Skip to main content
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry logoLink to Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry
. 2006 Jan;77(1):28–33. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.069120

Cognitive profile of subcortical ischaemic vascular disease

H Jokinen 1,2,3,4,5,6, H Kalska 1,2,3,4,5,6, R Mäntylä 1,2,3,4,5,6, T Pohjasvaara 1,2,3,4,5,6, R Ylikoski 1,2,3,4,5,6, M Hietanen 1,2,3,4,5,6, O Salonen 1,2,3,4,5,6, M Kaste 1,2,3,4,5,6, T Erkinjuntti 1,2,3,4,5,6
PMCID: PMC2117424  PMID: 16361588

Abstract

Objectives

Subcortical ischaemic vascular disease (SIVD) is a subtype of vascular cognitive impairment characterised by extensive white matter lesions and multiple lacunar infarcts. Radiologically defined diagnostic criteria for SIVD have been introduced, but only a few studies have presented empirical data on its clinical and cognitive features. The aim of this study is to describe in detail the neuropsychological characteristics of patients with SIVD from a large well defined stroke cohort.

Methods

A sample of 323 consecutive patients with ischaemic stroke, aged 55–85 years, was investigated using neuropsychological examination and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Patients fulfilling the MRI criteria of SIVD (n = 85) were compared to the other stroke patients (n = 238) and to normal control subjects (n = 38).

Results

Cognitive performance of the SIVD group was inferior to that of the normal control group throughout all domains. As compared to the other stroke patients, the SIVD group performed significantly worse in tests measuring executive functions and delayed memory recall. Adjusting for depression had no effect on these results. Instead, after controlling for medial temporal lobe atrophy, the differences disappeared for delayed memory but remained significant for executive functions.

Conclusion

Executive deficits are the most prominent cognitive characteristic associated with SIVD. Patients with SIVD also exhibit subtle deficits in delayed memory, which is explained in part by medial temporal lobe atrophy. Cognitive and mood changes seem to be parallel but independent processes related to SIVD. The results support the concept of SIVD as a separate clinical entity.

Keywords: ageing, cognition, executive functions, stroke, subcortical ischaemic vascular disease


The concept of vascular cognitive impairment1 covers a wide spectrum of cognitive dysfunction ranging from subtle and clinically often undetected deficits to overt dementia. The major subtypes of vascular dementia are cortical vascular dementia, strategic infarct dementia, and subcortical vascular dementia, which all have varying aetiological mechanisms and manifestations.2 Because of the heterogeneity of the clinical appearance of vascular cognitive impairment and dementia, the concept of subcortical ischaemic vascular disease (SIVD), defined by specific features on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), has been adopted.2 SIVD is regarded as a relatively homogeneous subgroup of vascular cognitive impairment with a more cohesive aetiology and a more predictable outcome. It arises from small vessel disease and is characterised by extensive cerebral white matter lesions and lacunar infarcts in deep grey and white matter structures.3 Diagnostic consensus criteria have been introduced recently.4 However, empirical data describing its clinical and cognitive manifestations are still sparse.

A previous study by our group5 has shown that patients with SIVD, as compared to other elderly stroke patients, more often have a history of progressive cognitive decline, dependence in activities of daily living, executive dysfunction, and DSM‐III defined dementia. Other studies investigating the cognitive characteristics of patients with SIVD are few and have used diverse diagnostic criteria. A recent study has found subtle declines in executive functions and visual memory in non‐demented patients with one or more subcortical lacunes.6 It has been suggested that subcortical ischaemic vascular pathology can be distinguished from Alzheimer's disease by neuropsychological features, that is, better recognition memory7,8 and spatial cognition7 but worse verbal fluency.8 Due to a disruption of the frontal‐subcortical circuits, the frontal lobe mediated executive functions are considered particularly vulnerable to ischaemic lesions.9,10,11

The aim of this study was to describe in detail the neuropsychological profile of SIVD in order to further validate the brain imaging criteria in use. In our large consecutive stroke sample, we compared patients fulfilling the criteria of SIVD to all the other stroke patients (OS) and to normal control subjects (NC). All subjects underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological examination and MRI.

Methods

Subjects and study protocol

As part of the Helsinki Stroke Aging Memory (SAM) Study, 486 consecutive patients, aged 55–85 years, were examined 3 months after an ischaemic stroke. The examination included a clinical neurological assessment, brain MRI, and a detailed neuropsychological assessment. The study protocol and the clinical characteristics of the cohort have been described in previous publications.5,12,13 In brief, 409 patients participated in the neuropsychological assessment after patients with considerable hearing or sight disabilities, or reduced level of consciousness, and those who were unwilling to participate were excluded. Cases with inadequate neuropsychological or MRI data (n = 86) were also excluded from this study. The total patient sample was therefore 323 subjects. Compared to the included patients, the excluded patients (n = 163) were older (72.9 v 70.3 years, p<0.001), had less education (8.4 v 9.5 years, p<0.01), and more severe stroke as measured with the Scandinavian Stroke Scale (46.0 v 55.5, p<0.001). They were also more impaired in activities of daily living according to the Barthel Index (14.8 v 19.0, p<0.001) and had lower global cognitive status in the Mini‐Mental State Examination14 (22.1 v 26.3, p<0.001). There were no gender differences between the excluded and included patients.

The control subjects (n = 38) were drawn from a population based study conducted previously.15 They were clinically evaluated as neurologically healthy and all participated in the neuropsychological assessment and MRI.

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Neurology, Helsinki University Central Hospital. The study design was fully explained to the subjects. Those who participated gave their informed consent.

Neuropsychological assessment

The neuropsychological battery covered a wide range of cognitive domains, and standard test instructions and scoring were used.16,17 In order to reduce the number of variables, seven composite scores were constituted by calculating the mean of the standardised z scores across the whole study sample in each domain. The distributions of the individual and composite scores were approaching normality. When appropriate, the scales were first reversed to correspond with each other. The variables and the reliability coefficients for the composite scores are presented in table 1.

Table 1 Neuropsychological variables used in composite z scores.

Cognitive domains and variables Reliability coefficient (Cronbach's α)
Speed of mental processing 0.66
 Trail Making A test, time
 Stroop dots, time
Executive functions 0.78
 Trail Making test, subtraction score (B time−A time)
 Trail Making B test, correct responses
 Stroop test, subtraction score (words, time−dots, time)
 Stroop words, correct responses
 WCST, correct responses
 WCST, perseverations
 Verbal fluency, animal category
 Verbal fluency, letter word list generation
Short term memory (working memory) 0.62
 WMS digit span, forwards
 WMS digit span, backwards
Immediate recall 0.71
 WMS‐R logical memory story A, immediate recall
 WMS‐R visual reproduction, immediate recall
 FOME, total retrieval
Delayed recall 0.76
 WMS‐R logical memory story A, delayed recall
 WMS‐R visual reproduction, delayed recall
 FOME, delayed recall
Verbal intellectual functions 0.79
 WAIS‐R similarities
 WAIS‐R information, shortened version
 WAIS‐R comprehension, shortened version
Visuospatial functions
 WAIS‐R block design

The reliability coefficient for the visuospatial score was not calculated as it contained only one test.

FOME, Fuld Object Memory Evaluation; WAIS‐R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WCST, Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; WMS‐R, Wechsler Memory Scale‐Revised

Speed of mental processing was assessed with the Trail Making test part A18 and a short version of the Stroop test, coloured dots section (Stroop dots).19 The total time to complete the tasks was measured.

Executive functions were evaluated with the Trail Making test part B18 and the Stroop coloured colour names section (Stroop words)19 as measuring mental flexibility, set shifting, and response inhibition. In order to extract the executive component in these tests, a difference score was calculated by subtracting the time taken in Trail Making A from the time taken in Trail Making B. A similar calculation was performed for the Stroop dots and words tests. The number of correct responses was also recorded for the Trail Making B and the Stroop words tests. Abstract problem solving and the ability to shift cognitive sets were assessed with the modified version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST),20 where the parameters were the number of correct responses and perseverations. Verbal fluency was assessed in semantic category (animals) and letter word list generation (words beginning with the letter K) in 60 s.21

Short term memory (working memory) was measured with the digit span forward and backward subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS),22 in which the scores were the longest correctly repeated series of digits.

Immediate memory was evaluated by using the Fuld Object Memory Evaluation (FOME, total retrieval of five learning trials),23 and the immediate recalls of the logical memory (story A) and visual reproduction subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale‐Revised (WMS‐R).24

Delayed memory was assessed with the delayed free recalls of the FOME, and WMS‐R logical memory and visual reproduction subtests.

Verbal intellectual functions were evaluated with the similarities subtest, and shortened versions of the information (10 items) and comprehension (four items) subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐Revised (WAIS‐R).25

Visuospatial functions were assessed with the block design subtest of the WAIS‐R.25

Magnetic resonance imaging

The radiological method has been described in previous reports.5,26,27 MRI was performed with a superconducting 1.0 T system and the images were reviewed by a neuroradiologist (RM) blinded to the clinical data. Focal lesions were recorded according to their number, location, side, and type. Lesions approaching the signal characteristics of cerebrospinal fluid on T1 weighted images and measuring over 3 mm in diameter were regarded as brain infarcts. Infarcts were classified by size into four groups according to largest diameter, and the average radii were used for volume calculations. The infarct was determined as lacunar if it was situated in deep white or grey matter, was 3–9 mm in diameter, and was supplied by the deep branches of the anterior, middle, or posterior cerebral arteries, or the internal carotid artery.

White matter hyperintensities were assessed on proton density weighted images in periventricular (PVH) and in deep, borderzone, and subcortical regions (deep white matter hyperintensities, DWMH), and were graded from 0 to 3 as detailed earlier.26,27 Brain atrophy was rated visually from 0 to 3 separately on both hemispheres based on T1 weighted images. General cortical atrophy was rated in the frontal, parietal, and occipital lobes, and in the temporal neocortex. Medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) was rated on coronal slices in the hippocampal formation and the entorhinal cortex comprising the parahippocampal gyrus. Central atrophy was rated by assessing the width of the temporal, frontal, and occipital horns, the bodies of lateral ventricles, and the third ventricle. The ratings of cortical and central atrophy and MTA were combined into three sum variables for the purposes of the present study (ranges 0–24, 0–27, and 0–12, respectively).28

Definition of SIVD

SIVD was determined according to the MRI criteria (for full details, see Erkinjuntti et al4) that include (a) predominantly white matter cases: extending periventricular and deep white matter lesions, and (b) predominantly lacunar cases: multiple lacunes (>5) in deep grey matter and at least moderate white matter lesions. Cases with cortical and/or cortico‐subcortical non‐lacunar territorial infarcts, borderzone infarcts, haemorrhages, signs of normal pressure hydrocephalus, and specific causes of white matter lesions were excluded from SIVD. All the other patients not fulfilling these criteria were classed as “other stroke patients”. Patients with both first ever strokes and with previous stroke incidents were included.

Data analysis

The demographic and clinical characteristics of the study groups were examined with independent‐samples t test, analysis of variance, and χ2 test. Neuropsychological test performance was studied with a multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) of the composite z scores and with univariate analyses of covariance of the raw scores. Pairwise contrasts were calculated between the covariate adjusted group means by comparing the SIVD group to the OS and NC groups. A p level <0.05 was regarded as statistically significant, but for the pairwise contrasts the more conservative Bonferroni correction is also reported (0.05/2 = 0.025; denoted in the text with B*, if significant). Effect sizes for the group differences were analysed with partial eta squared (ηp2) reflecting the proportion of the total variance attributable to the effect; it is considered small if 0.01, moderate around 0.06, and high if >0.13 (converted from f according to Cohen).29

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Of the 323 patients, 85 fulfilled the criteria for SIVD. The characteristics of the study groups are presented in table 2 (see also Pohjasvaara et al5). As to the demographic variables, the study groups differed significantly in terms of age and education but not gender. Pairwise contrast comparisons revealed significant differences between the patients with SIVD and OS in age (p<0.05) and education (p<0.01B*). The SIVD group differed from the NC group in age (p<0.01B*) but not education (p = 0.19). In the Mini‐Mental State Examination,14 the SIVD group had lower scores than the NC group (p<0.001B*), but the SIVD and OS groups did not differ from each other (p = 0.07). The presence of dementia according to the DSM‐III‐R criteria30 did not differentiate the two patient groups. However, patients with SIVD exhibited significantly more depressive symptoms than patients with OS as measured with the Beck Depression Inventory31 (21 items) (table 2).

Table 2 Characteristics of the study groups.

SIVD, n = 85 OS, n = 238 NC, n = 38 Statistics
p value
Demographic characteristics
 Age 71.7 (6.9) 69.8 (7.8) 67.4 (5.3) F = 4.6 0.011
 Gender, men 42 (49.4) 118 (49.6) 18 (47.4) χ2 = 0.1 0.968
 Education, years 8.3 (3.6) 9.8 (4.3) 9.4 (3.5) F = 4.2 0.016
Clinical characteristics
 Mini‐Mental State Examination 25.8 (3.1) 26.5 (3.2) 28.2 (2.0) F = 8.0 0.001
 Dementia, DSM‐III‐R 16 (18.8) 31 (13.0) 0 (0) χ2 = 1.7* 0.193
 Beck Depression Inventory 11.8 (9.0) 8.4 (6.6) NA t = 3.1* 0.002
MRI findings
 Number of infarcts 4.3 (2.4) 2.7 (2.2) 0.3 (0.7)
 Total volume of infarcts, cm3 16.3 (29.9) 29.1 (42.3) 0.2 (1.0)
 PVH, range 0–3 3.0 (0.3) 1.7 (0.9) 1.2 (1.1)
 DWMH, range 0–3 1.8 (0.9) 1.0 (0.6) 0.8 (0.8)
 Cortical atrophy, range 0–24 11.9 (6.4) 10.5 (6.4) 4.8 (4.4) F = 18.1 0.001
 MTA, range 0–12 4.2 (3.6) 3.1 (3.3) 1.4 (2.3) F = 9.1 0.001
 Central atrophy, range 0–27 12.7 (8.2) 11.0 (8.5) 2.4 (3.6) F = 2.7 0.001

The results are given as mean (standard deviation, SD), using analysis of variance F or t test, except for the gender and dementia variables, for which the results are given as frequency (percentage), using χ2 test. Statistics are not calculated for infarcts or white matter hyperintensities to avoid circularity.

*Comparisons are made only between the two patient groups.

DSM‐III‐R, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders‐Revised; DWMH, deep white matter hyperintensities; MTA, medial temporal lobe atrophy; NC, normal control subjects; OS, other stroke patients; NA, not available; PVH, periventricular white matter hyperintensities; SIVD, subcortical ischaemic vascular disease.

In addition to the number and volume of the infarcts and white matter hyperintensities, the study groups differed from each other in terms of several other MRI findings (table 2). In contrast to the NC group, the patients with SIVD had significantly more cortical (p<0.001B*) and central atrophy (p<0.001B*), and MTA (p<0.001B*). As compared to the OS group, they had more MTA (p<0.05B*) but not more cortical (p = 0.07) or central (p = 0.10) atrophy.

Neuropsychological performance of the study groups

When the composite z scores of the neuropsychological tests were used as dependent variables, and age and education were used as covariates, the multivariate main effect for the study groups was highly significant (MANCOVA Wilks' λ F = 4.1, p<0.0001, ηp2 = 0.077). The study groups differed significantly from each other in all of the dependent variables (table 3). Pairwise comparisons showed that the SIVD group differed from the NC group in each composite z score. Moreover, the composite z scores for executive functions and delayed memory recall were significantly lower in patients with SIVD than in patients with OS. Between these groups, no significant differences were found in speed of mental processing, short term memory, immediate recall, verbal intellectual functions, or visuospatial functions. Figure 1 shows the neuropsychological test performance profile in patients with SIVD and OS as normalised in relation to the NC group. The results of the two patient groups remained unchanged even after controlling for the total score of the Beck Depression Inventory. Instead, when MTA was controlled, the patient groups still differed in executive functions (p<0.05, ηp2 = 0.013) but not in delayed memory.

Table 3 Comparisons of the neuropsychological composite scores of the study groups.

Dependent variable Composite z scores Between‐subjects effects Pairwise contrasts
SIVD OS NC F p value ηp2 SIVD v OS SIVD v NC
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p value ηp2 p value ηp2
Speed of mental processing −0.15 (0.86) −0.00 (0.91) 0.44 (0.32) 3.6 0.028 0.020 0.759 0.000 0.014 0.017
Executive functions −0.33 (0.70) −0.01 (0.66) 0.45 (0.42) 14.9 0.001 0.079 0.006 0.022 0.001 0.079
Short term memory −0.15 (0.76) 0.01 (0.87) 0.38 (0.88) 3.9 0.022 0.022 0.433 0.002 0.007 0.021
Immediate recall −0.25 (0.83) −0.01 (0.76) 0.70 (0.55) 17.3 0.001 0.091 0.164 0.006 0.001 0.085
Delayed recall −0.32 (0.87) −0.00 (0.78) 0.74 (0.56) 19.3 0.001 0.100 0.028 0.014 0.001 0.098
Verbal intellectual functions −0.23 (0.80) 0.02 (0.85) 0.53 (0.50) 10.1 0.001 0.055 0.248 0.004 0.001 0.052
Visuospatial functions −0.32 (0.95) −0.02 (0.98) 0.84 (0.74) 15.5 0.001 0.082 0.233 0.004 0.001 0.075

MANCOVA F adjusted for age and education. All pairwise comparisons were also significant after Bonferroni correction except for the delayed recall score between SIVD and OS groups. Effect size is expressed as partial eta squared (ηp2).

NC, normal control subjects; OS, other stroke patients; SD, standard deviation; SIVD, subcortical ischaemic vascular disease.

graphic file with name jn69120.f1.jpg

Figure 1 Neuropsychological test performance of patients with subcortical ischaemic vascular disease (SIVD) and other stroke patients standardised according to the normal control group. Error bars indicate standard error. *p<0.05, **p<0.01.

The results were also analysed by using the raw scores of the neuropsychological tests. Pairwise comparisons between the SIVD and OS groups, as calculated with the analysis of covariance adjusted for age and education, revealed significant differences in the delayed recall of FOME (SIVD mean 7.1 (SD 2.2) v OS mean 7.9 (SD 1.9), p<0.01B*, ηp2 = 0.020), verbal fluency, animal category (14.3 (6.0) v 17.2 (6.2), p<0.01B*, ηp2 = 0.024) and letter word list generation (9.6 (5.2) v 11.7 (5.6), p<0.05B*, ηp2 = 0.014), as well as in the subtraction scores of the Trail Making test (147.5 (101.4) v 117.7 (75.9), p<0.05, ηp2 = 0.015) and the Stroop test (35.0 (28.1) v 27.2 (19.6), p<0.05B*, ηp2 = 0.019).

Discussion

In order to characterise the cognitive deficits associated with SIVD, a large sample of elderly stroke patients and healthy controls were investigated using a detailed neuropsychological battery and MRI. The patients with SIVD tended to be older and had less education than the OS patients. After controlling for these demographic factors, the results revealed both quantitative and qualitative differences in cognitive functioning between the study groups. Firstly, as compared to the NC group, the neuropsychological test performance of patients with SIVD was clearly inferior throughout cognitive domains. Secondly, despite comparable levels of global cognitive functioning in the Mini‐Mental State Examination, the patients with SIVD had significantly lower scores in executive functioning and delayed memory recall in contrast to the OS patients with larger infarct volume. However, the performance of these two patient groups did not differ in mental speed, short term memory, immediate memory recall, or verbal and visuospatial functions. Of the individual neuropsychological tests, the measures of mental flexibility, response inhibition, verbal fluency, and delayed recall of learned material significantly differentiated the patients with SIVD from the OS patients.

In previous studies, the cognitive performance of patients with variously defined subcortical vascular disease has been compared to healthy elderly subjects6,32 or to patients with mild cognitive impairment33,34 or Alzheimer's disease.7,8,32 It has been suggested that the pattern of cognitive deficits in patients with subcortical ischaemic lesions is distinctly different from that of Alzheimer's disease,7,8 and resembles Parkinson's disease with disproportionate impairment in executive functioning.35 However, studies investigating patients with mild cognitive impairment and subcortical vascular disease detected on computed tomography scan have reported diverse results.33,34 To our knowledge, the present study is the first to describe in detail the cognitive features of SIVD in comparison with elderly stroke patients. Our results indicate that executive deficits, comprising deficiency in mental flexibility, set shifting, response inhibition, and fluency, are salient cognitive characteristics in SIVD. It is notable that verbal fluency can distinguish patients with SIVD not only from those with Alzheimer's disease as suggested earlier,8 but also from other elderly stroke patients having large vessel disease and major strokes.

As reported in our previous study,5 the patients with SIVD exhibited more depressive symptoms than patients with OS. Subcortical vascular pathology has also been observed to contribute to late life depression in other studies.36 According to the vascular depression hypothesis, depressive syndrome in elderly patients can be predisposed, precipitated, or perpetuated by cerebrovascular disease.37 In our sample, however, depression scores did not explain the cognitive deficits in patients with SIVD. Although cognitive and mood changes are assumed to be parallel consequences of frontal subcortical disconnections,9,37 our results indicate that they are nevertheless independent processes associated with SIVD.

Another important finding was that controlling for MTA had no effect on the group differences in executive functioning. Instead, the patient groups no longer differed from each other in delayed memory recall after adjusting for MTA. Kramer et al32 have found a heterogeneous pattern of forgetting in patients with SIVD, and they suggest that rapid forgetting in SIVD may imply concomitant Alzheimer's disease. The patients with good retention had worse executive performance and thus were assumed to suffer from pure vascular pathology. Similarly, in our study, it is possible that the delayed memory deficits explained by MTA reflect co‐existing Alzheimer pathology, and that only the executive deficits are vascular in origin. Unfortunately, to date there are no means available to unequivocally exclude Alzheimer's disease in vivo and therefore Alzheimer pathology may also contribute to a fraction of our sample in both patient groups. On the other hand, it is not clear whether MTA explicitly indicates Alzheimer's disease or whether it equally belongs to the pathological processes in “pure” SIVD. In a post mortem study, the pattern and the degree of hippocampal neuronal loss were similar in patients with Alzheimer's disease and subcortical vascular dementia.38 It has been proposed that hippocampal atrophy in patients with SIVD is a result of a mixture of ischaemic and degenerative processes.39 Moreover, in our sample the memory deficits associated with white matter hyperintensities have earlier proven to be mediated by executive deficits and, therefore, they may reflect secondary impairment due to inefficient encoding and retrieval strategies.28

It should be noted that the effect sizes of the differences between the two patient groups were rather small. Despite specific MRI findings, there is substantial overlap in the cognitive features of the patients with SIVD and other consecutive stroke patients. The small effect sizes can also reflect high variability in cognitive performance among elderly stroke patients. Notwithstanding this heterogeneity, subtle but significant differences were found between the patient groups. Furthermore, when the patients with SIVD were compared to healthy control subjects, the group differences in cognitive functioning were moderate in magnitude. Another important point concerns the generalisability of the results. They accurately represent the clinical stroke population, but may not be entirely applicable to the general elderly population. Future studies should augment the clinical picture of MRI defined SIVD in population based settings.

The grouping of the neuropsychological test variables into cognitive domains was based on clinical experience and judgment instead of statistical factor modelling. These domains are multidimensional and also, to some extent, overlapping constructs that reflect general, but clinically relevant, aspects of cognitive functioning. In particular, executive functions are here considered as a global concept encompassing a wide array of subfunctions related to attention, volition, planning, and effective performance.21 The reliability coefficients indicated that the individual test variables in each of the composite scores had sufficient internal coherence. In addition, the results of the composite and individual scores were consistent with each other.

In conclusion, cognitive deficits in SIVD 3 months post stroke are notable in severity and can be qualitatively distinguished from those of OS patients. The results support the view that SIVD is a separate clinical entity.

Supplementary Material

[Commentary]

Acknowledgements

We thank Pertti Keskivaara, MA for statistical support.

Abbreviations

B* - significant result after Bonferroni correction

ηp2 - partial eta squared

DSM‐III‐R - Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders‐Revised

DWMH - deep white matter hyperintensities

FOME - Fuld Object Memory Evaluation

MANCOVA - multivariate analysis of covariance

MRI - magnetic resonance imaging

MTA - medial temporal lobe atrophy

NC - normal control subjects

OS - other stroke

PVH - periventricular white matter hyperintensities

SD - standard deviation

SIVD - subcortical ischaemic vascular disease

WAIS‐R - Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐Revised

WCST - Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

WMS - Wechsler Memory Scale

WMS‐R - Wechsler Memory Scale‐Revised

Footnotes

This study was financially supported by the Finnish Graduate School of Psychology

Competing interests: none declared

References

  • 1.Bowler J V. The concept of vascular cognitive impairment. J Neurol Sci 2002203–20411–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Erkinjuntti T, Inzitari D, Pantoni L.et al Limitations of clinical criteria for the diagnosis of vascular dementia in clinical trials. Is a focus on subcortical vascular dementia a solution? Ann N Y Acad Sci 2000903262–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Erkinjuntti T. Subcortical vascular dementia. Cerebrovasc Dis 200213(suppl 2)58–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Erkinjuntti T, Inzitari D, Pantoni L.et al Research criteria for subcortical vascular dementia in clinical trials. J Neural Transm 200059(suppl)23–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Pohjasvaara T, Mäntylä R, Ylikoski R.et al Clinical features of MRI‐defined subcortical vascular disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 200317236–242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kramer J H, Reed B R, Mungas D.et al Executive dysfunction in subcortical ischemic vascular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 200272217–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Schmidtke K, Hüll M. Neuropsychological differentiation of small vessel disease, Alzheimer's disease and mixed dementia. J Neurol Sci 2002203–20417–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tierney M C, Black S E, Szalai J P.et al Recognition memory and verbal fluency differentiate probable Alzheimer disease from subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Arch Neurol 2001581654–1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cummings J L. Frontal‐subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch Neurol 199350873–880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pugh K G, Lipsitz L A. The microvascular frontal‐subcortical syndrome of aging. Neurobiol Aging 200223421–431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Román G C, Erkinjuntti T, Wallin A.et al Subcortical ischaemic vascular dementia. Lancet Neurol 20021426–436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pohjasvaara T, Erkinjuntti T, Vataja R.et al Dementia three months after stroke. Baseline frequency and effect of different definitions of dementia in the Helsinki Stroke Aging Memory Study (SAM) cohort. Stroke 199728785–792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Pohjasvaara T, Erkinjuntti T, Ylikoski R.et al Clinical determinants of poststroke dementia. Stroke 19982975–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Folstein M F, Folstein S E, McHugh P R. ‘Mini‐mental state': a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 197512189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ylikoski R, Ylikoski A, Erkinjuntti T.et al White matter changes in healthy elderly persons correlate with attention and speed of mental processing. Arch Neurol 199350818–824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Leskelä M, Hietanen M, Kalska H.et al Executive functions and speed of mental processing in elderly patients with frontal or nonfrontal ischemic stroke. Eur J Neurol 19996653–661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Jokinen H, Kalska H, Ylikoski R.et al Medial temporal lobe atrophy and memory deficits in elderly stroke patients. Eur J Neurol 200411825–832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Reitan R M. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills 19588271–276. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Perret E. The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behavior. Neuropsychologia 197412323–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Nelson H E. A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 197612313–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lezak M D, Howieson D B, Loring D W.Neuropsychological assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 2004
  • 22.Wechsler D. A standardized memory scale for clinical use. J Psychol 19451987–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fuld P A.Fuld object memory evaluation. Chicago: Stoelting, 1982
  • 24.Wechsler D.WMS‐R, Wechsler memory scale‐revised manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, 1987
  • 25.Wechsler D.WAIS‐R manual. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1981
  • 26.Mäntylä R, Erkinjuntti T, Salonen O.et al Variable agreement between visual rating scales for white matter hyperintensities on MRI. Stroke 1997281614–1623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mäntylä R, Pohjasvaara T, Vataja R.et al MRI pontine hyperintensity after supratentorial ischemic stroke relates to poor clinical outcome. Stroke 200031695–700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jokinen H, Kalska H, Mäntylä R.et al White matter hyperintensities as a predictor of neuropsychological deficits post‐stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 29.Cohen J.Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1988
  • 30.American Psychiatric Association Committee on Nomenclature and Statistics Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM‐III‐R). 3rd ed., revised. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1987
  • 31.Beck A T, Ward C H, Mendelson M.et al An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 19614561–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kramer J H, Mungas D, Reed B R.et al Forgetting in dementia with and without subcortical lacunes. Clin Neuropsychol 20041832–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.de Mendoca A, Ribeiro F, Guerreiro M.et al Clinical significance of subcortical vascular disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Eur J Neurol 200512125–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Galluzzi S, Sheu C ‐ F, Zanetti O.et al Distinctive clinical features of mild cognitive impairment with subcortical cerebrovascular disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 200519196–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Libon D J, Bogdanoff B, Leopold N.et al Neuropsychological profiles associated with subcortical white matter alterations and Parkinson's disease: implications for the diagnosis of dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 20011619–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Krishnan K R R, Taylor W D, McQuoid D R.et al Clinical characteristics of magnetic resonance imaging‐defined subcortical ischemic depression. Biol Psychiatry 200455390–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Alexopoulos G S, Meyers B S, Young R C.et al ‘Vascular depression' hypothesis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 199754915–922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kril J J, Patel S, Harding A J.et al Patients with vascular dementia due to microvascular pathology have significant hippocampal neuronal loss. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 200272747–751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Fein G, Di Sclafani V, Tabane J.et al Hippocampal and cortical atrophy predict dementia in subcortical ischemic vascular disease. Neurology 2000551626–1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

[Commentary]

Articles from Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES