Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cognition. 2006 Oct 24;105(1):218–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.005

Words (but not Tones) Facilitate Object Categorization: Evidence From 6- and 12-Month-Olds

Anne L Fulkerson 1, Sandra R Waxman 2
PMCID: PMC2099297  NIHMSID: NIHMS29741  PMID: 17064677

Abstract

Recent studies reveal that naming has powerful conceptual consequences within the first year of life. Naming distinct objects with the same word highlights commonalities among the objects and promotes object categorization. In the present experiment, we pursued the origin of this link by examining the influence of words and tones on object categorization in infants at 6 and 12 months. At both ages, infants hearing a novel word for a set of distinct objects successfully formed object categories; those hearing a sequence of tones for the same objects did not. These results support the view that infants are sensitive to powerful and increasingly nuanced links between linguistic and conceptual units very early in the process of lexical acquisition.

Keywords: Infancy, Categorization, Object Naming

Questions concerning the relation between linguistic and conceptual organization have long occupied a central position in the cognitive sciences. Recently, considerable attention has been devoted to discovering the origin of this relation in infants and toddlers. Research in this arena has focused primarily on early word-learning. This is an apt focus because at its core, learning a word involves establishing a relation between a linguistic unit (the word) and a conceptual unit (the concept to which the word refers). The key developmental question is when words begin to carry conceptual force, and how early in the process of word-learning they begin to influence infants’ representations of individual objects and the relations among them (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Booth & Waxman, 2003; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003; Namy, 2001; Namy & Waxman, 1998; Roberts & Jacob, 1991; Waxman & Booth, 2003; Waxman & Braun, 2005; Waxman & Markow, 1995; Woodward & Hoyne, 1999; Xu, 1999, 2002).

There is now robust evidence that words and conceptual organization are linked within the first year of life. Even at 9 to 12 months of age, when infants have just begun to produce their first words, naming has powerful conceptual consequences. Applying the same name to distinct objects (e.g., four different animals) highlights commonalities among the objects and supports infants’ formation of an inclusive category (e.g., animal). This phenomenon was first documented in 12-month-olds using a novelty-preference task (Waxman & Braun, 2005; Waxman & Markow, 1995). Infants were familiarized to four different category exemplars (e.g., four animals) in conjunction with infant-directed speech. In the experimental conditions, an experimenter introduced these using a novel name (e.g., “See the fauna?”); in the No Word control condition, she introduced no novel words (e.g., “See here?”). At test, the experimenter introduced a new object from the now-familiar category (e.g., another animal) and an object from a novel category (e.g., a vehicle), saying, “See what I have?”

The results reveal a powerful effect of naming on object categorization. Infants hearing novel words successfully formed categories, as witnessed by their reliable preference for the novel test object; those in the No Word control condition did not. Moreover, recent evidence reveals that by the close of the first year, naming supports the acquisition of novel categories comprised of entirely novel objects (Booth & Waxman, 2002; Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998, in press) and also promotes the strength of object categories as an inductive base (Graham, Kilbreath, & Welder, 2004; Welder & Graham, 2001, 2006).

Additional research has considered whether the effect of naming on infants’ categorization stems specifically from the presentation of novel words, or is the consequence of a more general, attention-engaging function associated with auditory stimuli. To address this issue, researchers have compared the effects of novel words vs. non-linguistic stimuli on infant categorization. For example, Balaban and Waxman (1997) compared the influence of novel words vs. novel tone sequences on 9-month-olds’ object categorization. Infants viewed a series of pictures representing one object category (e.g., rabbits) in conjunction with an auditory stimulus that emanated from a hidden speaker. In the Word condition, this stimulus was a naming phrase (e.g., “a rabbit”). In the Tone condition, this stimulus was a sine-wave tone (matched to the naming phrase in amplitude, duration and pause length). Infants in the Word condition successfully formed object categories; those hearing tones did not. Subsequent work with a broader range of stimuli has extended this finding: words (including content-filtered words) promoted successful categorization, but non-linguistic sounds (including tones, melodies, mechanical sounds, and even mouth sounds) failed to do so (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Fulkerson and Haaf, 2003; Woodward & Hoyne, 1999).

Together, these results suggest that a link between words and concepts is in place quite early. As soon as infants say their first words, there is something special about words: they interpret words, but not other non-linguistic sounds, as inherently connected to meaning. What remains unclear, however, is the origin of this link (Waxman, 1998; Waxman & Lidz, 2006). We know that newborns are especially attuned to human speech (Vouloumanos & Werker, 2004), but this leaves open the question of when infants begin to interpret words as relevant to meaning. Might such a link be available when infants first begin to identify words reliably from the speech stream? And if so, is this early link specific to words, or does it emerge from a more general attention-engaging function associated with auditory stimuli?

We designed the current experiment to address these questions, adapting the novelty-preference paradigm to compare the influence of words and tone sequences on object categorization in infants at two strategically-selected ages. We opened our developmental window at 6 months. There are several indications that this is the point at which infants are poised to make their first forays into mapping words to meaning. In particular, they reliably distinguish open-class (content) from closed-class (function) words, and prefer listening to the former (Gomez, 2002; Shi & Werker, 2003); they are able to reliably identify some familiar content words within fluent speech (Mandel-Emer, 1997); and they begin to map a small set of content words (e.g., ‘mommy’, ‘daddy’), to their referents (Tincoff & Jusczyk, 1999). At the same time, infants at 6 months are far from proficient in word-learning. There are several essential developmental milestones not yet in their repertoires. Notably, it is not until 7 months that infants’ begin to babble the speech sounds characteristic of their ambient language (Oller, 1980; Oller & Lynch, 1992). Moreover, it is not until 8 months that infants come to recognize the stress patterns typical of words in their ambient language (Houston, Jusczyk, Kuijpers, Coolen, & Cutler, 2000; Johnson & Jusczyk, 2001); it is not until 9 months that they come to recognize phonemic categories that are specific to their ambient language (Cheour et al., 1998; Werker & Lalonde, 1988; Werker & Tees, 1984); and it is not until 14 months that they are able to recruit their sensitivity to phonemic contrasts (e.g., ‘bin’ vs ‘din’) as they map words to meaning (Fennell & Waxman, 2006; Werker, Fennell, Corcoran & Stager, 2002). It is therefore an open question whether infants at this developmental juncture would interpret novel words as inherently connected to meaning and reference. We also included infants at 12 months to anchor the results of the current paradigm with infants for whom the link between words and concepts has been robustly demonstrated (Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998; in press; Waxman & Braun, 2005; Waxman & Markow, 1995).

Based on previous work, our predictions for 12-month-olds were clear: novel words (but not tone sequences) should support the establishment of object categories (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Waxman & Markow, 1995; Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998, 2003, in press). At issue is the performance of the 6-month-olds. One possibility is that for these young infants, words and tones will carry the same force. If this is the case, then infants should perform comparably in the context of words and tones (either successfully establishing object categories, or failing to do so). But it is also possible that infants at this developmental juncture are already sensitive to the distinct conceptual force of words. If this is the case, then infants hearing words (but not those hearing tones) will successfully form object categories.

Method

Participants

Participants included 64 six-month-olds (25 female) averaging 185.0 days in age (SD = 7.0) and 64 twelve-month-olds (38 female) averaging 364.0 days in age (SD = 14.5). Infants were recruited from county birth records and were predominantly from white, middle-class families. All participating infants were full-term, healthy, and acquiring English as their native language. Eleven additional infants were excluded: equipment failure (4), parental/sibling interference (2), fussiness (2), experimenter error (1), obstructed view of infant (2).

Stimuli

Visual stimuli

Twenty slides of line-drawn dinosaurs and fish were produced to form two familiarization sets of eight stimuli each and two test sets of two stimuli each (see Figure 1). Within each familiarization set, stimuli varied in color; within each test set, stimuli were matched in color. Stimuli were outlined in black, filled with a solid color, and projected to an average angular size of 13.3 by 9.0 degrees onto a white screen.

Auditory stimuli

Auditory stimuli included two sets of naming phrases (“Oh look, it’s a toma/modi. Do you see the toma/modi?”) and two sequences of pure tones (400 Hz, 800 Hz). Naming phrases were spoken by a female in the infant-directed speech register and recorded for presentation. Tone sequences were created to match the naming phrases in timing, duration (3.4 s), and volume (92 dB).

Apparatus

Infants sat on a parent’s lap in a gray three-sided booth. One wall of the booth contained two 18 by 18 cm screens (with 12 cm of separation) centered 61 cm from infants’ eye level. Visual stimuli were displayed via slide projector. Auditory stimuli were presented through a speaker hidden 58 cm below the midpoint of the screens. Infants were videotaped through a 6 cm hole located 11 cm below the projection screens.

Procedure

Each infant participated in a novelty-preference task that included a familiarization phase and a test phase (see Figure 1). Parents were blind to the hypotheses and were instructed not to influence their infant’s attention in any way.

Familiarization phase

Infants were presented with eight different exemplars from the same category (e.g., dinosaurs), one at a time, in random order, for 20 s each. The lateral position of the stimulus was determined randomly on the first trial and alternated thereafter. Half the infants at each age were randomly assigned to the Word or Tone condition. In the Word condition, infants heard a naming phrase as they viewed the stimuli; in the Tone condition, infants heard a sequence of pure tones. In both conditions, the auditory stimulus was introduced during the first 4 s of each trial and once again approximately 3 s later. Stimulus set and auditory stimulus were counterbalanced.

Test phase

In both conditions infants viewed two stimuli: a new exemplar from the now-familiar category (e.g., another dinosaur) and an exemplar from a novel category (e.g., a fish). These were presented side-by-side in silence. The test phase began with infants’ first visual fixation to one of the stimuli and lasted 10 s. Test stimulus set and the lateral position of the stimuli were counterbalanced.

Coding

A trained observer, blind to the hypotheses and condition assignment, coded infants’ visual fixations. Twenty-five percent of infants in each age and condition were re-coded by an independent observer. Reliability between observers was r = .96 during familiarization and r = .88 during test.

Results

Familiarization Phase

We submitted infants’ total accumulated looking time during familiarization to an Age x Condition ANOVA. A significant effect of Age, F(1, 124) = 31.67, p < .001, prep = .997, d = .99, revealed that 12-month-olds (M = 115.6 s, SD = 18.1 s) accumulated more looking than did 6-month-olds (M = 93.8 s, SD = 26.0 s). In addition, the Age x Condition interaction was significant, F(1, 124) = 7.66, p = .007, prep = .959, d = .49, indicating that with age, infants showed more interest in objects presented with words than objects presented with tones.

Test Phase

To measure categorization, we calculated a novelty-preference score for each infant, dividing looking to the novel-category exemplar by total looking. If infants formed a category, then they should reveal a novelty-preference score greater than chance (.50). We therefore compared performance in each age group and condition to this level of responding.

As predicted, 12-month-olds in the Word condition demonstrated a reliable novelty-preference, (M = .59, SD = .18), t(31) = 2.89, p = .007, two-tailed, prep = .959, d = 1.02, but those in the Tone condition performed at chance, (M = .53, SD = .14), t(31) = 1.32, p = .20, prep = .724, d = .47. Remarkably, 6-month-olds demonstrated the same effect. Infants in the Word condition demonstrated a reliable novelty-preference (M = .63, SD = .19; t(31) = 3.95, p < .001, prep = 1.00, d = 1.40, but those in the Tone condition performed at chance (M = .54, SD = .20; t(31) = 1.05, p = .30, prep = .645, d = .38.

We next submitted infants’ novelty-preference scores to an Age x Condition ANOVA. A significant effect of Condition, F(1, 128) = 5.92, p = .016, prep = .935, d = .43, revealed that infants in the Word condition (M = .61, SD = .19) showed reliably higher novelty-preference scores than did infants in the Tone condition (M = .54, SD = .17). There were no other main effects or interactions (ps > .45). Thus, at both 6- and 12-months of age, words served as a more powerful impetus than tones in the establishment of object categories.

A subsequent analysis of individual infants’ performance provides additional evidence that by 6 months, novel words have conceptual consequences that are not observed with tone sequences. We tabulated the number of infants in each age and condition with novelty-preference scores above .50. We then used the binomial probability distribution to determine whether this number differed from chance expectations (16 of 32 infants). At 12 months, 24 infants in the Word condition revealed a novelty-preference (p = .007, two-tailed); only 17 did so in the Tone condition (p = .86). At 6 months, 25 infants in the Word condition exhibited a novelty-preference (p = .002); only 20 did so in the Tone condition (p = .22). This suggests that results of the main parametric analyses reflect tendencies that are characteristic of most individuals. At both 6- and 12-months of age, words, but not tones, support the establishment of object categories.

Discussion

Previous work has documented that by the time infants begin to produce their first words, they are sensitive to a link between words and the concepts to which they refer. Present results reveal that infants are sensitive to this link as early as 6 months and suggest that this sensitivity is tied specifically to words, rather than to general attention-engaging properties associated with sound. To the best of our knowledge, this constitutes the earliest documentation to date of a link between linguistic units (words) and conceptual units (object categories), and suggests that this link is available to guide infants’ earliest efforts to map words to meaning.

These results provide insight into the capacities of infants as they enter the process of word-learning. Characterizing these capacities has been the focus of considerable recent debate. Some have argued that early word-learning is the product of a general associative mechanism, that infants’ first words are acquired without the benefit of any guiding expectations, and that it is only after infants have gained a substantial productive lexicon that they become sensitive to links between linguistic and conceptual units (Roberts, 1997; Smith, 1999; Smith, Colunga & Yoshida, 2003). Others have argued that words hinder infants’ ability to form categories (Robinson & Sloutsky, in press; Gogate & Bahrick, 2001; Gogate, Bahrick, & Watson, 2000).

We have argued for a very different view, asserting that infants are guided from the start by powerful and increasingly nuanced links between linguistic, conceptual, and perceptual units (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Booth & Waxman, 2003; Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998, 2003, in press; Gopnik & Nazzi, 2003; Graham, Baker & Poulin-Dubois, 1998; Waxman & Braun, 2005; Waxman & Lidz, 2006; Waxman & Markow, 1995; Xu, 2002). The current results favor this latter interpretation. They accord well with the view that there is indeed something special about words in the first year of life, and they challenge seriously the claim that infants enter the process of word-learning as tabulae rasae, with no links between linguistic units (e.g., words) and conceptual units (e.g., object categories) to guide acquisition.

The current results represent a first step toward characterizing the relation between words and concepts in infants on the threshold of word-learning. They also open the door for further investigation. In particular, it will be crucial to examine infants’ responses to a broader range of stimuli, both linguistic and non-linguistic. Evidence is mounting that infants actually coordinate several kinds of cues in the service of establishing word meaning (Hall & Waxman, 2004; Hollich, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2000; Roberts, 1995). For example, even non-linguistic elements such as whistles and brief melodic bursts may be interpreted as ‘names for things’ when an experimenter makes it clear that her intention is to treat these stimuli as object names (Campbell & Namy, 2003; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003; Hollich et al., 2000; Namy, 2001; Namy & Waxman, 1998; Woodward & Hoyne, 1999). Importantly, when strong interpersonal cues to naming are stripped away, non-linguistic stimuli have no such effect (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003). However, these findings highlight the need to identify the relevant linguistic and non-linguistic cues brought to bear in the service of word-learning and to ascertain how infants recruit these cues in mapping words to meaning.

It will also be important to detail with greater precision which properties of linguistic stimuli provide them their conceptual force in the first year of life. We suspect that the influence initially reflects infants’ sensitivity to infant-directed speech and to content words and that a sensitivity to more specific syntactic and semantic properties follows. From birth, infants are exquisitely attuned to human speech (Molfese, Freeman, & Polermo, 1975; Vouloumanos & Werker, 2004), and are especially attracted to the heightened pitch and exaggerated intonational contours of infant-directed speech (Cooper & Aslin, 1990; Fernald, 1992; Fernald, Taeschner, Dun, Papousek, de Boysson-Bardies, & Fukui, 1989). During the first six months, infant-directed speech primarily engages and modulates attention (Fernald, 1992; Kaplan, Goldstein, Huckeby, Owren, & Cooper, 1995). However, in the second six months, “words begin to emerge from the melody” (Fernald, 1992, p. 403) as infants start to parse content words from the ongoing speech stream and to show a preference for content words over function words (Mandel-Emer, 1997; Shi & Werker, 2003). By their second year, infants show even greater differentiation as they begin to distinguish between more specific grammatical forms (such as nouns and adjectives) and link them to different kinds of meaning (object categories and object properties, respectively) (Booth & Waxman, 2003; Waxman & Booth, 2001, 2003).

In sum, the current results reveal that a link between linguistic and conceptual units is available early enough to guide infants’ first steps in word-learning. To characterize this link with greater precision, it will be critical to examine infants’ sensitivity to various properties of the speech signal, including acoustic, syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic properties of words, as they unfold over development and as they relate to meaning.

Trial Word Tone Left Screen Right Screen
Familiarization 1 Look at the toma!
Do you see the toma?
graphic file with name nihms29741t1.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t2.jpg
Familiarization 2 Look at the toma!
Do you see the toma?
graphic file with name nihms29741t3.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t4.jpg
Familiarization 3 Look at the toma!
Do you see the toma?
graphic file with name nihms29741t5.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t6.jpg
Familiarization 4 Look at the toma!
Do you see the toma?
graphic file with name nihms29741t7.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t8.jpg
Familiarization 5 Look at the toma!
Do you see the toma?
graphic file with name nihms29741t9.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t10.jpg
Familiarization 6 Look at the toma!
Do you see the toma?
graphic file with name nihms29741t11.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t12.jpg
Familiarization 7 Look at the toma!
Do you see the toma?
graphic file with name nihms29741t13.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t14.jpg
Familiarization 8 Look at the toma!
Do you see the toma?
graphic file with name nihms29741t15.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t16.jpg

Test graphic file with name nihms29741t17.jpg graphic file with name nihms29741t18.jpg

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIH grant #HD045296 to Anne Fulkerson and NIH grant #HD030410 to Sandra Waxman. We are indebted to the infants and parents who generously volunteered their participation, as well as to Jennifer Seymour, Kristin Szymanowski, and Julie Hupp for their assistance with data collection. We thank Jeff Lidz, Thomas Piccin, Erin Leddon, and Robert Haaf for their comments on previous drafts of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Balaban MT, Waxman SR. Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1997;64:3–26. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1996.2332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Booth AE, Waxman SR. Object names and object functions serve as cues to categories for infants. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:948–957. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.6.948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Booth AE, Waxman SR. Mapping words to the world in infancy: Infants’ expectations for count nouns and adjectives. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2003;4:357–381. [Google Scholar]
  4. Campbell AL, Namy LL. The role of social-referential context in verbal and nonverbal symbol learning. Child Development. 2003;74:549–563. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Lehtokoski A, Luuk A, Allik J, Alho K, Naatanen R. Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nature Neuroscience. 1998;1:351–353. doi: 10.1038/1561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cooper RP, Aslin RN. Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development. 1990;61:1584–1595. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Fennell CT, Waxman SR. Infants of 14 months use phonetic detail in novel words placed in naming phrases. In: Fennell CT, editor. How the speech signal affects word learning, and vice-versa; Symposium conducted at the International Conference on Infant Studies; Kyoto, Japan. 2006. Jun, Chair. [Google Scholar]
  8. Fernald A. Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In: Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J, editors. The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press; 1992. pp. 391–428. [Google Scholar]
  9. Fernald A, Taeschner T, Dun J, Papousek M, de Boysson-Bardies B, Fukui I. A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language. 1989;16:477–501. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900010679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Fulkerson AL, Haaf RA. New words for new things: The relationship between novel labels and 12-month-olds’ categorization of novel objects. Poster session presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies; Atlanta, GA. 1998. [Google Scholar]
  11. Fulkerson AL, Haaf RA. The influence of labels, non-labeling sounds, and source of auditory input on 9- and 15-month-olds’ object categorization. Infancy. 2003;4:349–369. [Google Scholar]
  12. Fulkerson AL, Haaf RA. Does object naming aid 12-month-olds’ formation of novel object categories? First Language in press. [Google Scholar]
  13. Gogate LJ, Bahrick LE. Intersensory redundancy and 7-month-old infants’ memory for arbitrary syllable-object relations. Infancy. 2001;2:219–231. [Google Scholar]
  14. Gogate LJ, Bahrick LE, Watson JD. A study of multimodal motherese: The role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Development. 2000;71:878–894. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gomez R. Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science. 2002;13:431–436. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gopnik A, Nazzi T. Words, kinds, and causal powers: A theory theory perspective on early naming and categorization. In: Rakison DH, Oakes LM, editors. Early category and concept development: Making sense of the blooming, buzzing confusion. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 303–329. [Google Scholar]
  17. Graham SA, Baker RK, Poulin-Dubois D. Infants’ expectations about object label reference. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1998;52:103–113. doi: 10.1037/h0087285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Graham SA, Kilbreath CS, Welder AN. 13-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences. Child Development. 2004;75:409–427. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00683.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Johnson EK, Jusczyk PW. Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count more than statistics. Journal of Memory and Language. 2001;44:548–567. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hall DG, Waxman SR, editors. Weaving a lexicon. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hollich GJ, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2000;65(3 Serial No 262) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Houston DM, Jusczyk PW, Kuijpers C, Coolen R, Cutler A. Cross-language word segmentation by 9-month-olds. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2000;7:504–509. doi: 10.3758/bf03214363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kaplan PS, Goldstein MH, Huckeby ER, Owren MJ, Cooper RP. Dishabituation of visual attention by infant- versus adult-directed speech: Effects of frequency modulation and spectral composition. Infant Behavior and Development. 1995;18:209–223. [Google Scholar]
  24. Mandel-Emer DR. Names as early lexical candidates: Helpful in language processing? (PhD Dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo) Dissertation Abstracts International. 1997;57:5947. [Google Scholar]
  25. Molfese DL, Freeman RB, Palermo DS. The ontogeny of brain lateralization for speech and nonspeech stimuli. Brain and Language. 1975;2:356–368. doi: 10.1016/s0093-934x(75)80076-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Namy LL. What’s in a name when it isn’t a word? 17-month-olds’ mapping of nonverbal symbols to object categories. Infancy. 2001;2:73–86. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0201_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Namy LL, Campbell AL, Tomasello M. The changing role of iconicity in non-verbal symbol learning: A U-shaped trajectory in the acquisition of arbitrary gestures. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2004;5:37–57. [Google Scholar]
  28. Namy LL, Waxman SR. Words and gestures: Infants’ interpretations of different forms of symbolic reference. Child Development. 1998;69:295–308. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Oller DK. The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In: Yeni-Komshian GH, Kavanagh JF, Ferguson CA, editors. Child phonology. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press; 1980. pp. 93–112. [Google Scholar]
  30. Oller DK, Lynch MP. Infant vocalizations and innovations in infraphonology: Toward a broader theory of development and disorders. In: Ferguson CA, Menn L, Stoel-Gammon C, editors. Phonological development: Models, research, implications. Timonium, MD: York Press; 1992. pp. 509–538. [Google Scholar]
  31. Roberts K. Categorical responding in 15-month-olds: Influence of the noun-category bias and the covariation between visual fixation and auditory input. Cognitive Development. 1995;10:21–41. [Google Scholar]
  32. Roberts K. Linguistic and nonlinguistic factors influencing infant categorization: Studies of the relationship between cognition and language. In: Rovee-Collier C, Lipsitt LP, editors. Advances in infancy research. Vol. 11. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1997. pp. 45–108. [Google Scholar]
  33. Roberts K, Jacob M. Linguistic versus attentional influences on nonlinguistic categorization in 15-month-old infants. Cognitive Development. 1991;6:355–375. [Google Scholar]
  34. Robinson CW, Sloutsky VM. Categorization in infancy: When sounds and labels hinder category learning. Proceedings of the XXVIII Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. in press. [Google Scholar]
  35. Shi R, Werker JF. The basis of preference for lexical words in 6-month-old infants. Developmental Science. 2003;6:484–488. [Google Scholar]
  36. Smith LB. Children’s noun learning: How general learning processes make specialized learning mechanisms. In: MacWhinney B, editor. The emergence of language. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1999. pp. 277–303. [Google Scholar]
  37. Smith LB, Colunga E, Yoshida H. Making an ontology: Cross-linguistic evidence. In: Rakison DH, Oakes LM, editors. Early category and concept development: Making sense of the blooming, buzzing confusion. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 275–302. [Google Scholar]
  38. Tincoff R, Jusczyk P. Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science. 1999;10:172–175. [Google Scholar]
  39. Vouloumanos A, Werker JF. Tuned to the signal: the privileged status of speech for young infants. Developmental Science. 2004;7:270–276. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00345.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Waxman SR. Linking object categorization and naming: Early expectations and the shaping role of language. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. 1998;38:249–291. [Google Scholar]
  41. Waxman SR, Booth AE. Seeing pink elephants: Fourteen-month-olds’ interpretations of novel nouns and adjectives. Cognitive Psychology. 2001;43:217–242. doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Waxman SR, Booth AE. The origins and evolution of links between word learning and conceptual organization: New evidence from 11-month-olds. Developmental Science. 2003;6:128–135. [Google Scholar]
  43. Waxman SR, Braun IE. Consistent (but not variable) names as invitations to form object categories: new evidence from 12-month-old infants. Cognition. 2005;95:B59–B68. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Waxman SR, Lidz J. Early word learning. In: Kuhn D, Siegler R, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology. 6. Vol. 2. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2006. pp. 299–335. [Google Scholar]
  45. Waxman SR, Markow DB. Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology. 1995;29:257–302. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Welder AN, Graham SA. The influence of shape similarity and shared labels on infants’ inductive inferences about nonobvious object properties. Child Development. 2001;72:1653–1673. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Welder AN, Graham SA. Infants’ categorization of novel objects with more or less obvious features. Cognitive Psychology. 2006;52:57–91. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Werker JF, Fennell CT, Corcoran KM, Stager CL. Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy. 2002;3:1–30. [Google Scholar]
  49. Werker JF, Tees RC. Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Infant Behavior & Development. 1984;7:49–63. [Google Scholar]
  50. Woodward AL, Hoyne KL. Infants’ learning about words and sounds in relation to objects. Child Development. 1999;70:65–77. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Werker JF, Lalonde CE. Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental Psychology. 1988;24:672–683. [Google Scholar]
  52. Xu F. Object individuation and object identity in infancy. The role of spatiotemporal information, object property information, and language. Acta Psychologica. 1999;102:113–136. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00029-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Xu F. The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition. 2002;85:223–250. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00109-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES