Abstract
In light of increasing numbers of controlled studies evaluating alcohol abuse prevention interventions for college drinkers, we conducted a meta-analysis to summarize the current status of the literature. The meta-analysis includes 62 studies, published between 1985-early 2007, with 13 750 participants and 98 intervention conditions. All studies were content coded for study descriptors, participant characteristics, and intervention components. We derived weighted mean effect sizes for alcohol interventions versus comparison conditions for consumption variables and alcohol-related problems, over four measurement intervals. Over follow-up intervals lasting up to 6 months, participants in risk reduction interventions drank significantly less relative to controls. Students receiving interventions also reported fewer alcohol-related problems over longer intervals. Moderator analyses suggest that individual, face-to-face interventions using motivational interviewing and personalized normative feedback predict greater reductions in alcohol-related problems. Implications for future research include attention to maintenance of effects, and developing more efficacious interventions for at-risk college drinkers.
Keywords: meta-analysis, college students, alcohol drinking, binge drinking, randomized clinical trials, intervention
1. Introduction
Young adults between the ages of 18-25 report high rates of alcohol consumption, including heavy episodic (binge) drinking, defined as consuming five or more drinks at a time (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration [SAMHSA], 2006). Compared to their peers not attending college, college students consume larger quantities of alcohol on drinking occasions (SAMHSA, 2006), and as many as 43% of undergraduates report heavy episodic drinking at least once in the last two weeks (Wechsler et al., 2002). Heavy episodic drinking increases risks for social and academic problems as well as risks for unintended injuries, assault, and death (Hingson, Heeren, Winter, & Wechsler, 2005; Wechsler et al., 2002). Healthy People 2010, a report outlining national priorities in health promotion and disease prevention, characterized heavy episodic drinking by young adults as a major national health problem (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2000). This report set an objective of reducing binge drinking among college students to 20% by 2010.
More than 4000 colleges and universities in the United States enroll more than 14 million undergraduate students (National Center for Education Statistics, 2005), many of whom are younger than the minimum legal drinking age of 21. Institutions of higher education are charged with upholding state and local laws, as well as promoting safe living and learning environments. A recent survey of college administrators from 747 institutions revealed that all campuses engaged in some form of alcohol abuse prevention programming; 90% provided counseling and treatment services for students, and nearly as many provided prevention services (e.g., alcohol education) for freshmen or other at-risk groups (Wechsler, Seibring, Liu, & Ahl, 2004). Given the universal nature of these prevention efforts and the numbers of students affected, it is important that college and university administrators have access to information regarding empirically-supported interventions to prevent alcohol abuse.
Several qualitative reviews have summarized the results of studies evaluating interventions to reduce heavy drinking among college students. For example, Larimer and Cronce (2002) reviewed 32 studies and found evidence of efficacy for some individual-level interventions (e.g., brief motivational interventions, cognitive-behavioral skills training) but not for others (e.g., alcohol education). More recent qualitative reviews support the efficacy of feedback-based interventions (Walters & Neighbors, 2005) and interventions for mandated students (Barnett & Read, 2005) in producing within-groups reduction on consumption and problems. A review of the literature on computer-administered alcohol prevention programs for college students found only two controlled studies with behavioral outcomes (Walters, Miller, & Chiauzzi, 2005). However, the numbers of publications in peer-reviewed journals has burgeoned in the last decade, and the numbers of controlled studies that allow strong inference are now sufficient to support a meta-analysis of this literature. A meta-analysis can clarify the current status of the efficacy literature, and help to guide future research.
This meta-analysis includes published studies that evaluated individual-level interventions designed to reduce alcohol use by college students. We recognize that limiting a review to published studies potentially excludes information (i.e., the file-drawer effect; Rosenthal, 1979); however, publication in a peer-reviewed journal serves as a proxy indicator of minimum methodological quality. To be included, studies needed to (a) be designed to reduce alcohol use and/or consequences in college drinkers; (b) use random assignment to intervention conditions, including a control condition; and (c) report behavioral outcomes related to alcohol consumption and/or alcohol-related problems. Excluded are studies evaluating interventions not administered to individuals, where the dose received could not be determined (i.e., campus-wide media or social norms campaigns).
Three decisions guided this meta-analysis. First, we report between-groups effects rather than within-groups effects; this decision recognizes that college drinking fluctuates over the course of an academic semester (Del Boca, Darkes, Greenbaum, & Goldman, 2004) as well as across years in college (e.g., Baer et al., 2001). Focusing on between-groups effects controls for maturation and historical variables; thus, we use a stringent test of efficacy to establish whether interventions influence college drinking beyond what would be expected from naturally occurring change (Campbell & Stanley, 1963). Second, we present effect sizes for multiple consumption variables and for alcohol-related problems; we aim to clarify the extent to which interventions produce changes on measures of consumption quantity, drinking frequency, indices of intoxication, and/or negative consequences resulting from drinking. Third, we evaluate effect sizes separately for follow-ups of different durations. This decision allows conclusions regarding the maintenance or duration of intervention effects. We predict that alcohol risk reduction interventions for college drinkers will produce significant between-groups effects, that risk reduction will be observed both on alcohol consumption and alcohol-related consequences, and that effects will be apparent relatively soon after intervention exposure.
When significant and heterogeneous effect sizes emerge from our meta-analysis, we plan to conduct exploratory analyses to evaluate what intervention and student characteristics predict alcohol risk reduction. The interventions obtained for this review vary with respect to theoretical underpinnings, intervention content, mode of administration (e.g., computer vs. in-person), length (one vs. multiple sessions), format (group vs. individual), and degree of tailoring. In addition, study participants vary by year in school, gender and racial/ethnic composition, and volunteer or mandated status. A meta-analysis can address whether these potential moderators influence the magnitude of effect sizes. Results from moderator analyses will allow us to draw empirically-informed conclusions regarding types of interventions and student characteristics associated with intervention efficacy.
2. Method
2.1 Search strategy and study selection
Several strategies were used to search for relevant published (or in press) manuscripts: (a) electronic reference databases (PsycINFO, PubMED, ERIC, CRISP, and the Cochrane Library) using a Boolean search strategy with the following abbreviated and full keywords: (alcohol OR drink* OR binge) AND (college OR university) and (intervention OR prevention) and (random* OR control*); (b) reference sections of relevant review or published studies; (c) examining online contents of relevant journals (e.g., Addiction, Addictive Behaviors), and (d) sending requests for published or in press manuscripts to authors.
Published (or in press) studies were included if they (a) examined any educational, behavioral, or psychological alcohol intervention; (b) sampled college or university students; (b) used a randomized controlled trial (RCT) or a quasi-experimental design with a control group; (c) assessed drinking behavior (e.g., frequency or quantity); and (d) provided sufficient information to calculate between-group effect size estimates. Consistent with these criteria, studies were excluded if the intervention did not specifically focus on alcohol (e.g., comprehensive drug and alcohol intervention; McCambridge & Strang, 2004), included non-college participants and the results were not separated (e.g., Monti et al., 1999), did not include an adequate control or comparison condition, did not measure any alcohol behavior, or provided insufficient statistical information to calculate effect sizes. When studies reported insufficient study content or statistical details, study author(s) were contacted for additional information. Of the 17 authors contacted, 94% responded resulting in the retention of 15 studies and the exclusion of 2 studies. Studies that fulfilled the search criteria and were available as of March 9, 2007 were included.
In some cases, several publications provided information about the same intervention or outcomes (e.g., an author may have published the results from the 6-month and 1-year follow-up in separate publications). In these instances, information from multiple publications reporting on the same sample was pooled for content coding, and effect sizes were calculated separately for each measurement occasion. When more than one control or comparison condition was used (e.g., standard education and wait-list), the control condition with the least contact (e.g., wait-list) was used as the comparison condition to facilitate interpretation of intervention effects. Using these criteria, 62 manuscripts with 98 separate interventions (k) qualified for the meta-analysis (Figure 1).
2.2 Study Outcomes
For each study, effect size estimates were calculated from the information provided in the report(s). Effect sizes were calculated for alcohol-related behaviors and problems. Specifically, the alcohol consumption outcomes included: (a) quantity consumed over a period time (e.g., week, month), on specific occasions (e.g., Friday night, birthday), and per drinking day; (b) maximum quantity consumed on a single occasion; (c) frequency of heavy drinking, usually defined as 5 or more drinks for men and 4 or more drinks for women (Wechsler, Davenport, Dowdall, Moeykens, & Rimm, 1995); (d) frequency of drinking days, (e) peak and/or typical blood alcohol concentration (BAC); and (e) composite alcohol consumption (when multiple indicators of alcohol use were combined). Alcohol-related problems were typically operationalized using multi-item scales.
2.3 Coding and Reliability
Two researchers independently coded overall study information (e.g., publication year), sample characteristics (e.g., ethnicity, gender, age), target group (e.g., heavy drinkers, Greek members, freshmen), design and measurement specifics (e.g., recruitment method, number of follow-ups), and content of control and intervention condition(s) (e.g., number of sessions, intervention content). Twenty studies were randomly selected to evaluate interrater reliability. For the categorical dimensions, raters agreed on 55% to 100% of the judgments (mean Cohen’s kappa = .67). Reliability for the continuous variables was calculated using the intraclass correlation coefficient (ρ); ρ ranged from .40 to 1.00, with an average ρ = 0.90 across categories (Mdn = 0.98). Because all studies were double-coded, disagreements were resolved through discussion.
2.4 Effect Size Derivation
Because the majority of the outcomes were continuous rather than dichotomous, effect sizes (d) were calculated as the mean differences between the treatment and control group divided by the pooled standard deviation (Cohen, 1988). If the pooled SD was unavailable or could not be derived from the reported statistics, the denominator was instead another form of SD (e.g., the SD of the paired comparisons). When means and standard deviations were unavailable, other statistical information (e.g., t- or F-values) was used (Johnson & Eagly, 2000; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). If a study reported dichotomous outcomes (e.g., frequencies), we calculated an odds ratio and transformed it to d using the Cox transformation (Sánchez-Meca, Marín-Martínez, & Chacón-Moscoso, 2003). If no statistical information was available (and could not be obtained from the authors) and the study reported no significant between-group differences, we estimated that effect size to be zero (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). In calculating d, we controlled for baseline differences when pre-intervention measures were available, and effect sizes were corrected for sample size bias using Hedges’ correction (Hedges, 1981). A positive sign indicated that participants in the treatment group decreased their alcohol consumption or problems compared to controls. Effect sizes were calculated using DSTAT 2.0 (Johnson & Wood, 2006).
We calculated multiple effect sizes from individual studies when they had more than one outcome, multiple intervention conditions, or when outcomes were separated by sample characteristics (e.g., gender). Effect sizes calculated for each intervention and by sample characteristic were analyzed as a separate study (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). When a study contained multiple measures of the same outcome (e.g., alcohol-related problems measured using three separate items), the effect sizes were averaged.
2. 5 Statistical Analysis
Each set of effect sizes (i.e., for each dependent variable) was examined first for extreme outliers, resulting in the exclusion of 10 out of 394 effect sizes across the 30 dependent variables where k ≥ 5 (see Table 2)i. After excluding the outliers, weighted mean effect sizes, d+s, were calculated using fixed- and random-effects procedures (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001), such that individual studies’ effect sizes were weighted by the inverse of their fixed- or random-effects. Effect size analyzes were conducted in Stata 9.0 (StataCorp, 2005) using macros provided by Lipsey and Wilson (2001).
Table 2.
Outcome | k of interventions | Weighed mean d (and 95% confidence interval) | Homogeneity of effect sizes | Variation due to heterogeneity I2 index (and 95% uncertainty interval) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fixed effects | Random effects | Q | P | |||
Immediate Posttest (≤ 3 weeks) | ||||||
Quantity | 18 | 0.19 (0.07, 0.32) | 0.20 (0.06, 0.33) | 20.53 | .36 | 9% (0%, 45%) |
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking daysa | 19 | 0.03 (-0.05, 0.11) | 0.03 (-0.05, 0.11) | 13.92 | .73 | 0% |
Maximum quantity | 6 | 0.05 (-0.12, 0.23) | 0.04 (-0.19, 0.28) | 8.96 | .11 | 44% (0%, 78%) |
Frequency of heavy drinkinga | 13 | 0.17 (0.03, 0.31) | 0.18 (0.02, 0.35) | 15.04 | .24 | 20% (0%, 58%) |
Frequency of drinking daysa | 5 | -0.08 (-0.26, 0.10) | -0.08 (-0.26, 0.10) | 3.14 | .53 | 0% |
Peak BAC | 5 | 0.41 (0.26, 0.57) | 0.36 (0.03, 0.69) | 11.66 | .02 | 66% (10%, 87%) |
Problems | 9 | 0.02 (-0.08, 0.12) | 0.02 (-0.08, 0.13) | 8.91 | .35 | 21% (0%, 64%) |
Short-term Follow-up (4 – 13 weeks) | ||||||
Quantitya | 38 | 0.13 (0.06, 0.19) | 0.14 (0.07, 0.22) | 44.98 | .17 | 18% (0%, 45%) |
Quality, specific intervals/drinking days | 22 | 0.13 (0.05, 0.21) | 0.15 (0.06, 0.21) | 23.17 | .33 | 9% (0%, 44%) |
Maximum quantity | 8 | 0.08 (-0.04, 0.20) | 0.07 (-0.07, 0.21) | 8.74 | .27 | 20% (0%, 62%) |
Frequency of heavy drinkinga | 27 | 0.18 (0.10, 0.26) | 0.21 (0.12, 0.30) | 31.03 | .23 | 16% (0%, 48%) |
Frequency of drinking days | 13 | 0.02 (-0.07, 0.10) | 0.08 (-0.06, 0.22) | 19.68 | .07 | 39% (0%, 68%) |
Peak BACa | 15 | 0.13 (0.04, 0.21) | 0.13 (0.04, 0.21) | 7.36 | .92 | 0% |
Composite alcohol consumption | 10 | 0.03 (-0.07, 0.12) | 0.04 (-0.10, 0.18) | 16.72 | .06 | 46% (0%, 74%) |
Problems | 33 | 0.15 (0.08, 0.21) | 0.16 (0.08, 0.25) | 46.31 | .05 | 31% (0%, 55%) |
Intermediate Follow-up (14 – 26 weeks) | ||||||
Quantitya | 19 | 0.11 (0.02, 0.20) | 0.11 (0.02, 0.20) | 14.60 | .69 | 0% |
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking days | 8 | 0.19 (0.08, 0.31) | 0.20 (0.06, 0.34) | 10.02 | .19 | 30% (0%, 69%) |
Frequency of heavy drinking | 12 | 0.11 (0.01, 0.22) | 0.11 (0.01, 0.22) | 11.08 | .44 | 1% (0%, 11%) |
Frequency of drinking days | 5 | -0.03 (-0.16, 0.10) | -0.03 (-0.16, 0.10) | 0.25 | .99 | 0% |
Peak BAC | 12 | 0.10 (-0.01, 0.20) | 0.11 (-0.01, 0.22) | 11.93 | .37 | 8% (0%, 46%) |
Composite alcohol consumption | 5 | 0.06 (-0.08, 0.19) | 0.06 (-0.08, 0.19) | 0.92 | .92 | 0% |
Problems | 12 | 0.22 (0.12, 0.32) | 0.22 (0.12, 0.32) | 8.04 | .71 | 0% |
Long-term Follow-up (27 – 195 weeks) | ||||||
Quantitya | 14 | 0.06 (-0.04, 0.16) | 0.06 (-0.04, 0.16) | 8.32 | .74 | 0% |
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking days | 7 | 0.07 (-0.05, 0.19) | 0.07 (-0.05, 0.19) | 3.99 | .68 | 0% |
Frequency of heavy drinking | 8 | -0.04 (-0.16, 0.08) | -0.04 (-0.16, 0.08) | 6.98 | .44 | 0% |
Frequency of drinking days | 5 | 0.16 (0.03, 0.30) | 0.16 (0.03, 0.30) | 3.06 | .55 | 0% |
Peak BAC | 10 | 0.09 (-0.02, 0.20) | 0.09 (-0.02, 0.20) | 10.53 | .31 | 15% (0%, 56%) |
Typical BAC | 5 | -0.01 (-0.16, 0.14) | -0.01 (-0.16, 0.14) | 3.50 | .48 | 0% |
Composite alcohol consumptiona | 5 | 0.11 (-0.05, 0.27) | 0.11 (-0.05, 0.27) | 1.00 | .91 | 0% |
Problemsa | 16 | 0.14 (0.06, 0.22) | 0.12 (0.01, 0.23) | 24.19 | .06 | 38% (0%, 66%) |
Note. BAC = blood alcohol concentration
Outliers were excluded.
The homogeneity statistic, Q, was computed to determine whether each set of d+s shared a common effect size. The homogeneity of variance statistics has an approximate chi-square distribution with the number of effect sizes (k) minus 1 degrees of freedom (Hedges & Olkin, 1985); a significant Q indicates a heterogeneous relationship. To further assess heterogeneity, the I2 index was calculated, to assess the proportion of total variability in a set of effect sizes attributable to true heterogeneity (Higgins & Thompson, 2002; Huedo-Mendina, Sanchez-Meca, Marin-Martinez, & Botella, 2006). Percentages of 25%, 50%, and 75%, are considered low, medium, and high heterogeneity respectively (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003). The I2 index and corresponding 95% uncertainty intervals which were calculated using published formulas (Higgins & Thompson, 2002). If the 95% uncertainty interval around the I2 index includes a zero, the set of effect sizes are considered homogeneous.
If Q remained significant after the exclusion of outliers, or the I2 index (and corresponding 95% uncertainty interval) indicated that a medium-to-large amount of between-studies variability could be explained, the relation between study characteristics and the magnitude of the effects was examined using a modified least squares regression analysis. Consistent with meta-analytic procedures (e.g., Hoffman, Papas, Chatkoff, & Kerns, 2007; Prendergast, Urada, & Podus, 2001; Scott-Sheldon & Johnson, 2006), moderator tests were conducted only if the dependent variable consisted of a minimum of six effect sizes.
3. Results
3.1 Description of Studies
Table 1 provides sample characteristics, target group, and intervention details for the 62 included studies. Studies appeared between 1985 and early 2007; the median publication year was 2004. All were written in English and were published (or in press) in journals. Studies were conducted predominately in the United States (85%). Most studies were conducted at public universities (72%) of large size (>10,000; 89%). Participants typically volunteered (71%; e.g., experimental credit or self-selection from community) to participate, although some were recruited (19%; e.g., letters sent by college official to entering freshman) or mandated for violating alcohol policy (10%). Studies usually targeted a specific group (89%), mostly heavy drinkers (65%).
Table 1.
Study | Locationa | Nb | Demographicsc | Target | Recruitd | Theorye | Controlf | Modeg | Intervention
Componentsh |
Contacts/
Total Minutesi |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Agostinelli et al. (1995) | US-SW
L public |
23 | F 48%;
W 83%, B 4%, H 4%, A 4% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | SRT | AO | P | AE, FC, FP, NC, M, MI | 1/10 |
Baer et al. (1992) | US-NW
L public |
100 | 52% F;
W 91%, A 5%, B 1% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | CBT | BV | G | AE, FC, FE, EC, ST,
MS, HR, GS, M, MI |
6/540 |
Barnett et al. (1996) | US-NW
L public |
317 | F 50%;
W 100% |
- | M | CDT,
RGT, SDT |
AO | G |
Peer Norms Education:
NC Values Clarification: VC Peer Norms + Values Clarification: NC, VC |
1/45 |
Borsari & Carey (2000) | US-NE
L private |
59 | F 57%;
W 88% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | AO | I | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
DB, MS, HR, MI |
1/60 | |
Borsari & Carey (2005) | US-NE
L private & S LAC |
60 | F 17%;
W 92% |
heavy
drinkers |
M | HR | RM | I | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
HR, GS, MI |
1/60 |
Borsari et al. (in press) | US-NE
M private |
28 | F 33%;
W 94% |
alcohol
offenders |
M | AE | I | AE, FC, FP, NC, DB,
MS, HR, GS, MI |
2/68.5 | |
Carey et al. (2006) | US-NE
L private |
496 | F 65%;
W 89%, A 5%, B 4% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | AO | I |
TLFB/BMI Enhanced:
AE, FC, FP, NC, FE, DB, MS, HR, GS, MI |
2/120 | |
Enhanced BMI: AE, FC,
FP, NC, FE, DB, MS, HR, GS, MI |
1/60 | |||||||||
TLFB/Basic BMI: AE,
FC, FP, NC, FE, MS, HR, MI |
2/120 | |||||||||
Basic BMI: AE, FC, FP,
NC, FE, MS, HR, MI |
1/60 | |||||||||
TLFB/Control | 1/60 | |||||||||
Chiauzzi et al. (2005) | US-NE
4 M/L private & public |
215 | F 54%;
W 73%, A 11%, H 8%, B 3% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | RM | C | AE, FC, FP, NC, MS,
HR |
4/80 | |
Collins & Carey (2005) | US-NE
L private |
84 | F 63%;
W 92% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | DMT | AO | I |
In-Person Decisional
Balance: DB, GS, MI |
1/30 |
P |
Written Decisional Balance:
DB, GS, MI |
|||||||||
Collins et al. (2002) | US-NE
L private |
94 | F 50%;
W 86% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | AE | P | AE, FC, FP, NC, M | 1/15 | |
Corbin et al. (2001) | US-SE
L public |
62 | F 50% | moderate to
heavy drinkers |
V | monitor-
only |
G | AE, FC, FE, ST | 3/255 | |
Cronin (1996) | US-SE
S LAC |
128 | F 52%;
W 100% |
- | V | HR | AO | G | AE, HR, GS | 1/15 |
Curtin et al. (2001) | SU-SE
L public |
74 | F 100%;
W 95% |
female heavy
drinkers |
V | SCT,
SRT |
AO | I |
No Goal/Feedback: AE,
FC, FP, MS, M Proximal Goal/Feedback: AE, FC, MS, GS, M Distal Goal/Feedback: AE, FC, MS, GS, M Proximal Goal/No Feedback: AE, MS, GS Distal Goal/No Feedback: AE, MS, GS |
2/60 |
Darkes & Goldman (1993) | US-SE
L-public |
50 | F 0%;
W 95%, B 3%, H 3% |
heavy
drinking males |
V | NNT | AO | G |
Expectancy Challenge: AE,
FE, EC Traditional Information: AE, MS |
3/315 |
Darkes & Goldman (1998) | US-SE
L-public |
54 | F 0%;
W 87%, A 6%, H 6%, B 2% |
heavy
drinking Males |
V | NNT | INM | G |
Social/sexual challenge:
FE, EC Affective/cognitive challenge: FE, EC |
4/240 |
Dimeff & McNeely (2000) | US-NW
L public |
33 | F 62%;
W 74%, A 10%, B 2% |
heavy
drinkers |
R | AO | C | AE, FC, FP, NC, MS, M | 1/15 | |
Donohue et al. (2004) | US-SW
L public |
104 | F 56%;
W 63%, A 13%, H 12%, B 8% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | CBT | RNM | C | AE, NC, DB, ST, HR | 1/45 |
Fromme & Corbin (2004) | US-SW
L public |
106 |
Disciplinary
Sample: F 24%; W 75%, H 12%, A 7%, B 2% |
heavy
drinkers |
M | CBT, SC | AO | G |
Peer Lifestyle Management
Class: AE, FC, FP, NC, ST, MS, GS, M, MI |
2/240 |
332 |
Volunteer sample:
F 41%; W 59%, H 18%, A 17%, B 5% |
- | V | G |
Professional Lifestyle
Management Class: AE, FC, FP, NC, ST, MS, GS, M, MI |
|||||
Garvin et al. (1990) | US-SE | 60 | F 0% | fraternity
members |
V | AO | G |
Behavior Self-Management:
AE, ST, MS |
3/135 | |
G | Alcohol Education: AE | 3/135 | ||||||||
I | Monitoring Only | 0/0 | ||||||||
Glindermann et al. (2007) | US-SE
L public |
366 | F 32% | party
attendee |
V | AE | I | AE, MS, M | 1/5 | |
Hansson et al. (2006) | Sweden
L public |
78 | F 72% | students
with alcoholic parents |
V | CBT | IM | I |
Alcohol Program: AE, FC,
NC, FE, MS, HR, GS Alcohol + Coping Program: AE, FC, NC, FE, MS, HR, GS |
2/240 |
Hernandez et al. (2006) | US-SW;
L public |
86 | F 40%;
H 100% |
drinkers | V | HR | BV | 2 | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
ST, MS, HR, MI |
1/120 |
Howat et al. (1991) | Australia
L public |
48 | F 0% | heavy
drinking males |
AO | I | AE, EC, M | 2/72 | ||
Ingersoll et al. (2005) | US-SE;
L public |
199 | F 100%;
Ethnicity: 70% W, 16% B, 6% A, 2% H |
heavy
drinkers at risk for alcohol- exposed pregnancy |
V | AE | I | FC, FP, DB, HR, GS,
MI |
1/67.5 | |
Johnsson & Berglund (2003) | Sweden;
L public |
658 | F 32% | pub patrons | V | AO | I |
Bartender Program:
AE, NC, FE, ST |
5/720 | |
Johnsson & Berglund (2006) | Sweden;
L public |
149 | F 25% | heavy
drinking freshman |
V | CBT | BV | G | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
EC, MS, HR, GS, MI |
5/600 |
Jones et al. (1995) | US-SE;
M private |
90 | F 46%;
W 90% |
drinkers | V | SLT | IM | G |
Expectancy Challenge: AE,
EC |
2/120 |
Expectancy Challenge +
Inoculation: AE, FE, EC |
2/120 | |||||||||
Juarez et al. (2006) | US-SW;
L public |
89 | F 53%;
W 57%, H 30% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | AO | I |
MI + Immediate Feedback:
AE, FC, FP, NC, FE, DB, GS, MI |
1/105 | |
I | MI: AE, DB, GS, MI | 1/50 | ||||||||
I + P |
MI + Mailed Feedback:
AE, FC, FP, NC, FE, DB, GS, M, MI |
1/60 | ||||||||
P |
Mailed Feedback: AE, FC,
FP, NC, FE, M |
0/10 | ||||||||
Keillor et al. (1999) | US-SW;
L public |
25 | F 0% | heavy
drinking males |
M | RM | G | FE, EC | 2/180 | |
Kivlahan et al. (1990) | US-NW;
L public |
36 | F 42%;
W 91%, B 5%, A 2%, H 2% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | CBT,
SLT |
AO | G |
Cognitive-Behavioral Skills:
AE, FC, FP, EC, ST, MS, HR, GS Alcohol Information: AE |
8/720 |
Kypri & McAnally (2005) | New
Zealand; L public |
126 | F 51%;
W 75% |
- | R | AO | C | AE, FC | 1/10 | |
Kypri et al. (2004) | New
Zealand; L public |
94 | F 50%;
W 91% |
heavy
drinkers |
R | AO | C | AE, FC, FP, NC, M, MI | 1/7.8 | |
Larimer et al. (2001) | US-NW;
L public |
120 | F 0%;
W 82%, A 13%, H 1% |
freshman
fraternity members |
V | HBM,
SM |
AE | I + G | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
DB, MS, GS |
2/120 |
Lysaught et al. (2003) | US-NE;
colleges |
55 | F 53%;
W 78%, A 8%, B 7%, H 2% |
- | V | SCT | AO | I | AE, FC, FP, M | 1/10 |
Marlatt et al. (1998) | US-NW;
L public |
299 | F 53%;
W 85%, A 9%, H 3%, B 1% |
freshman
heavy drinkers |
R | AO | I | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
MS, M, MI |
1/60 | |
McNally & Palfai (2003) | US-NE;
L private |
76 | F 65%;
W 77%, A 8%, H 5%, B 4% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | CDT,
SDT, SNT |
IM | G |
Actual-Ideal Discrepancies:
DB, VC, MS, GS, MI Social Norms: AE, NC, MI |
1/40 |
McNally et al. (2005) | US-NE;
private |
73 | F 71%;
W 85%, A 8%, H 3% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | CDT | IM | I | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
DB, MS, GS, M, MI |
1/30 |
Michael et al. (2006) | US-SE;
L public |
91 | F 63%;
W 94%, B 4%, A 1%, H 1% |
freshman | V | AO | G | AE, NC, DB, VC, GS,
MI |
1 - 2/50 - 75 | |
Moore et al. (2005) | US-SE;
L public |
106 | F 58%;
W 70%, H 10%, A 9%, B 8% |
- | V | EPPM,
HBM, SCT |
RM | C | AE, FE, MS | 4/60 |
Murgraff et al. (2006) | UK | 347 | F 70% | moderate
drinkers |
V | SCT,
TPB |
AO | P | AE, NC, MS, GS | 1/10 |
Murgraff et al. (1996) | UK | 102 | F 74% | heavy
drinkers |
V | TE | BV | G | AE, MS, GS, M | 1/15 |
J. G. Murphy et al. (2004) | US-SE;
L public |
51 | F 69%;
W 94% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | RM | I | AE, FC, FP, NC, GS,
M, MI |
1/40 | |
J. G. Murphy et al. (2001) | US-SE;
L public |
79 | F 54%;
W 94% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | SCT | AO | I |
BASICS: AE, FC, FP,
NC, FE, VC, MS, GS, M, MI Education: AE, M |
1/50 |
T. J. Murphy et al. (1986) | US-NW;
L public |
31 | F 0% | heavy
drinking males |
V | AO | I + G |
Exercise
Meditation |
24/1680
24/480 |
|
Musher-Eizenman & Kulick (2003) | US-MW;
L public |
54 | F 100%;
W 94%, B 4%, H 2% |
moderate to
heavy drinking females |
V | SLT | RM | G |
Social/Sexual: AE, FE,
EC, M Cognitive/Motor: AE, FE, EC, M |
3/315 |
Neal & Carey (2004) | US-NE;
L private |
92 | F 55%;
W 85%, A 5%, B 2%, H 2% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | SRT | RM | G |
Feedback: FC, FP, NC,
M, MI Personal Strivings Assessment: GS, MI |
1/40 |
Neighbors et al. (2004) | US-NW;
L private |
207 | F 59%;
W 80%, A 14% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | SNT | AO | C | FC, NC, M | 1/5 |
Neighbors et al. (2006) | US-MW;
M private |
185 | F 56%;
W 98% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | SNT,
SDT |
AO | C | FC, NC, M | 1/5 |
Neighbors et al. (2005) | US-MW;
L public |
164 | F 59%;
W 96% |
students
turning 21 |
R | AO | P | HR, M | 1/5 | |
Peeler et al. (2000) | US-NW;
L public |
157 | F 62%;
W 84% |
- | V | SNT | IM | G | AE, NC, ST, HR | 13/650 |
Rohsenow et al. (1985) | US-NW;
L public |
34 | F 0% | heavy
drinking males |
CBT | AO | I + G | ST | 6/360 | |
Saitz et al. (2006) | US-NE;
L private |
235 | F 55%;
W 81%, A 11%, H 6% |
heavy
drinking freshman |
R | SCA,
SNT |
AE | C | AE, FC, FP, NC, DB,
MI |
1/30 |
Smith et al. (2006) | US-SE;
L public |
444 |
Cohort #1:
F 58%; W 72%, B 21% |
students
turning 21 |
R | HR,
SNT |
IM | P |
BRAD Card: AE, MS,
HR, M Social Norm Card: NC, M |
1/5 |
550 |
Cohort #2:
F 56%; W 74%, B 19% |
Information Card: MS, M
Combination Card: NC, MS, M |
||||||||
Stahlbrandt et al. (2007) | Sweden;
L public |
371 | F 36% | resident hall
students |
R | CBT | AO | G |
Brief Skills Training: AE,
FC, FP, NC, FE, HR, M, MI |
2/190 |
CBT | AO | G |
12-Step: AE, FC, FP,
NC, M, MI |
|||||||
Stamper et al. (2004) | US-SE;
L public |
874 | F 61%;
W 75%, B 18%, A 3%, H 1% |
freshman | V | SNT | BV | G | AE, FC, NC, FE, VC | 1/60 |
Tevyaw et al. (2007) | US-NE;
M private |
28 | F 34%;
W 85% |
alcohol
offenders |
R | RM | G | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
DB, MS, GS, M, MI |
1/90 | |
Walters et al. (2000) | US-SW;
L public |
37 | F 40%;
W 62%, H 30% |
heavy
drinkers |
V | AO | G + P |
Full Treatment AE, FC,
FP, NC, DB, VC, ST, MS, M, MI |
2/130 | |
P |
Mailed Feedback: AE, FC,
FP, NC, M, MI |
1/10 | ||||||||
Walters et al. (in press) | US-SW;
S & L public |
82 | F 48%;
W 73% |
heavy
drinking freshman |
V | SCT,
SNT |
AO | C | AE, FC, FP, NC, MI | 1/12.5 |
Werch et al. (2000) | US-SE;
L public |
521 | F 64%;
W 83%, B 11%, H 3% |
freshman | V | SNT | AO | P | AE, NC, HR, M | 3/15 |
White et al. (2006) | US-NE;
L private |
319 | F 40%;
W 79%, A 16%, B 2% |
alcohol
offenders |
M | BV | I | AE, FC, FP, NC, FE,
MS, GS, M, MI |
1/30 |
Location refers to university location, including size and type (public, private, liberal arts college). US = United States; UK = United Kingdom; NE = Northeast; SE = Southeast; MW = Midwest; SE = Southeast; SW = Southwest; S = Small; M = Medium; L = Large; LAC = Liberal Arts College.
N refers to the largest available n at a follow-up measurement occasion, not initial N.
Demographics: F = female; A = Asian; B = Black; H = Hispanic/Latina; W = White.
Recruitment method: V = volunteered; R = recruited; M = mandated.
Stated theory used: CBT = Cognitive-Behavioral Theory; CDT = Cognitive-Dissonance Theory; DMT = decision-making theory; EPPM = Extended Parallel Processing Model; HBM = Health Belief Model; HR = harm-reduction; NNT = neural network theory; RGT = Reference Group Theory; SC = Stages of Change; SCA = self-change approach; SCT = Social-Cognitive Theory; SD = Self-Determination Theory; SDT = Self-Discrepancy Theory; SLT = Social Learning Theory; SM = self-monitoring; SNT = Social Normative Theory; SOC = Stages of Change; SRT = Self Regulation Theory; TE = theories of enactment; TPB = Theory of Planned Behavior.
Type of control/comparisons condition: AO = assessment-only; AE = alcohol-education; BV = brief version of intervention; IM = irrelevant content, time-matched; INM = irrelevant content; not time-matched; RM = relevant content, time-matched.
Mode of intervention delivery: C = computer/internet; G = group; I = individual; P = print.
Intervention components: AE = alcohol education; DB = decisional balance; EC = expectancy challenge; FC = feedback on consumption; FE = feedback on expectancies and/or motives; FP = feedback on problems; GS = goal-setting; HR = identification of high-risk situations; M = provided materials; MI = motivational interviewing; MS = moderation strategies; NC = normative comparisons; ST = skills-training; VC = values clarification.
Estimated dosage excluding measurement.
All studies randomly assigned participants or groups of participants (e.g., classrooms) to intervention conditions; 94% evaluated participants at both pre- and post-test with an average of 1.58 (SD = 0.86) follow-ups after baseline (range = 1 to 5; Mdn = 1). The first post-intervention assessment period occurred an average of 6.69 weeks (SD = 10.92, range = 0 to 52 weeks; k = 97) after the intervention, the next assessment took place 22.70 weeks (SD = 22.06; range = 3 to 104 weeks; k = 46) post-intervention, the third averaged 52.30 weeks (SD = 19.88; range = 24 to 91 weeks; k = 10) post-intervention, and the fourth averaged 82.33 weeks (SD = 52.54; range = 52 to 143 weeks; k = 3) post-intervention, with a single follow-up reported at 195 weeks (k = 1). Because the timing of follow-ups varied widely across studies, we divided outcomes into four measurement intervals: (a) immediate posttest (assessments ≤ 3 weeks; k = 43), (b) short-term follow-up (4 to 13 weeks; k = 61), (c) intermediate follow-up (14 to 26 weeks; k = 24), and (d) long-term follow-up (27 to 195 weeks; k = 20).
3.2 Description of Samples
A total of 13 750 college students participated in the studies, with a retention rate of 75% at follow-up (based on the largest available n at any follow-up). Studies sampled about equal numbers of males (53%) and females (47%) and 81% White participants with a mean age of 20.37 years (SD = 1.98; range = 18 to 26). Of the 39 studies reporting participants’ year in college, most were freshman (66%) or sophomores (17%). Eleven studies reported participants’ Greek membership status; of these, 91% of the samples consisted of Greek members and/or pledges.
3.3 Description of Intervention Conditions
In the 62 studies included in the meta-analysis, 98 separate intervention conditions were evaluated. Studies consisted of an average of 1.87 (SD = 1.05; range = 1 to 5; Mdn = 2) intervention conditions. Most interventions involved face-to-face delivery by a facilitator to an individual or a group (70%), with a minority using computer or print delivery (22%), or a combination (7%). Furthermore, interventions delivered in groups (44%) consisted of a median of 2 sessions, of 50 minutes each, with a median of 1 facilitator and 9.8 participants; individually delivered interventions (40%) consisted of a median of 2 sessions, of 50 minutes each, with a median of 1 facilitator and 1 participant; and interventions with no face-to-face contact (e.g., computer/internet, mailing; 30%) consisted of a median of 1 session of 15 minutes with no facilitators and 1 participant.
Most interventions were informed by theory (82%) and guided by manuals (61%); manuals were used in 66% of the interventions delivered face-to-face, 46% of the interventions delivered via computer or print, and 57% of the combined intervention delivery modes. Motivational interviewing techniques were used in 44% of the interventions. Intervention components frequently included alcohol/BAC education (73%), normative comparisons (56%), and feedback on consumption (49%); they often included moderation strategies (43%), feedback on problems (37%), goal setting (35%), or feedback on expectancies and/or motives (34%). Less frequent components included identification of high-risk situations (24%), decisional balance exercisesii (17%), skills-training (16%), an expectancy challengeiii (12%), or values clarificationiv (7%). Alcohol-related materials (e.g., brochures, pamphlets) were provided in 47% of the interventions. Intervention content was typically tailored to the participants (86%); tailoring occurred at the level of the individual (56%, e.g., personalized feedback), the group (27%; e.g., Greeks or freshmen), or to both the individual and group (3%). Of the 73 interventions delivered face-to-face, session leaders were professionals-in-training (66%), professionals (21%), peers (18%), or paraprofessionals (12%); some interventions used more than one type of facilitator.
3.4 Description of the Comparison Conditions
Risk reduction interventions were compared most often to a no-treatment condition (i.e., assessment-only, 55%); active comparison conditions involved a time-matched irrelevant intervention (16%), time-matched relevant intervention (10%), briefer but relevant intervention (10%), or alcohol education (8%). Of the 44 interventions using an active comparison condition, these typically consisted of an individual session lasting 20 minutes. Alcohol education materials were provided to 29% of the control/comparison conditions.
3.5 Intervention Effects
Table 2 provides the weighted mean effect sizes by measurement interval comparing interventions with any comparison condition.v At immediate follow-up, intervention participants reduced their quantity of drinking (d+ = 0.19, 95% CI 0.07, 0.32), frequency of heavy drinking (d+ = 0.17, 95% CI 0.03, 0.31), and peak BAC (d+ = 0.41, 95% CI 0.26, 0.57) compared to controls. All of these effects were parallel using fixed- or random-effects assumptions. Homogeneity analyses (Q) indicated that the effects for drinking quantity and frequency of heavy drinking were homogenous (ps >.05); examination of the I2 index also confirmed homogeneity. Although significant heterogeneity was found for peak BAC, Q (4) = 11.66, p = .02, and examination of the I2 index indicated moderate heterogeneity, the small number of effect sizes (k = 5) precluded moderator analyses.
Results for the short-term follow-up (4 – 13 weeks post-intervention) showed that intervention participants reduced their quantity of drinking (d+ = 0.13, 95% CI 0.06, 0.19; Figure 2), quantity for specific time intervals/drinking days (d+ = 0.13, 95% CI 0.05, 0.21), frequency of heavy drinking (d+ = 0.18, 95% CI 0.10, 0.26), peak BAC (d+ = 0.13, 95% CI 0.04, 0.21), and alcohol-related problems (d+ = 0.15, 95% CI 0.08, 0.21; Figure 3). Again, these effects were parallel using fixed- or random-effects assumptions. All significant effects were homogenous except for alcohol-related problems, Q (30) = 45.27, p = .04. The I2 index for alcohol-related problems indicated a low to moderate level of heterogeneity (34%); however, the uncertainty interval for the I2 index contained a zero suggesting true homogeneity. Nonetheless, when the results of the Q and I2 index do not agree, moderator analyses are recommended (T. B. Huedo-Mendina, personal communication, March 21, 2007).
At intermediate follow-up (14 – 26 weeks post-intervention), intervention participants reduced quantity of alcohol consumed (d+ = 0.11, 95% CI 0.02, 0.20), quantity for specific time intervals/drinking days (d+ = 0.19, 95% CI 0.08, 0.31), and frequency of heavy drinking (d+ = 0.11, 95% CI 0.01, 0.22). They also reduced alcohol-related problems (d+ = 0.22, 95% CI 0.12, 0.32) compared to control participants.
At long-term follow-up (27 – 195 weeks post-intervention), frequency of drinking days (d+ = 0.16, 95% CI 0.03, 0.30) and alcohol-related problems were reduced (d+ = 0.14, 95% CI 0.06, 0.22) among intervention participants compared with controls. Results were parallel using fixed- or random-effects assumptions and all effects were homogeneous.
3.6 Moderators of Alcohol-Related Problems at Short-Term Follow-Up
Weighted least squares regression analyses were conducted to examine potential moderators of alcohol-related problems at short-term follow-up. We created indicator variables to represent sample characteristics (2: percent women; percent white), target group (2: heavy drinkers; and other at-risk group including Greeks, athletes, freshman, party attendees, and students turning 21), recruitment method (3: volunteered, recruited, mandated), intervention modality (2: individual, group), contact context (3: in-person, computer/mail, both), tailoring of the intervention (2: individual-level, group-level), control type (1: assessment-only vs. any comparison condition) and intervention content (13: See Table 1 for list of intervention components). Therefore, 28 separate univariate regression analyses were conducted; 12 were statistically significant. Interventions were more successful at reducing alcohol-related problems at short-term follow-up if the sample contained more women (B = 0.56, p = 0.01); interventions were delivered individually (B = 0.28, p <.001), or in-person (B = 0.17, p = .01); motivational interviewing techniques were used (B = 0.21, p <.01); and the intervention content contained normative feedback (B = 0.17, p =.01), feedback on expectancies and/or motives for drinking (B = 0.27, p <.001), or a decisional balance exercise (B = 0.17, p =.05). Interventions were less successful if they targeted heavy drinkers (B = -0.34, p <.001) or any other at-risk group (B = -0.14, p = .05), when the intervention was delivered via computer/mailing (B = -0.22, p <.01) and the intervention content included an expectancy challenge exercise (B = -0.20, p <.01) or skills-training (B = -0.28, p <.001).
4. Discussion
To our knowledge, this study provides the first meta-analytic integration of the results of randomized clinical trials designed to evaluation alcohol abuse prevention programs for college drinkers. A compelling benefit of meta-analysis is that it can provide a powerful method to test effects across many studies, each of which may not be adequately powered to detect intervention effects at standard conventions of significance. Meta-analysis also affords the opportunity to identify moderators when a set of effect sizes demonstrates heterogeneity. Our goals were to characterize the efficacy of interventions for college student drinkers across outcomes and follow-up intervals. Three major findings emerged: (a) individual-level alcohol interventions for college drinkers reduce alcohol use; (b) these interventions also reduce alcohol-related problems, and reductions in problems vary by sample and intervention characteristics; and (c) the contrast between students who receive interventions and those in control conditions diminishes over time. Each of these findings will be discussed in turn.
The primary finding of this meta-analysis is that students who received risk reduction interventions subsequently engaged in less extreme drinking behavior than students in control conditions. Significant effect sizes were observed across multiple measures of alcohol consumption, representing quantity frequency, and measures of intoxication, with d+s ranging from 0.11 to 0.41. Conventionally, these effect sizes are in the range of small to medium (Cohen, 1988). Notably, a meta-analytic review of 62 meta-analyses with nearly 600 000 participants conducted across health areas revealed an average weighted mean effect size of 0.20 (k = 13) across addictions interventions (Johnson, Scott-Sheldon, & Carey, 2007). Thus, the magnitudes of the observed effects are consistent with those found in the broader addictions treatment literature. Our findings demonstrate clearly that alcohol risk reduction interventions of various forms reliably reduce quantity and frequency of drinking by college students.
A second and equally important finding is that alcohol risk reduction interventions succeed in reducing alcohol-related problems reported by college drinkers. Negative consequences of alcohol misuse for the drinker can occur across one or more functional domains (e.g., social, physical, academic, legal; Perkins, 2002). In addition, negative consequences such as drinking and driving, property damage, fights, and alcohol poisoning also have implications for other people and the institution. Thus, reductions in negative consequences not only benefit the drinker but also benefit others. Effect sizes for problems were d+ = 0.15 (at short-term follow-up), peaking at d+ = 0.22 (at intermediate follow-up), and d+ = 0.14 (at long-term follow-up), maintaining significance into follow-ups lasting over six months. Effect sizes for alcohol-related problems tended to be homogenous with one exception; that is, at short-term follow-up we did observe heterogeneity among the effect sizes. We explored whether systematic variation across sample characteristics, and intervention components could explain variability within effect size estimates.
Several sample characteristics moderated alcohol-related problems at short-term follow-up. Interventions were more successful in reducing alcohol-related problems when more females were sampled. Female students generally experience fewer alcohol related problems than do male students (American College Health Association, 2007). More importantly, women may be less reliant on drinking situations and drinking “buddies” to meet their social needs (Borsari & Carey, 2006). Also, female students may be more willing to participate and respond to alcohol interventions compared with male students; research in community samples demonstrates that heavy drinking women respond more positively to brief interventions than do heavy drinking men (Sanchez-Craig, Spivak, & Davila, 1991).
Interventions were less successful in reducing problems (compared with controls) when they were targeted to heavy drinkers or other high risk groups. Such students are likely to have heavy drinking peers and be embedded in more alcohol-involved social networks (e.g., Reifman, Watson, & McCourt, 2006). Drinking in high risk groups may serve functions different than drinking in the general student population, requiring tailored interventions that address deep structure, such as core beliefs, values, and norms (Resnicow et al., 2000). Overall, these findings highlight the need to develop more efficacious interventions for heavy drinking students and those who belong to other at-risk groups such as Greeks and athletes.
Intervention characteristics also predicted variability in problems outcomes. Specifically, interventions delivered to individuals rather than groups, and interventions that used motivational interviewing, provided feedback on expectancies or motives, normative comparisons, and included decisional balance exercises were more successful at reducing alcohol-related problems than a range of comparison conditions. This set of components represents the core of brief motivational intervention packages, many modeled after the Brief Alcohol Screening and Intervention for College Students (BASICS) protocol developed by Dimeff, Baer, Kivlahan, and Marlatt (1999). In adult samples, brief motivational interventions for alcohol abuse using feedback produce small to medium effects when compared to no-treatment controls (Burke et al., 2003). Our findings provide indirect evidence of their efficacy with young adult drinkers. In contrast, interventions that included skills training or expectancy challenge components were less successful at reducing alcohol-related problems, relative to control interventions. These intervention components appeared relatively less frequent than others in the studies reviewed (23% and 16% respectively); however, it is unclear why they would predict less change in problems. It is worth noting that the presence of these intervention components was correlated with samples made up of heavy drinkers; thus, participant characteristics might explain the smaller intervention effects.
The third major finding is that effect size magnitude diminishes over time. The number of significant effect sizes peaked at the short-term (4-13 weeks) and intermediate term (14-26 weeks) follow-ups. Between-groups effects on consumption (quantity, heavy drinking frequency, and peak BAC) were observed immediately post-intervention, but few persist beyond 6 months (an exception is frequency of drinking days at long-term follow-up). In contrast, reduction in alcohol-related problems takes longer to emerge and continues into long-term follow-ups. This delayed response may be due to the lower base rate of problems relative to drinking occasions, even for heavier drinkers, requiring more time for reductions in problems to be noticed. The overall pattern suggests that interventions reduce consumption within one month, but between-groups differences cease to be significant after 6 months. It is worth noting that studies with large samples have reported reductions in drinking within assessment-only control conditions over periods of one to two years (e.g., Carey et al., 2006; Marlatt et al., 1998), perhaps reflecting naturally-occurring risk reduction associated with greater experience and maturity. Thus, one interpretation consistent with the observed reductions in effect size over time is that participation in an intervention prompts rapid risk reduction, but that controls “catch up” over time. Alternatively, initial risk reduction may deteriorate over time in intervention conditions. Few studies analyzed their longitudinal outcome data in such a way as to describe trends over time (see Carey et al., 2006 for an exception). As a result, the observed pattern across studies may reflect either a deterioration of intervention effects or an improvement in control group behavior.
The primary limitation is that the numbers of studies available were too few to allow evaluation of some predictors. For example, few studies investigated some intervention components (e.g., computer-administered interventions, peer facilitators); thus, evaluation of their unique relationship to intervention effects was not possible. Similarly, the small number of studies could not support multivariate predictor models that would allow evaluation of independent contributions, or interactions among predictors. Thus, our predictor analyses should be considered preliminary, and an underestimate of the potential promise of some intervention components.
These findings have implications for college drinking interventions development. First, future interventions should be designed to evaluate maintenance of effects. The typical study in this sample of 62 reported only one follow-up; future studies should evaluate maintenance of intervention effects over periods of 6-12 months. Second, investigators should explore ways of enhancing the efficacy of interventions to achieve larger between-group effect sizes. More explicit consideration of the developmental context and functions of drinking among adolescents and young adults (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2007) will likely produce stronger effects. For example, given the social-affiliative function of drinking among college students, interventions that incorporate peers (e.g., Tevyaw et al., 2007) or pre-existing social groups (Larimer et al., 2001) warrant greater attention. Both efficacy and maintenance may be enhanced by supplementing individual-focused interventions with environmental changes. Multi-level interventions include coordinated media, law enforcement, and community initiatives (National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2004). Evaluation of multi-level intervention strategies may require partnership between researchers with individual- and population-level expertise. Third, the finding that student and intervention characteristics explain heterogeneity of effects on problems suggests two implications for research. One is the use of components analysis (Kazdin, 1994) to reveal additive or interactive effects of intervention components. Another is continued investigation of participant characteristics that might moderate intervention response.
In sum, providing interventions to reduce hazardous drinking by college students is clearly worthwhile. These results demonstrate that risk reduction interventions for college drinkers result in significantly less drinking over follow-up intervals lasting up to 6 months. The interventions summarized in this meta-analysis also averted alcohol-related problems over longer intervals. Moderator analyses suggest that individually-administered interventions, providing feedback and normative comparisons are most likely to reduce alcohol-related problems over time. Needed are more efficacious interventions for at-risk students, and interventions that promote maintenance of risk reduction.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grants K02-AA15574 and R01-AA12518 to Kate B. Carey. The authors thank Jennifer Elliott for her assistance with this project. We thank the following study authors for providing additional intervention or statistical information: Arthur W. Blume, PhD., William R. Corbin, PhD, Lisa Curtin, PhD, John Darkes, PhD, Kim Fromme, PhD, Helena Hansson, PhD, Kyp Kypri, PhD, James G. Murphy, PhD, Dara R. Musher-Eizenman, PhD, Richard Saitz, MD, Scott C. Walters, PhD, Chudley C. Werch, PhD, Helene R. White, PhD, and John S. Wodarski, PhD.
Footnotes
Although there are no formal guidelines regarding the minimum number of effect sizes necessary to calculate weighted mean effect sizes, outcomes with fewer than five interventions at a measurement interval are not reported due to potential interpretation bias from a single intervention. This resulted in the exclusion of typical BAC at immediate (k = 0), short-term (k = 2), and intermediate (k = 1) follow-up; maximum quantity at intermediate (k = 3) and long-term (k = 1) follow-up; and composite alcohol consumption at immediate follow-up (k = 4). None of the weighted mean effect sizes (when k ≥ 2) were significant.
Decisional balance refers to an exercise in which pros and cons (or costs and benefits) of a behavior are elicited and discussed. Often pros and cons of change options are also elicited, and the overall “balance” between continuing the behavior (e.g., high risk drinking) and the change option (e.g., drinking less) is evaluated (e.g., Dimeff et al., 1999).
Expectancy challenge is an intervention designed to illustrate the effects of alcohol-related expectancies through experiential learning. As originally developed by Darkes and Goldman (1993), the procedure involves providing beverages to groups of drinkers into a bar-like setting; some contain alcohol and others contain a placebo beverage, but the participants do not know the content of their drinks. They engage in activities that promote social interaction, and after time passes, participants guess who was drinking alcohol versus a placebo. Incorrect identification provides opportunities to consider the effects of alcohol attributable to expectancies. The procedure may be repeated, and information about the effects of alcohol may also be provided.
Values clarification refers to exercises designed to help participants identify their values and attitudes regarding alcohol (particularly about the acceptability of drinking or certain types of drinking), and to explore how they express those values in their behavior.
We further examined the weighted mean effect sizes separated by type of control condition (no-contact vs. any contact). The pattern of results was similar.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis. Supplement papers (e.g., intervention details, additional measurement occasions) are also asterisked and the reference paper is indicated in brackets.
- *.Agostinelli G, Brown JM, Miller WR. Effects of normative feedback on consumption among heavy drinking college students. Journal of Drug Education. 1995;25:31–40. doi: 10.2190/XD56-D6WR-7195-EAL3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American College Health Association. American College Health Association National College Health Assessment Spring 2006 Reference Group data report (abridged) Journal of American College Health. 2007;55:195–206. doi: 10.3200/JACH.55.4.195-206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Blume AW, McKnight P, Marlatt GA. Brief intervention for heavy-drinking college students: 4-year follow-up and natural history. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91:1310–1316. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.8.1310. Additional measurement occasions for Marlatt et al., 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Marlatt GA. High-risk drinking across the transition form high school to college. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1995;19:54–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01472.x. Sample details for Marlatt et al., 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Baer JS, Marlatt GA, Kivlahan DR, Fromme K, Larimer ME, Williams E. An experimental test of three methods of alcohol risk reduction with young adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1992;60:974–979. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.60.6.974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Fromme K, Marlatt GA. Secondary prevention of alcohol abuse with college student populations: A skills-training approach. In: Heather N, Miller WR, Greeley L, editors. Self-control and the addictive behaviors. New York: Macmillan; 1991. pp. 339–356. Intervention details for Baer et al., 1992. [Google Scholar]
- *.Barnett LA, Far JM, Mauss AL, Miller JA. Changing perceptions of peer norms as a drinking reduction program for college students. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 1996;41:39–62. [Google Scholar]
- Barnett NP, Read JP. Mandatory alcohol intervention for alcohol-abusing college students: a systematic review. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2005;29:147–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari BE, Carey KB. Understanding fraternity drinking: Five recurring themes in the literature, 1980-1998. Journal of American College Health. 1999;48:30–37. doi: 10.1080/07448489909595669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Borsari B, Carey KB. Effects of a brief motivational intervention with college student drinkers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:728–733. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Borsari B, Carey KB. Two brief alcohol interventions for mandated college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19:296–302. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.3.296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Carey KB. How the quality of peer relationships influences college alcohol use. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2006;25:361–370. doi: 10.1080/09595230600741339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Borsari B, Tevyaw TO, Barnett NP, Kahler CW, Monti PM. Stepped care for mandated college students: A pilot study. American Journal on Addictions. doi: 10.1080/10550490601184498. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell DT, Stanley J. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin; 1963. [Google Scholar]
- *.Carey KB, Carey MP, Maisto SA, Henson JM. Brief motivational interventions for heavy college drinkers: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74:943–954. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.5.943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Chiauzzi E, Green TC, Lord S, Thum C, Goldstein M. My student body: a high-risk drinking prevention web site for college students. Journal of American College Health. 2005;53:263–274. doi: 10.3200/JACH.53.6.263-274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis of the Behavioral Sciences. 2. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- *.Collins SE. Weighing the pros and cons: Evaluating decisional balance as a brief motivational intervention for at-risk college drinkers. (Doctoral dissertation, Syracuse University, 2003) Dissertation Abstracts International. 2003;64:5775. Intervention and statistical details for Collins & Carey, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- *.Collins SE, Carey KB. Lack of effect for decisional balance as a brief motivational intervention for at-risk college drinkers. Addictive Behaviors. 2005;30:1425–1430. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Collins SE, Carey KB, Sliwinski MJ. Mailed personalized normative feedback as a brief intervention for at-risk college drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;63:559–567. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2002.63.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Corbin WR, McNair LD, Carter JA. Evaluation of a treatment-appropriate cognitive intervention for challenging alcohol outcome expectancies. Addictive Behaviors. 2001;26:475–488. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(00)00138-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Cronin C. Harm reduction for alcohol-use-related problems among college students. Substance Use and Misuse. 1996;31:2029–2037. doi: 10.3109/10826089609066450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Curtin L, Stephens RS, Bonenberger JL. Goal setting and feedback in the reduction of heavy drinking in female college students. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy. 2001;15:17–37. [Google Scholar]
- *.Darkes J. Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: An evaluation of two expectancy challenge strategies. Dissertation Abstracts International. 1994;56:504. Intervention and statistical details for Darkes & Goldman, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- *.Darkes J, Goldman MS. Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: experimental evidence for a mediational process. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:344–353. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.2.344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Darkes J, Goldman MS. Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: process and structure in the alcohol expectancy network. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1998;6:64–76. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.6.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Greenbaum PE, Goldman MS. Up close and personal: temporal variability in the drinking of individual college students during their first year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:155–164. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Dimeff LA. Brief intervention for heavy and hazardous college drinkers in a student primary health care setting. Dissertation Abstracts International. 1997;58:6805. Intervention and statistical details for Dimeff & McNeely, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Dimeff LA, Baer J, Kivlahan D, Marlatt G. Brief Alcohol Screening and Intervention for College Students. The Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- *.Dimeff LA, McNeely M. Computer-enhanced primary care practitioner advice for high-risk college drinkers in a student primary health-care setting. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 2000;7:82–100. [Google Scholar]
- *.Donohue B, Allen DN, Maurer A, Ozols J, DeStefano G. A controlled evaluation of two prevention programs in reducing alcohol use among college students at low and high risk for alcohol related problems. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 2004;48:13–33. [Google Scholar]
- *.Fromme K, Corbin W. Prevention of heavy drinking and associated negative consequences among mandated and voluntary college students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:1038–1049. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.6.1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Garvin RB, Alcorn JD, Faulkner KK. Behavioral strategies for alcohol abuse prevention with high-risk college males. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 1990;36:23–34. [Google Scholar]
- *.Glindemann KE, Ehrhart IJ, Drake EA, Geller ES. Reducing excessive alcohol consumption at university fraternity parties: A cost-effective incentive/reward intervention. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Hansson H, Rundberg J, Zetterlind U, Johnsson KO, Berglund M. An intervention program for university students who have parents with alcohol problems: a randomized controlled trial. Alcohol & Alcoholism. 2006;41:655–663. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agl057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedges LV. Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics. 1981;6:107–128. [Google Scholar]
- Hedges LV, Olkin L. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez DV, Skews MC, Resor MR, Villanueva MR, Hanson BS, Blume AW. A pilot test of an alcohol skills training programme for Mexican-American college students. International Journal of Drug Policy. 2006;17:320–328. [Google Scholar]
- Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine. 2002;21:1539–1558. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analysis. British Medical Journal. 2003;327:557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hingson R, Heeren T, Winter M, Wechsler H. Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18-24: Changes from 1998 to 2001. Annual Review of Public Health. 2005;26:259–279. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman BM, Papas RK, Chatkoff DK, Kerns RD. Meta-analysis of psychological interventions for chronic low back pain. Health Psychology. 2007;26:1–9. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Howat P, Robinson S, Binns C, Palmer S, Landauer A. Educational biofeedback driving simulator as a drink-driving prevention strategy. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 1991;37:7–14. [Google Scholar]
- Huendo-Mendina TB, Sánchez-Meca J, Marín-Martínez F, Botella J. Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychological Methods. 2006;11:193–206. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hurlbut SC, Sher KJ. Assessing alcohol problems in college students. Journal of American College Health. 1992;41:49–58. doi: 10.1080/07448481.1992.10392818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Ingersoll KS, Ceperich SD, Nettleman MD, Karanda K, Brocksen S, Johnson BA. Reducing alcohol-exposed pregnancy risk in college women: initial outcomes of a clinical trial of a motivational intervention. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2005;29:173–180. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BT, Eagly AH. Quantitative synthesis of social psychological research. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, editors. Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2000. pp. 496–528. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BT, Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Carey MP. Health behavior change: What can we learn from meta-analyses of the intervention literature? 2007 Manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BT, Wood T. DSTAT 2.00: Software for Meta-Analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- *.Johnsson KO, Berglund M. Education of key personnel in student pubs leads to a decrease in alcohol consumption among the patrons: a randomized controlled trial. Addiction. 2003;98:627–633. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00383.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Johnsson KO, Berglund M. Comparison between a cognitive behavioural alcohol programme and post-mailed minimal intervention in high-risk drinking university freshmen: results from a randomized controlled trial. Alcohol & Alcoholism. 2006;41:174–180. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agh243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Jones LM, Silvia LY, Richman CL. Increased awareness and self-challenge of alcohol expectancies. Substance Abuse. 1995;16:77–85. [Google Scholar]
- *.Juarez P, Walters ST, Daugherty M, Radi C. A randomized trial of motivational interviewing and feedback with heavy drinking college students. Journal of Drug Education. 2006;36:233–246. doi: 10.2190/753N-8242-727T-G63L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazdin AE. Methodology, design, and evaluation in psychotherapy research. In: Bergin AE, Garfield SL, editors. Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change. 4. New York: Wiley; 1994. pp. 19–71. [Google Scholar]
- *.Keillor RM, Perkins WB, Horan JJ. Effects of videotaped expectancy challenges on alcohol consumption of adjudicated students. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly. 1999;13:179–187. [Google Scholar]
- *.Kivlahan DR, Marlatt GA, Fromme K, Coppel DB, Williams E. Secondary prevention with college drinkers: evaluation of an alcohol skills training program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1990;58:805–810. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.58.6.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Kulick AD. A new expectancy-challenge prevention program: The effects of drinking and alcohol expectancies in at-risk collegiate women. Dissertation Abstracts International. 2002;63:532. Intervention and statistical details for Musher-Eizenman & Kulick, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- *.Kypri K, McAnally HM. Randomized controlled trial of a web-based primary care intervention for multiple health risk behaviors. Preventative Medicine. 2005;41:761–766. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Kypri K, Saunders JB, Williams SM, McGee RO, Langley JD, Cashell-Smith ML, et al. Web-based screening and brief intervention for hazardous drinking: a double-blind randomized controlled trial. Addiction. 2004;99:1410–1417. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00847.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larimer ME, Cronce JM. Identification, prevention and treatment: a review of individual-focused strategies to reduce problematic alcohol consumption by college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;(Supplement):148–163. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Larimer ME, Turner AP, Anderson BK, Fader JS, Kilmer JR, Palmer RS, Cronce JM. Evaluating a brief alcohol intervention with fraternities. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2001;62:370–380. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2001.62.370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- *.Lysaught EM, Wodarski JS, Parris H. A comparison of an assessment/information-based group versus an assessment-only group: An investigation of drinking reduction with young adults. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment. 2003;8:23–43. [Google Scholar]
- *.Marlatt GA, Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Dimeff LA, Larimer ME, Quigley LA, Somers JM, Williams E. Screening and brief intervention for high-risk college student drinkers: results from a 2-year follow-up assessment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1998;66:604–615. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.66.4.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCambridge J, Strang J. The efficacy of single-session motivational interviewing in reducing drug consumption and perceptions of drug-related risk and harm among young people: results from a multi-site cluster randomized trial. Addiction. 2004;99:39–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00564.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.McNally AM, Palfai TP. Brief group alcohol interventions with college students: examining motivational components. Journal of Drug Education. 2003;33:159–176. doi: 10.2190/82CT-LRC5-AMTW-C090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.McNally AM, Palfai TP, Kahler CW. Motivational interventions for heavy drinking college students: examining the role of discrepancy-related psychological processes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19:79–87. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Michael KD, Curtin L, Kirkley DE, Jones DL, Harris R. Group-based motivational interviewing for alcohol use among college students: An exploratory study. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 2006;37:629–634. [Google Scholar]
- *.Moore MJ, Soderquist J, Werch C. Feasibility and efficacy of a binge drinking prevention intervention for college students delivered via the Internet versus postal mail. Journal of American College Health. 2005;54:38–44. doi: 10.3200/JACH.54.1.38-44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monti PM, Colby SM, Barnett NP, Spirito A, Rohsenow DJ, Myers M, Woolard R, Lewander W. Brief intervention for harm reduction with alcohol-positive older adolescents in a hospital emergency department. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1999;67:989–994. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.6.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Murgraff V, Abraham C, McDermott M. Reducing Friday alcohol consumption among moderate, women drinkers: Evaluation of a brief evidence-based intervention. Alcohol & Alcoholism. 2007;42:37–41. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agl083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Murgraff V, White D, Phillips K. Moderating binge drinking: it is possible to change behavior if you plan it in advance. Alcohol & Alcoholism. 1996;31:577–582. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.alcalc.a008193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Murphy JG, Benson TA, Vuchinich RE, Deskins MM, Eakin D, Flood AM, McDevitt-Murphy ME, Torrealday O. A comparison of personalized feedback for college student drinkers delivered with and without a motivational interview. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2004;65:200–203. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2004.65.200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Murphy JG, Duchnick JJ, Vuchinich RE, Davison JW, Karg RS, Olson AM, et al. Relative efficacy of a brief motivational intervention for college student drinkers. Psychology of Addictive Behavior. 2001;15:373–379. doi: 10.1037//0893-164x.15.4.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Murphy TJ, Pagano RR, Marlatt GA. Lifestyle modification with heavy alcohol drinkers: Effects of aerobic exercise and meditation. Addictive Behaviors. 1986;11:175–186. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(86)90043-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Musher-Eizenman DR, Kulick AD. An alcohol expectancy-challenge prevention program for at-risk college women. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2003;17:163–166. doi: 10.1037/0893-164x.17.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Center for Education Statistics. Number of U.S. colleges and universities and degrees awarded, 2003. 2005 Retrieved February 1, 2007, from http://www.infoplease.com/ipa/A0908742.html.
- National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Reducing underage drinking: A collective responsibility. Washington DC: The National Academies Press; 2004. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Neal DJ, Carey KB. Developing discrepancy within self-regulation theory: use of personalized normative feedback and personal strivings with heavy-drinking college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2004;29:281–297. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Lewis MA. Targeting misperceptions of descriptive drinking norms: efficacy of a computer-delivered personalized normative feedback intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:434–447. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Neighbors C, Lewis MA, Bergstrom RL, Larimer ME. Being controlled by normative influences: self-determination as a moderator of a normative feedback alcohol intervention. Health Psychology. 2006;25:571–579. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Neighbors C, Spieker CJ, Oster-Aaland L, Lewis MA, Bergstrom RL. Celebration intoxication: an evaluation of 21st birthday alcohol consumption. Journal of American College Health. 2005;54:76–80. doi: 10.3200/JACH.54.2.76-80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Peeler CM, Far JM, Miller JA, Brigham TA. An analysis of the effects of a program to reduce heavy drinking among college students. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 2000;45:39–54. [Google Scholar]
- *.Peeler CM. An analysis of the effects of a course designed to reduce the frequency of high-risk sexual behavior and heavy drinking. Dissertation Abstracts International. 2000;61:5546. Intervention details for Peeler et al., 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Prendergast ML, Urada D, Podus D. Meta-analysis of HIV risk-reduction interventions within drug abuse treatment programs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2001;69:389–405. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.3.389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reifman A, Watson WK, McCourt A. Social Networks and College Drinking: Probing Processes of Social Influence and Selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32:820–832. doi: 10.1177/0146167206286219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnicow K, Soler R, Braithwaite RL, Ahluwalia JS, Butler J. Cultural sensitivity in substance use prevention. Journal of Community Psychology. 2000;28(3):271–290. [Google Scholar]
- *.Roberts LJ, Neal DJ, Kivlahan DR, Baer JS, Marlatt GA. Individual drinking changes following a brief intervention among college students: Clinical significance in an indicated preventive context. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:500–505. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.3.500. Intervention and statistical information for Marlatt et al., 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Rohsenow DJ, Smith RE, Johnson S. Stress management training as a prevention program for heavy social drinkers: cognitions, affect, drinking, and individual differences. Addictive Behaviors. 1985;10:45–54. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(85)90052-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R. The “file-drawer” problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin. 1979;86:638–641. [Google Scholar]
- *.Saitz R, Palfai TP, Freedner N, Winter MR, Macdonald A, Lu J, Ozonoff A, Rosenbloom DJ, DeJong W. Screening and brief intervention online for college students: The iHealth study. Alcohol & Alcoholism. 2007;42:28–36. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agl092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sánchez-Meca J, Marín-Martínez F, Chacón-Moscoso S. Effect-size indices for dichotomized outcomes in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods. 2003;8:448–467. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Johnson BT. Eroticizing creates safer sex: A research synthesis. Journal of Primary Prevention. 2006;27:619–640. doi: 10.1007/s10935-006-0059-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Smith BH, Bogle KE, Talbott L, Gant R, Castillo H. A randomized study of four cards designed to prevent problems during college students’ 21st birthday celebrations. Journal on Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67:607–615. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Stahlbrandt H, Johnsson KO, Berglund M. Two-Year Outcome of Alcohol Interventions in Swedish University Halls of Residence: A Cluster Randomized Trial of a Brief Skills Training Program, Twelve-Step-Influenced Intervention, and Controls. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2007;31:458–466. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00327.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Stamper GA, Smith BH, Gant R, Bogle KE. Replicated findings of an evaluation of a brief intervention designed to prevent high-risk drinking among first-year college students: Implications for social norming theory. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 2004;48:53–72. [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 9. College Station, TX: StataCorp; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2005 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings, DHHS Publication No. SMA 06-4194. Rockville, MD: Office of Applied Studies; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- *.Tevyaw TO, Borsari B, Colby SM. Peer enhancement of a brief motivational intervention with mandated college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.1.114. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2010. With Understanding and Improving Health and Objectives for Improving Health. 2. II. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. The Surgeon General’s Call to Action to Prevent and Reduce Underage Drinking. Washington, DC: Office of the Surgeon General; 2007. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Walters ST, Bennett ME, Miller JH. Reducing alcohol use in college students: a controlled trial of two brief interventions. Journal on Drug Education. 2000;30:361–372. doi: 10.2190/JHML-0JPD-YE7L-14CT. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walters ST, Miller E, Chiauzzi E. Wired for wellness: e-Interventions for addressing college drinking. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2005;29:139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walters ST, Neighbors C. Feedback interventions for college alcohol misuse: what, why and for whom? Addictive Behaviors. 2005;30:1168–1182. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Walters ST, Vader AM, Harris TR. A Controlled Trial of Web-Based Feedback for Heavy Drinking College Students. Prevention Science. doi: 10.1007/s11121-006-0059-9. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler H, Lee JE, Kuo M, Seibring M, Nelson TF, Lee H. Trends in college binge drinking during a period of increased prevention efforts. Findings from 4 Harvard School of Public Health College Alcohol Study surveys: 1993-2001. Journal of American College Health. 2002;50:203–217. doi: 10.1080/07448480209595713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler H, Seibring M, Liu IC, Ahl M. Colleges respond to student binge drinking: reducing student demand or limiting access. Journal of American College Health. 2004;52:159–168. doi: 10.3200/JACH.52.4.159-168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Werch CE, Pappas DM, Carlson JM, DiClemente CC, Chally PS, Sinder JA. Results of a social norm intervention to prevent binge drinking among first-year residential college students. Journal of American College Health. 2000;49:85–92. doi: 10.1080/07448480009596288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White HR, Labouvie EW. Toward the assessment of adolescent problem drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1989;50:30–37. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1989.50.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.White HR, Morgan TJ, Pugh LA, Celinska K, LaBouvie EW, Pandina RJ. Evaluating Two Brief Substance-Use Interventions for Mandated College Students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67:309–317. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.White HR, Mun EY, Pugh L, Morgan TJ. Long-term effects of brief substance used interventions for mandated college students: Sleeper effect of an in-person personal feedback intervention. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00435.x. in press [Additional participants and measurement occasion for White et al., 2006] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]