampus. The negative impact of elevated glucocorticoids on hippocampal formation is implicated in cognitive impairment,2225, 68 and our observed negative association of higher waking cortisol with memory performance is consistent with prior observations.17, 42 Fewer studies have examined the relationship between HPA function and hippocampal volume in normal, older adults, and the variable findings may reflect the fact that moderating factors such as 5-HTT genotype were not considered.16, 42, 69

Andrews et al.12 suggest that interactions between serotonin and glucocorticoids are likely bidirectional, thus any dysregulation of HPA function may also have a forward cascade effect on serotonin function. For example, prenatal exposure to glucocorticoids appear to influence serotonin production70 and also 5-HTT expression.12 Such effects may be exacerbated in those with the s allele. To date, a limited number of human studies have directly considered the impact of 5-HTT genotype on measures of HPA function. These investigations found no association of 5-HTT genotype with plasma cortisol,65, 71 although Smith et al.65 observed a trend towards blunted cortisol and prolactin response to acute pharmacologic increase of serotonin in those with the s allele. Most of these investigations did not examine basal levels, and were conducted on small samples of younger adults, with limited power to test genotype differences. However, in one of the few large studies conducted, involving 139 middle-aged, adults, baseline plasma prolactin levels were found to be lower in s allele homozygotes.72 Animal models have yielded mixed findings with some observing blunted neuroendocrine response, and others observing increased neuroendocrine response to stress in genetic variations hypothesized to be similar to s allele status.7376 The animal findings are also mixed regarding basal levels of cortisol.

In our study, the association of s allele status with higher waking cortisol levels did not appear to reflect cumulative life stress. Indeed, cumulative life stress, alone or interaction with 5-HTT genotype and cortisol, was not associated with any outcome. Given the likely developmental effects of the 5-HTTLPR on neural circuitry mediating stress reactivity and growing evidence that early life trauma plays a key role in modulating stress response on behavioral outcomes and hippocampal volume,27, 77, 78 we examined individual items of the LSC-R, including the age at which the event occurred. The components of the LSC-R cover many of the same stressful life events assessed by other investigations of the relationship of 5-HTT genotype and stress on behavioral outcomes.2729 Only seven of the subcomponents of the LSC-R emphasized stressful events during childhood (see supplementary materials for complete details) but we found no item to be associated with cognition alone or in interaction with 5-HTT genotype or HPA measures. The impact of cumulative life stress was moderate across all three genotype groups, and may not have been sufficient to reflect vulnerability of the s allele to such stressors. HPA axis measures may be more sensitive indicators of mild stress. Indeed, the observed dose-dependent association of the s allele with higher waking cortisol values may account in part for previously observed vulnerability of this allele to a variety of neuropsychiatric outcomes, such as anxiety or depression, in response to stress.27

It is also possible that our retrospective self-report measure underestimated the level of life stress, especially in older adults who have recall difficulties. Further, distal traumatic events may be less relevant to older adults than chronic, ongoing daily hassles, and level of social support. Yet, Gillespie et al.,29 in one of the few investigations to include older adults, observed no interactive effect of recent stressful events and 5-HTT genotype, albeit on depression. These authors did observe stressful events to be associated with increased depression, while we observed no impact of cumulative life stress on cognitive function. Our sample was healthy and well-educated, factors which may have modified any negative effects of stress exposure. For example, Manuck et al.72 observed increased prolactin response to fenfluramine challenge in s allele carriers who had lower socioeconomic status. With a mean annual household income of $50k, few of our participants had low socioeconomic status. The 5HTT s allele has been negatively associated with smoking and drinking behaviors,79, 80 but findings are considerably mixed.81, 82 Increased smoking is also documented to affect HPA function.83 In our study, neither current nor past history of smoking or drinking, nor household income impacted our findings. Our observed interaction between 5-HTT genotype and higher cortisol levels on hippocampal volume and memory is all the more striking given that our sample was healthy, well-educated, and had a high mean level of education.

Another perspective is that the current findings reflect increased vulnerability in s allele carrier status to neurodegenerative processes rather than 5HTT-associated impairments in neurodevelopment. Smith et al.65 suggest that 5-HTT genotype may interact with normal aging processes, in terms of reduced capacity for older adults with the s allele to adapt to age-related alterations in serotonin function, with the resulting emergence of behavioral symptoms, particularly secondary to neurodegenerative diseases. Certainly, AD is associated with an extensive serotonergic deficit.84 This has led investigators to consider the prevalence of the 5-HTT s allele in AD patients. Studies have been inconclusive, with some observing the s/s genotype to be more prevalent in late-onset AD85, 86 and other larger studies observing no association.87, 88 However, an increasing number of investigations suggest that diminished serotonergic function may contribute to cognitive impairment in both demented and non-demented patients.8991 Animal studies implicate reduced reuptake and excess extracellular 5-HT in hippocampal dendritic atrophy.23 Thus, the impaired transcriptional efficiency of the 5-HTT s allele may contribute to or exacerbate the cognitive impairment and reduced hippocampal volume that reflects ongoing age-related changes in neuronal function in the elderly. The observed elevations in cortisol in our investigation may also independently reflect age-related neurophysiological changes in this cohort. However, the relationship of 5-HTT genotype and cortisol to brain structure and function did not covary with age in our study. Future studies are required to investigate whether our findings might generalize to younger or middle-aged adults.

Another consideration is that any negative impact of less effective serotonergic function on memory in older adults may reflect the lack of any benefit of serotonin on mood. None of our subjects had major depression. Yet even mild levels of depressive symptoms are associated with cognitive impairment,92 and hyperactivity of the HPA axis is implicated in depression.93 When we included level of depressive symptomatology, assessed by the Geriatric Depression Scale, in the model in secondary analyses we observed no impact of level of depressive symptomatology on the interaction of 5-HTT s allele and cortisol to memory. Thus, any association of the 5-HTT s allele to lower memory performance did not appear to reflect increased levels of even mild depression. However, given that the GDS is a screening instrument and the range of values for the sample under investigation was relatively narrow, this measure may not have captured more subtle depressive symptoms. The lack of any impact of depressive symptomatology on the interaction of 5-HTT s allele and cortisol to memory must be interpreted cautiously. Future studies should consider measures that better characterize mild depressive symptoms, which may not fulfill the criteria for Axis I depressive disorder but may still mediate the relationship between 5-HTT genotype and memory function.

There are several other limitations which constrain the interpretations that can be drawn from our study. The cross-sectional design limits the extent to which we can identify developmental or mediational relationships among 5-HTT genotype, HPA axis function and cognitive function and brain structure. Also, we focused only on hippocampal volume, and did not examine other brain regions related to stress and cognition, such as the amygdala, frontal and pre-frontal regions. Additionally, the moderate levels of life stress reported by our participants may have restricted the extent to which we could examine the 5-HTT stress-vulnerability hypotheses. Finally, although the 5-HTTLPR was first reported to consist of s and l alleles, Hu et al94 reported a single nucleotide polymorphism which gives rise to two functional variants of the L allele. The LG variant, occurs in approximately 6.5% of Caucasian Americans95 and is associated with comparable levels of serotonin transporter expression levels to the s allele.94 Presence of the LG variant among the l/s genotype group may have accounted for some of the observed variability between waking cortisol and delayed recall scores in that group. Future studies with sufficient power to investigate the trialleleic subgroups should examine this possibility. Finally, all subjects were Caucasian and our findings may not be generalizable to non-Caucasian populations. Further investigations of this issue need to be conducted on other ethnic groups.

In conclusion, the current study finds the 5-HTT s allele to be a marker for poorer memory and higher wakening cortisol in older adults. The significant negative interaction of the 5-HTT s allele and cortisol on both lower memory and hippocampal volume may reflect independent or combined neurodevelopmental, functional, or neurodegenerative effects of the serotonin transporter polymorphism. A clearer understanding of the involvement of the 5-HT system in contributing to HPA activity, hippocampal and cognitive changes in older adults, including increased understanding of the mediational pathways among these factors, could point to new therapeutic approaches for ameliorating memory impairment in older adults who may be at increased risk for memory decline conferred by 5-HTT s allele carrier status. Longitudinal studies are required to more fully elucidate the potential complex relationships among these factors. Future investigations should also consider whether the serotonin polymorphism impacts any neuropsychiatric or behavioral outcome or function, which is influenced by HPA axis function, independently or secondary to stress.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health grants AG 18784; AG 17824; and MH70886, and the Department of Veteran Affairs, Sierra-Pacific Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center (MIRECC).

References

  • 1.Bondy B, Erfurth A, de Jonge S, Kruger M, Meyer H. Possible association of the short allele of the serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) with violent suicide. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5:193–5. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–31. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Steffens DC, Svenson I, Marchuk DA, Levy RM, Hays JC, Flint EP, et al. Allelic differences in the serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region in geriatric depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002;10:185–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Heils A, Teufel A, Petri S, Stober G, Riederer P, Bengel D, et al. Allelic variation of human serotonin transporter gene expression. J Neurochem. 1996;66:2621–4. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1996.66062621.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lesch KP, Gutknecht L. Pharmacogenetics of the serotonin transporter. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2005;29:1062–73. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hariri AR, Holmes A. Genetics of emotional regulation: the role of the serotonin transporter in neural function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:182–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Buhot MC, Martin S, Segu L. Role of serotonin in memory impairment. Ann Med. 2000;32:210–21. doi: 10.3109/07853890008998828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cassel JC, Jeltsch H. Serotonergic modulation of cholinergic function in the central nervous system: cognitive implications. Neuroscience. 1995;69:1–41. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00241-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Garcia-Alloza M, Gil-Bea FJ, Diez-Ariza M, Chen CP, Francis PT, Lasheras B, et al. Cholinergic-serotonergic imbalance contributes to cognitive and behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:442–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Schultz SK, Moser DJ, Bishop JR, Ellingrod VL. Phobic anxiety in late-life in relationship to cognition and 5HTTLPR polymorphism. Psychiatr Genet. 2005;15:305–6. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200512000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Canli T, Omura K, Haas BW, Fallgatter A, Constable RT, Lesch KP. Beyond affect: a role for genetic variation of the serotonin transporter in neural activation during a cognitive attention task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:12224–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503880102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Andrews MH, Matthews SG. Programming of the hypothalamopituitary-adrenal axis: serotonergic involvement. Stress. 2004;7:15–27. doi: 10.1080/10253890310001650277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Taylor MM, Samson WK. A possible mechanism for the action of adrenomedullin in brain to stimulate stress hormone secretion. Endocrinology. 2004;145:4890–6. doi: 10.1210/en.2004-0806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lowry CA. Functional subsets of serotonergic neurones: implications for control of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. J Neuroendocrinol. 2002;14:911–23. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2002.00861.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mitchell JB, Iny LJ, Meaney MJ. The role of serotonin in the development and environmental regulation of type II corticosteroid receptor binding in rat hippocampus. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1990;55:231–5. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(90)90204-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lupien SJ, Fiocco A, Wan N, Maheu F, Lord C, Schramek T, et al. Stress hormones and human memory function across the lifespan. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:225–42. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Seeman TE, McEwen BS, Singer BH, Albert MS, Rowe JW. Increase in urinary cortisol excretion and memory declines: MacArthur studies of successful aging. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1997;82:2458–65. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.8.4173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Brown ES, D JW, Frol A, Bobadilla L, Khan DA, Hanczyc M, et al. Hippocampal volume, spectroscopy, cognition, and mood in patients receiving corticosteroid therapy. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:538–45. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Uno H, Tarara R, Else JG, Suleman MA, Sapolsky RM. Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged and fatal stress in primates. J Neurosci. 1989;9:1705–11. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-05-01705.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Woolley CS, Gould E, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS. Naturally occurring fluctuation in dendritic spine density on adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 1990;10:4035–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-12-04035.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Karlamangla AS, Singer BH, Chodosh J, McEwen BS, Seeman TE. Urinary cortisol excretion as a predictor of incident cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26 (Suppl 1):80–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators: allostasis and allostatic load. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1998;338:171–179. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801153380307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.McEwen BS. Stress and the aging hippocampus. Front Neuroendocrinol. 1999;20:49–70. doi: 10.1006/frne.1998.0173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. Prolonged glucocorticoid exposure reduces hippocampal neuron number: implications for aging. J Neurosci. 1985;5:1222–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-05-01222.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. The neuroendocrinology of stress and aging: the glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis. Endocr Rev. 1986;7:284–301. doi: 10.1210/edrv-7-3-284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sephton SE, Sapolsky RM, Kraemer HC, Spiegel D. Diurnal cortisol rhythm as a predictor of breast cancer survival. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2000;92:994–1000. doi: 10.1093/jnci/92.12.994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kendler KS, Kuhn JW, Vittum J, Prescott CA, Riley B. The interaction of stressful life events and a serotonin transporter polymorphism in the prediction of episodes of major depression: a replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:529–35. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gillespie NA, Whitfield JB, Williams B, Heath AC, Martin NG. The relationship between stressful life events, the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and major depression. Psychol Med. 2005;35:101–11. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704002727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mizuno T, Aoki M, Shimada Y, Inoue M, Nakaya K, Takahashi T, et al. Gender difference in association between polymorphism of serotonin transporter gene regulatory region and anxiety. J Psychosom Res. 2006;60:91–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sjoberg RL, Nilsson KW, Nordquist N, Ohrvik J, Leppert J, Lindstrom L, et al. Development of depression: sex and the interaction between environment and a promoter polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006;9:443–9. doi: 10.1017/S1461145705005936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Vergne DE, Nemeroff CB. The interaction of serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and early adverse life events on vulnerability for major depression. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2006;8:452–7. doi: 10.1007/s11920-006-0050-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jacobs N, Kenis G, Peeters F, Derom C, Vlietinck R, van Os J. Stress-related negative affectivity and genetically altered serotonin transporter function: evidence of synergism in shaping risk of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:989–96. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.9.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Taylor SE, Way BM, Welch WT, Hilmert CJ, Lehman BJ, Eisenberger NI. Early family environment, current adversity, the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism, and depressive symptomatology. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:671–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Covault J, Tennen H, Armeli S, Conner TS, Herman AI, Cillessen AH, et al. Interactive Effects of the Serotonin Transporter 5-HTTLPR Polymorphism and Stressful Life Events on College Student Drinking and Drug Use. Biol Psychiatry. 2006 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Borroni B, Grassi M, Agosti C, Archetti S, Costanzi C, Cornali C, et al. Cumulative effect of COMT and 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms and their interaction with disease severity and comorbidities on the risk of psychosis in Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:343–51. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000192491.50802.c3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state" A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Golden CJ, Hammeke TA, Purisch AD. Diagnostic validity of a standardized neuropsychological battery derived from Luria's neuropsychological tests. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1978;46:1258–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. Boston (MA): Kaplan & Goodglass; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Benton AL, Hamsher K, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lupien SJ, de Leon M, de Santi S, Convit A, Tarshish C, Nair NP, et al. Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:69–73. doi: 10.1038/271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Mortimer JA, Gosche KM, Riley KP, Markesbery WR, Snowdon DA. Delayed recall, hippocampal volume and Alzheimer neuropathology: findings from the Nun Study. Neurology. 2004;62:428–32. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000106463.66966.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.O'Hara R, Yesavage JA, Kraemer HC, Mauricio M, Friedman LF, Murphy GM., Jr The APOE epsilon4 allele is associated with decline on delayed recall performance in community-dwelling older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1998;46:1493–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1998.tb01532.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Craik FI, Salthouse TAE. The handbool of aging and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Yesavage JA, Brink T, Rose T, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1983;17:37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wolfe JW, Kimerling R, Brown PJ, Chrestman KR, Levin K. Psychometric review of The Life Stressor Checklist-Revised. In: Stamm BH, editor. Measurement of Stress, Trauma, and Adaptation. Lutherville, MD: Sidran Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kraemer HC, Giese-Davis J, Yutsis M, Neri E, Gallagher-Thompson D, Taylor CB, et al. Design decisions to optimize reliability of daytime cortisol slopes in an older population. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:325–33. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000201816.26786.5b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Greendale GA, Kritz-Silverstein D, Seeman T, Barrett-Connor E. Higher basal cortisol predicts verbal memory loss in postmenopausal women: Rancho Bernardo Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:1655–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb03878.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Csernansky JG, Joshi S, Wang L, Haller JW, Gado M, Miller JP, et al. Hippocampal morphometry in schizophrenia by high dimensional brain mapping. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:11406–11. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.19.11406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Haller JW, Christensen GE, Joshi SC, Newcomer JW, Miller MI, Csernansky JG, et al. Hippocampal MR imaging morphometry by means of general pattern matching. Radiology. 1996;199:787–91. doi: 10.1148/radiology.199.3.8638006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Hsu YY, Schuff N, Du AT, Mark K, Zhu X, Hardin D, et al. Comparison of automated and manual MRI volumetry of hippocampus in normal aging and dementia. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2002;16:305–10. doi: 10.1002/jmri.10163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Tanabe JL, Amend D, Schuff N, DiSclafani V, Ezekiel F, Norman D, et al. Tissue segmentation of the brain in Alzheimer disease. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1997;18:115–23. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kraemer HC, Blasey CM. Centring in regression analyses: a strategy to prevent errors in statistical inference. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2004;13:141–51. doi: 10.1002/mpr.170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cho HJ, Meira-Lima I, Cordeiro Q, Michelon L, Sham P, Vallada H, et al. Population-based and family-based studies on the serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and bipolar disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:771–81. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.La Rue A. Aging and neuropsychological assessment. New York: Plenum Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F, Goldman D, et al. Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science. 2002;297:400–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1071829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Parsey RV, Hastings RS, Oquendo MA, Huang YY, Simpson N, Arcement J, et al. Lower serotonin transporter binding potential in the human brain during major depressive episodes. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:52–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Mathews TA, Fedele DE, Coppelli FM, Avila AM, Murphy DL, Andrews AM. Gene dose-dependent alterations in extraneuronal serotonin but not dopamine in mice with reduced serotonin transporter expression. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;140:169–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2004.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Montanez S, Owens WA, Gould GG, Murphy DL, Daws LC. Exaggerated effect of fluvoxamine in heterozygote serotonin transporter knockout mice. J Neurochem. 2003;86:210–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.01836.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Bengel D, Murphy DL, Andrews AM, Wichems CH, Feltner D, Heils A, et al. Altered brain serotonin homeostasis and locomotor insensitivity to 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine ("Ecstasy") in serotonin transporter-deficient mice. Mol Pharmacol. 1998;53:649–55. doi: 10.1124/mol.53.4.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Cases O, Vitalis T, Seif I, De Maeyer E, Sotelo C, Gaspar P. Lack of barrels in the somatosensory cortex of monoamine oxidase A-deficient mice: role of a serotonin excess during the critical period. Neuron. 1996;16:297–307. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80048-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Esaki T, Cook M, Shimoji K, Murphy DL, Sokoloff L, Holmes A. Developmental disruption of serotonin transporter function impairs cerebral responses to whisker stimulation in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:5582–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501509102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Smith GS, Lotrich FE, Malhotra AK, Lee AT, Ma Y, Kramer E, et al. Effects of serotonin transporter promoter polymorphisms on serotonin function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:2226–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Andrews MH, Kostaki A, Setiawan E, McCabe L, Owen D, Banjanin S, et al. Developmental regulation of the 5-HT7 serotonin receptor and transcription factor NGFI-A in the fetal guinea-pig limbic system: influence of GCs. J Physiol. 2004;555:659–70. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.056705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Meaney MJ, Diorio J, Francis D, Weaver S, Yau J, Chapman K, et al. Postnatal handling increases the expression of cAMP-inducible transcription factors in the rat hippocampus: the effects of thyroid hormones and serotonin. J Neurosci. 2000;20:3926–35. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-10-03926.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.de Leon MJ, McRae T, Rusinek H, Convit A, De Santi S, Tarshish C, et al. Cortisol reduces hippocampal glucose metabolism in normal elderly, but not in Alzheimer's disease. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1997;82:3251–9. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.10.4305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.MacLullich AM, Deary IJ, Starr JM, Ferguson KJ, Wardlaw JM, Seckl JR. Plasma cortisol levels, brain volumes and cognition in healthy elderly men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:505–15. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Muneoka K, Mikuni M, Ogawa T, Kitera K, Kamei K, Takigawa M, et al. Prenatal dexamethasone exposure alters brain monoamine metabolism and adrenocortical response in rat offspring. Am J Physiol. 1997;273:R1669–75. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1997.273.5.R1669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Wand GS, McCaul M, Yang X, Reynolds J, Gotjen D, Lee S, et al. The mu-opioid receptor gene polymorphism (A118G) alters HPA axis activation induced by opioid receptor blockade. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:106–14. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00294-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Manuck SB, Flory JD, Ferrell RE, Muldoon MF. Socio-economic status covaries with central nervous system serotonergic responsivity as a function of allelic variation in the serotonin transporter gene-linked polymorphic region. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:651–68. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00094-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Barr CS, Newman TK, Schwandt M, Shannon C, Dvoskin RL, Lindell SG, et al. Sexual dichotomy of an interaction between early adversity and the serotonin transporter gene promoter variant in rhesus macaques. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:12358–63. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403763101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Barr CS, Newman TK, Shannon C, Parker C, Dvoskin RL, Becker ML, et al. Rearing condition and rh5-HTTLPR interact to influence limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis response to stress in infant macaques. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:733–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Gross C, Santarelli L, Brunner D, Zhuang X, Hen R. Altered fear circuits in 5-HT(1A) receptor KO mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48:1157–63. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01041-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Tjurmina OA, Armando I, Saavedra JM, Goldstein DS, Murphy DL. Exaggerated adrenomedullary response to immobilization in mice with targeted disruption of the serotonin transporter gene. Endocrinology. 2002;143:4520–6. doi: 10.1210/en.2002-220416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Heim C, Nemeroff CB. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:1023–39. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01157-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Stein MB, Koverola C, Hanna C, Torchia MG, McClarty B. Hippocampal volume in women victimized by childhood sexual abuse. Psychol Med. 1997;27:951–9. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Hu S, Brody CL, Fisher C, Gunzerath L, Nelson ML, Sabol SZ, et al. Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene and neuroticism in cigarette smoking behavior. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5:181–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Munafo MR, Lingford-Hughes AR, Johnstone EC, Walton RT. Association between the serotonin transporter gene and alcohol consumption in social drinkers. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;135:10–4. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Hopfer CJ, Timberlake D, Haberstick B, Lessem JM, Ehringer MA, Smolen A, et al. Genetic influences on quantity of alcohol consumed by adolescents and young adults. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;78:187–93. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Kremer I, Bachner-Melman R, Reshef A, Broude L, Nemanov L, Gritsenko I, et al. Association of the serotonin transporter gene with smoking behavior. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:924–30. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Rasmusson AM, Wu R, Paliwal P, Anderson GM, Krishnan-Sarin S. A decrease in the plasma DHEA to cortisol ratio during smoking abstinence may predict relapse: a preliminary study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;186:473–80. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0367-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Lanctot KL, Herrmann N, Eryavec G, van Reekum R, Reed K, Naranjo CA. Central serotonergic activity is related to the aggressive behaviors of Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:646–54. doi: 10.1016/s0893-133x(02)00339-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Li T, Holmes C, Sham PC, Vallada H, Birkett J, Kirov G, et al. Allelic functional variation of serotonin transporter expression is a susceptibility factor for late onset Alzheimer's disease. Neuroreport. 1997;8:683–6. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199702100-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Oliveira JR, Gallindo RM, Maia LG, Brito-Marques PR, Otto PA, Passos-Bueno MR, et al. The short variant of the polymorphism within the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene is a risk factor for late onset Alzheimer's disease. Mol Psychiatry. 1998;3:438–41. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Ha TM, Cho DM, Park SW, Joo MJ, Lee BJ, Kong BG, et al. Evaluating associations between 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and Alzheimer's disease for Korean patients. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;20:31–4. doi: 10.1159/000085071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Zill P, Padberg F, de Jonge S, Hampel H, Burger K, Stubner S, et al. Serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene polymorphism in psychogeriatric patients. Neurosci Lett. 2000;284:113–5. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)00994-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Harrison BJ, Olver JS, Norman TR, Burrows GD, Wesnes KA, Nathan PJ. Selective effects of acute serotonin and catecholamine depletion on memory in healthy women. J Psychopharmacol. 2004;18:32–40. doi: 10.1177/0269881104040225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Luciana M, Burgund ED, Berman M, Hanson KL. Effects of tryptophan loading on verbal, spatial and affective working memory functions in healthy adults. J Psychopharmacol. 2001;15:219–30. doi: 10.1177/026988110101500410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Riedel WJ, Sobczak S, Schmitt JA. Tryptophan modulation and cognition. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2003;527:207–13. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-0135-0_24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Rasgon NL. Premorbid depression as a risk factor for Alzheimer's Disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. in press. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Keck ME, Holsboer F. Hyperactivity of CRH neuronal circuits as a target for therapeutic interventions in affective disorders. Peptides. 2001;22:835–44. doi: 10.1016/s0196-9781(01)00398-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Hu XZ, Lipsky RH, Zhu G, Akhtar LA, Taubman J, Greenberg BD, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78:815–26. doi: 10.1086/503850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Wendland JR, Martin BJ, Kruse MR, Lesch KP, Murphy DL. Simultaneous genotyping of four functional loci of human SLC6A4, with a reappraisal of 5-HTTLPR and rs25531. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11:224–6. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES