SUMMARY
For women with breast cancer, rates of depression are the third highest of any cancer diagnostic group. Stress, defined as life events or perceptions of stress, is associated with depressive symptoms. However, little is known about the relationships between different types of stress and these symptoms in women with breast cancer. This relationship was tested in 210 women assessed after initial surgical treatment for regional breast cancer. Using Hierarchical Multiple Regression, three types of stress were examined: the occurrence of five stressful life events in the year prior to cancer diagnosis, perceptions of global stress, and perceptions of cancer-related traumatic stress. Other potentially relevant correlates of depressive symptoms were also examined, including the personality trait neuroticism, sociodemographics, and disease/treatment characteristics. Fifty-three percent of the variance in depressive symptoms was accounted for by three stress variables (perceptions of global and cancer-related traumatic stress and the life event-major financial difficulty) and two control variables (neuroticism and racial group). Specifically, global stress perceptions coupled with cancer-related intrusive thoughts and financial concerns along with the tendency towards negativity (neuroticism) may conspire to heighten a women’s risk for depressive symptoms. Assessing multiple sources of stress would improve our ability to identify women ‘at risk’ for depressive symptoms and provide appropriate intervention.
INTRODUCTION
Cancer diagnosis is described as one of the most stressful medical diagnoses a person can receive (Weisman and Worden, 1976). Indeed, data from many studies document acute emotional stress/distress at the time of diagnosis and early treatment (Andersen et al., 1989; Epping-Jordan et al., 1999; Maunsell et al., 1992). Depressive symptoms are the most common affective symptoms reported by cancer patients (van’t Spiker et al., 1997). Research suggests that the majority of patients experience some degree of depressive symptoms and approximately 50% meet criteria for a psychiatric diagnosis (i.e. adjustment, mood and/or anxiety disorders; Derogatis et al., 1983). In a recent review on the prevalence of depression in cancer patients, McDaniel and colleagues (1997) found rates ranging from 1.5 to 50% (with a mean of 24% across studies), rates consistently higher than those for patients with other medical diagnoses.
For those with breast cancer, the rates for depression symptoms are the third highest of any cancer diagnostic group (only patients with pancreatic or head and neck cancers have been identified with higher rates; McDaniel et al., 1997). Within six months of initial breast cancer diagnosis and treatment, rates of diagnosable depression have been estimated to be 20–30% (e.g. Fallowfield et al., 1990; Goldberg et al., 1992), with individual studies ranging from 6% (Watson et al., 1991) to 29% (Rijken et al., 1995). As breast cancer is the most commonly diagnosed cancer in women (i.e. over 192,000 new cases annually in the United States; American Cancer Society, 2001), upwards of 96,000 (50%) women may experience some depressive symptoms. A subset, 38,400–57,600 (20–30%), may have symptoms of a magnitude sufficient to suggest clinical depression. Such numbers clearly illustrate the need for appropriate identification of women ‘at risk’ for depressive symptoms as these symptoms are not only associated with lower quality of life (Ganz and Lerman, 1992) but may lead to other difficulties such as poorer treatment compliance (Ayers et al., 1994; Dimatteo et al., 2000) and shorter disease free intervals (Watson et al., 1999).
Stress has been conceptualized as life events/stressors or as, perceptions of stress (i.e. global perceptions or cancer-related perceptions). Regardless of the definition, stress is associated with poor psychological outcomes in cancer patients. For instance, increased recent life events are positively related to distress in cancer populations (Bukberg et al., 1984; Grassi et al., 1997). While well studied in non-cancer medical populations (Hewitt et al., 1992; Martin et al., 1995; O’Leary et al., 1988; Pbert et al., 1992), little is known about the association of global stress perceptions (i.e. the degree to which individuals perceive their lives, in general, as uncontrollable/unmanageable) to psychological outcomes in cancer populations. One exception (Varni et al., 1992) found that higher perceived global stress is associated with increased depression and anxiety in adolescent survivors of pediatric cancer. Much more is known about cancer-related stress perceptions. The Impact of Events Scale (Horowitz et al., 1979), a measure of traumatic stress, is commonly used to assess the severity of cancer-related intrusive and avoidant thoughts/behaviors. Research consistently reveals that increased perceptions of cancer-related traumatic stress are correlated with psychological distress (Butler et al., 1999). Intrusive symptoms (e.g. recurrent distressing thoughts, flashbacks), not avoidant ones (e.g. feelings of detachment, restricted range of affect), account for this significant relationship (Baider and De-Nour, 1997; Cordova et al., 1995). However, we are unaware of any cancer studies examining the relative contributions of these types of stress to depressive symptoms.
In the present study, depressive symptoms were examined in a sample of women recently diagnosed and surgically treated for regional breast cancer. Importantly, the relationship of three types of stress to these symptoms was examined—the occurrence of stressful life events in the year prior to cancer diagnosis, perceptions of global stress, and perceptions of cancer-related traumatic stress. Other potentially relevant correlates of depressive symptoms were also examined. An individual difference variable, the personality trait of neuroticism was tested. Positive associations between neuroticism, the tendency to be emotionally negative, and psychological outcomes have been found in heterogeneous cancer groups (Jenkins et al., 1991; VanderZee et al., 1996). However, the importance of a neurotic style to adjustment after surgery for breast cancer patients is unknown. Other control variables examined were those related to the general context (i.e. sociodemographics) as well as the specific context (i.e. disease and treatment characteristics) of the breast cancer experience. We used a conservative strategy (i.e. controlling for other variables) in order to examine the contribution of different types of stress to depressive symptoms above and beyond other variables.
METHOD
Participants
Two hundred and ten women in a prospective, longitudinal study of women with breast cancer participated. Eligibility criteria for the study included: a confirmed diagnosis of regional (stage II or III) breast cancer, no previous cancer diagnoses, age between 20 and 85 years, no refusal of cancer treatment prior to accrual, and no adjuvant treatment (e.g. chemotherapy, radiation) prior to accrual. Women with mental retardation, severe or untreated psychopathology (e.g. schizophrenia, bipolar disorder), a current neurological disorder, dementia, and chronic fatigue syndrome or other immunologic conditions/diseases (e.g. rheumatoid arthritis) were excluded.
Consecutive breast cancer cases were screened from a university-affiliated, National Cancer Institute-designated Comprehensive Cancer Center (CCC). Once identified, the patient was approached in-person by project personnel (B.A. or M.A. level research assistants) at a postoperative appointment. To enhance the generalizability of the sample, the study was announced in the local community (e.g. press releases, newspaper advertisements, flyers in physician offices and libraries). Women self-referring were screened by telephone and also recruited in person. Eighty-two percent of the sample was accrued from the CCC and 18% from the community. Across recruitment strategies, the accrual rate was 52%, higher than similar longitudinal studies (Cunningham et al., 1998; Goodwin et al., 2001; Ilnyckyj et al., 1994). The common reasons for refusal were ‘too far to drive’ (i.e. ≥ 60 miles; 25%), ‘do not have time’ (20%), ‘not interested’ (17%) and ‘other’ (17%). ‘Other’ responses were scattered across items such as ‘too much stress’ (9%) and ‘no stress’ (2%). There were no significant differences (all p’s>0.05) between study participants versus non-participants in demographics (age, race, partner status), disease and prognostic characteristics (menopausal status, estrogen/progesterone receptor status, stage of disease, and number of positive lymph nodes), or treatment variables (extent of surgery, receipt of radiotherapy, type of adjuvant chemotherapy recommended). There were also no significant differences (all p’s>0.05) between recruitment strategies (CCC vs self-referral) in the same variables (sociodemographics, disease and prognostic characteristics, or treatment variables).
Procedures
All women were provided with oral and written informed consent, indicating their awareness of the investigational nature of the study, in keeping with the institutional guidelines and in accordance with an assurance filed with and approved by the US Department of Health and Human Services. Following informed consent, participants were scheduled for an assessment prior to beginning adjuvant treatment (i.e. chemotherapy, radiation). Assessments were conducted in-person by project personnel at the university’s General Clinical Research Center or breast cancer clinic. Data included psychological, behavioral, and medical/treatment information from interviews, questionnaires, summary of medical records, and when necessary, physician consultation. Patients were paid $25.00 for their participation.
MEASURES
Depressive symptoms
The short form (Kohout et al., 1993) of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) is a standardized self-report questionnaire used to identify current symptoms of depression, with emphasis on depressed affect. The CES-D, consisting of 11 items (e.g. ‘I felt everything I did was an effort’ and ‘I felt sad’), is rated for the previous week on a three-point Likert scale (0=‘hardly ever or never’ to 2=‘much or most of the time’). Higher scores (range 0–22) reflect greater depressive symptoms. The CES-D has been established as a valid and reliable measure of depressive symptoms in women with breast cancer (Hann et al., 1999). Coefficient alpha reliability was 0.77.
Stress
Life events
A life event scale from a nationwide epidemiology study and clinical trial (Women’s Health Initiative) was used (Matthews et al., 1997). The events assessed were: (1) death or serious illness of a spouse/partner, family member, or close friend, (2) major financial difficulty, (3) divorce or other breakup involving spouse/partner, family members, or close friends, (4) major conflict with children or grandchildren, and (5) muggings, robberies, accidents, or similar events. These are five of the most stressful events included in similar, but lengthy, event lists (e.g. Social Readjustment Rating Questionnaire; Holmes and Rahe, 1967) or interviews (e.g. Structured Event Probe and Narrative Rating Method for Measuring Stressful Life Events-SEPRATE; Dohrenwend et al., 1984). To lessen participant burden and assessment time, the latter strategies were not employed. Participants were asked about the presence (vs absence) of each event during the year prior to cancer diagnosis.
Global stress
The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen et al., 1983) is a standardized self-report questionnaire used to assess a person’s perception of her/his life, in general, as unpredictable, uncontrollable, or overloading. The ten-item version was used for its improved internal reliability and factor structure (Cohen and Williamson, 1988). Examples of the questions include: ‘How often have you felt nervous or stressed?’ and ‘How often have you felt confident about your ability to handle your personal problems?’ The items are rated for the past month on a five-point Likert scale (0=‘never’ to 4=‘very often’). Higher scores (range 0–40) indicate greater overall stress. An item differential analysis conducted with over 2000 subjects found the items to be invariant with respect to race, sex, and education (Cole, 1999). Coefficient alpha reliability was 0.87.
Cancer-related traumatic stress
The Impact of Events Scale (IES; Horowitz et al., 1979) is a 15-item standardized self-report measure used to examine cognitions involving the re-experiencing (Intrusion subscale—seven items) and denial of thoughts and avoidant behaviors (Avoidance subscale—eight items) related to traumatic stress. Similar to previous studies of cancer stress (Baider and De-Nour, 1997; Butler et al., 1999; Cordova et al., 1995) items were slightly reworded to ensure the respondent’s focus upon cancer-related thoughts and behaviors (Intrusion item, ‘I had trouble falling or staying asleep because pictures or thoughts about cancer or having cancer treatment came into my mind’ and avoidance item, ‘I felt as if my cancer diagnosis/treatments hadn’t happened or they weren’t real).’ Items are rated for the previous week with a four-point Likert scale (0=‘not at all’, 1=‘rarely’, 3=‘sometimes’, and 5=‘often’). Higher scores (total range 0–75) indicate increased levels of cancer-related stress. A psychometric review of the IES literature found the measure has satisfactory reliability and validity properties as a measure of intrusive and avoidant processes (Joseph, 2000). Coefficient alpha reliability ranged from 0.78 to 0.84 for the total scale and two subscales.
Control variables
Neuroticism
Neuroticism was assessed using Goldberg’s Big-Five Factor Measure (Goldberg, 1992). As suggested by Goldberg, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to confirm the assignment of items to the neuroticism scale. The neuroticism factor consisted of 15 trait adjectives, 9 scored in the positive direction (e.g. irritable, nervous) and 6 scored in the negative direction (e.g. even-tempered, at-ease). Each woman rated the extent to which these trait adjectives described her, as compared to others of the same sex and age, on a ten-point Likert scale (from 0=‘extremely accurate’ to 9=‘extremely inaccurate’). Higher scores (range −63 to 81) indicate stronger trait neuroticism. Coefficient alpha reliability was 0.90.
Sociodemographics
The sociodemographic variables included age (years), racial group (Caucasian vs Minority), partner status (yes vs no), education (years), and annual household income (dollars per year).
Disease/treatment characteristics
Stage of disease (stage II vs stage III), extent of surgery (lumpectomy vs. mastectomy), and time since surgery (in days) were examined. Disease staging was based on the American Joint Committee on Cancer and the International Union Against Cancer staging systems. Days since surgery was calculated as the number of days between surgery and the initial assessment.
RESULTS
Descriptive analyses
Participant characteristics
Participants were 210 women diagnosed with Stage II (89%) or III (11%) breast cancer. All had been surgically treated (lumpectomy=41%; mastectomy=59%) within the preceding three months (days since surgery: M = 36.20; S.D.=16.89) and were awaiting the start of adjuvant therapy. Sociodemographic description of the sample was as follows: age (M=51 years, S.D.=10.83), racial group (Caucasian=90%; African American=9%; Hispanic=1%), and partner status (72% with a partner). The distribution of the total years of education was <12 years=4%; 12 years=25%; 13–15 years=28%; 16 years=18%; and >16 years=25%. Distribution of annual household income was <$ 15,000=9%; $15–29,000=16%; $30–49,000=23%; $50–79,000=24%; and ≥ $80,000=28%. Eleven women declined to give information about income. The average neuroticism score was 49 (S.D.=17.66; range −36 to 68).
Depressive symptoms
CES-D scores ranged from 0 to 18 with a sample mean of 5.96 (S.D.=3.67). Based on previous psychometric studies of the CES-D, a cut-off score of ≥ 10 is considered suggestive of clinical depression (Andresen et al., 1994). In the current study, a score of 10 was 1 S.D. above the sample mean. Eighteen percent (n = 38) of the participants had CES-D scores meeting/exceeding the cut-off.
Stress
Life events, global stress, and cancer-related traumatic stress
The majority (74%) of participants experienced at least one major life event in the year prior to their breast cancer diagnosis (0 events=26%; 1 event=41%; 2 events=18%; 3 events=10%; 4 events=4%; and 5 events=1%). The most common event reported was the death or serious illness of a spouse/partner, family member, or close friend (50%), followed by major financial difficulty (24%), divorce or other breakup involving spouse/partner, family members, or close friends (20%), major conflict with children/grandchildren (18%), and muggings, robberies, accidents or similar events (12%). The mean PSS score was 18.32 (S.D.=6.96, range=1–36). The mean IES score was 25.23 (S.D.=14.06, range=0–65). Avoidance and Intrusion subscale means were 12.41 (S.D.=7.78, range 0–36) and 12.81 (S.D.=8.29, range 0–35), respectively.
Regression analyses
Hierarchical multiple regression (HMR) analyses along with squared semi-partials were used to examine how the stress measures and control variables were related to depressive symptomatology. First, the nine control variables (i.e. neuroticism, age, racial group, partner status, education, annual household income, stage of disease, extent of surgery, and days since surgery) were tested for their correlation with depressive symptoms. Only two were significantly correlated (p<0.05), neuroticism (r = 0.44, p<0.0001) and racial group (r = 0.17, p<0.02). Next, we examined the correlations among the life events, global stress, cancer-related traumatic stress measures and depressive symptoms. Only the life event ‘major financial difficulty’ was significantly correlated (r = 0.25, p<0.05) with CES-D scores. As expected, IES total and subscale scores as well as PSS scores were significantly (p<0.05) correlated with CES-D scores. As the shared variance between the PSS and CES-D was noteworthy (r = 0.65, 42%), it was important to rule out the possibility of measure overlap at the item level. Results of a factor analysis verified that items loaded highest onto their own measures (See Note 1). Thus, the results in the present study are not confounded by shared item/content variance among measures.
Finally, variables significantly correlated with depressive symptoms were tested in the regression analyses. Variable entry was determined by a priori theoretical and empirical rationale. Control variables were entered first. Regarding the stress variables, prior research has found perception-based stress measures are stronger predictors of psychological and physical outcomes than life event measures (Cohen et al., 1983; Martin et al., 1995; O’Leary et al., 1988). Therefore, life event/s was entered first. Next, cancer-related traumatic stress was entered and finally, global stress. Entering global stress last in the regression analysis allowed for a more rigorous test of its correlation with depressive symptoms. In sum, variable entry was as follows: (1) the control variables (i.e. neuroticism and racial group); (2) life events (i.e. major financial difficulty), (3) cancer-related traumatic stress (IES), and (4) global stress (PSS). As a follow-up to any significant results, we calculated squared semi-partial correlations, sr2, for each variable in the equation. These correlations indicate the amount of variance accounted by each variable above and beyond all other variables in the regression model (i.e. the amount of variance accounted for by the variable as if it had been entered last in the regression equation; Cohen and Cohen, 1983).
Significant regression results for the final model are shown in Table 1; F(5, 204)=45.84, p<0.0001. Fifty-three percent of the variance (total adjusted R2 = 0.518) in depressive symptoms was accounted for by the tested variables. Specifically, the control factors accounted for 20% of the variance, whereas the addition of the life events and perceived stress measures accounted for the larger portion of the total, 33%. Life events, cancer-related traumatic stress, and global stress, accounted for the following increments in total variance: 4, 18, and 10%, respectively. Considering the sr2 data, global stress (11%) accounted for the most unique variance, followed by cancer-related traumatic stress (5%), and then major financial difficulty (2%). The control variables, neuroticism and racial group, each accounted uniquely for approximately 1% of the variance in the regression model.
Table 1.
Results of final HMR model examining depressive symptoms
Step | TR2 | Beta | t | sr2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
1. Racial group | 0.202 | 0.100 | 2.06* | 0.010 |
Neuroticism | 0.114 | 2.06* | 0.010 | |
2. Financial difficulty | 0.240 | 0.137 | 2.82** | 0.018 |
3. Cancer-related stress | 0.423 | 0.280 | 4.75*** | 0.052 |
4. Global stress | 0.529 | 0.415 | 6.78*** | 0.106 |
N=209; TR2=squared multiple correlation for total equation; sr2=squared semi-partial correlation.
p<0.05.
p<0.01.
p<0.0001.
Post-hoc analyses
In order to test the relative contribution of intrusive versus avoidant thoughts/behaviors to depressive symptoms in the present sample as well as to conduct a replication of previous research, a second regression was conducted using the IES subscales, IES-A and IES-I. As avoidance has a suggested weaker relationship with psychological outcomes (Baider and De-Nour, 1997; Cordova et al., 1995), IES-A was entered prior to IES-I. Therefore, variable entry was the following: Step (1) neuroticism and racial group, (2) major financial difficulty, (3) IES-A, (4) IES-I, and (5) PSS. Results indicate that 54% of the variance (total adjusted R2 = 0.527) in depressive symptoms was accounted for by the HMR, F(6, 203)=39.83, p<0.0001. Specifically, avoidance did not contribute a significant increment of unique variance to the final model (beta=0.045; t<1.00; sr2 = 0.005) whereas intrusion did (beta=0.285; t = 4.51; sr2 = 0.05). As before, all of the remaining variables were significantly associated (p<0.05) with depressive symptoms in the final regression model. The amount of unique variance (sr2) accounted for was as follows: PSS=9%; IES-I=5%; life events (major financial difficulty)=2%; and for both control variables (neuroticism and racial group)=2%.
DISCUSSION
Research has suggested that the experience of depressive symptoms is a common one for many cancer patients (Derogatis et al., 1983; McDaniel et al., 1997). One fifth (18%) of the women in the present sample, all assessed post-surgery and prior to beginning adjuvant treatment, reported experiencing depressive symptoms of possible clinical significance, consistent with previous studies of depression in women with breast cancer (Fallowfield et al., 1990; Goldberg et al., 1992; Rijken et al., 1995). Interestingly, stress was found to have multiple significant relationships with depressive symptoms. These important correlates include: (1) perceptions of global stress, (2) perceptions of cancer-related traumatic stress, as manifest by intrusive thoughts, and (3) stressful life events, specifically major financial difficulty in the year prior to diagnosis, along with (4) the control variables, neuroticism and racial group (i.e. minority status).
Regarding life events, while half of the participants experienced the death or serious illness of a close friend or relative in the previous 12 months (a fact found to correlate with breast cancer development; McKenna et al., 1999), it was ‘major financial difficulty’ that was a significant correlate of depressive symptoms in the HMR model (p<0.01). We do not know if this life event represents a chronic or recent stressor as the time frame for these items was ‘the last 12 months.’ Research has indicated that the cancer experience can have detrimental consequences on finances (e.g. loss of income due to work absences, increased insurance costs; Hewitt et al., 1999). For our participants, however, these cancer specific effects may not have accumulated as they were in the earliest stages of their diagnosis/treatment (i.e. 36 mean days since surgery, S.D.=17). Nevertheless, considering this finding in the context of other important information, 28% of all US households are headed by unmarried women and 57% of all women work outside the home (US Bureau of the Census, 1995, 1996), it may be that the stressor of ‘major financial difficulty’ is a common one for a subset of the thousands of women diagnosed with breast cancer each year.
A testimony to the impact of the cancer experience (i.e. cancer-related traumatic stress) is from the study participants themselves, as many women related during the assessment that these were ‘the worst days of my life.’ Such a characterization is consistent with the IES follow-up analysis indicating intrusive thoughts rather than avoidance were a stronger correlate of depressive symptoms. This finding is consistent with previous literature (Baider and De-Nour, 1997; Cordova et al., 1995) showing stronger relationships between intrusive thoughts and psychological distress in cancer patients. Global stress was also related to depressive symptoms. This finding suggests that women were not only stressed by their cancer experience but more generally as well. This situation is understandable as they were dealing with their recent breast cancer diagnosis, surgery, and beginning of adjuvant treatment in the continuing context of their daily lives (family and work responsibilities). Additionally, the mean values on the PSS and IES were, at a minimum, one-half to one standard deviation higher than values from normative samples (e.g. PSS; Cohen and Williamson, 1988) and cancer samples assessed during and/or after treatment (e.g. IES; Cordova et al., 1995). Together, these data underscore the importance of assessing perceptions or appraisals of stress both cancer-related and global in addition to the assessment of life events, per se. Unfortunately, the contribution of global stress to cancer patient outcomes is rarely studied.
Of the control variables (personality, sociodemographics, and disease/treatment characteristics) examined, only neuroticism and racial group were significant. The current data are consistent with reports linking neuroticism and psychological outcomes among cancer patients (Jenkins et al., 1991; VanderZee et al., 1996). Furthermore, these data suggest that the trait of neuroticism may heighten risk for depressive symptoms in women with breast cancer as has been suggested in non-cancer populations (Clark et al., 1994; Watson and Pennebaker, 1989). Of note are other analyses we conducted to test for moderating effects of neuroticism on stress. With three additional regressions, we added an interaction term (i.e. neuroticism × major financial difficulty; neuroticism × cancer-related traumatic stress; and neuroticism × global stress), but none were significant, suggesting the effect of neuroticism on depressive symptoms may be a direct one. A relationship between minority status and depressive symptoms was also found. As the number of minority participants in the present study was small (n=22, 10% of the total sample), this finding will require replication with a more racially balanced sample before a general conclusion can be made.
Strengths and limitations
The present study has a number of strengths: a design controlling the timing of the assessment to post diagnosis/surgery yet prior to beginning adjuvant treatment (i.e. an optimal time in the cancer experience for assessing risk), a large homogeneous sample in terms of disease diagnosis and severity, and the use of reliable/valid measures. The examination of three types of stress simultaneously is unique in the cancer literature and provided new findings. While the life events measure was brief, focusing only on the more important life stressors, it was successful in distinguishing a role of some events (i.e. financial difficulties), even in relationship to more common ones (i.e. death of a spouse/partner, family member, or close friend), as potentially more relevant to depressive symptoms at the time of initial diagnosis and surgery. Additionally, the role of three important categories of variables (personality, sociodemographics, and disease/treatment characteristics) were examined and controlled for in the statistical analyses.
This study used a self-report measure of depressive symptoms, whereas a diagnostic interview would provide information of how severe symptoms were to yield a psychiatric diagnosis. With concurrent assessments of stress and depressive symptoms, causal inferences cannot be made. Nevertheless, important information regarding the contribution of stress (operationalized broadly as life events, cancer-related traumatic stress and global stress) as well as other potentially important variables (neuroticism), to depressive symptoms (a cancer quality of life issue), was revealed. Our follow-up studies as well as the work of other investigators will test reliability of these relationships, their importance in predicting post-diagnosis and treatment quality of life, and their generalizability with other cancer groups.
CONCLUDING REMARKS
An important look at the multiple relationships between stress and depressive symptoms at the time of initial cancer diagnosis and surgery in women with regional breast cancer was provided. The data indicate that global stress perceptions coupled with cancer-related traumatic stress and the life event, major financial difficulty, along with the tendency towards negativity (neuroticism) may conspire to heighten women’s risk for depressive symptoms. Assessing multiple sources of stress would improve our ability to identify women ‘at risk’ for depressive symptoms. Recently, Prieto and colleagues (2002) reported that it is not uncommon for psychological symptoms in cancer patients to wax and wane over time with initial adjustment disorders developing into depressive or anxiety disorders. Thus, the need to identify and treat ‘at risk’ women is imperative if quality of life outcomes are to be impacted. The literature indicates that psychosocial interventions targeted to these individuals are effective in reducing psychological distress (Sheard and Maguire, 1999) and that stress reduction is a component of successful interventions (Andersen, 2002).
Acknowledgments
This study was supported in part by grants from the American Cancer Society (PBR-89), the Longaberger Company-American Cancer Society Grant for Breast Cancer Research (PBR-89A), the US Army Medical Research Acquisition Activity Grants (DAMD17-94-J-4165; DAMD17-96-1-6294; and DAMD 17-97-1-7062), the National Institutes of Mental Health (1 RO1 MH51487), the National Cancer Institute (RO1 CA92704-06), the General Clinical Research Center (MO1-RR0034), and The Ohio State University Comprehensive Cancer Center Core Grant from the National Cancer Institute (P30 CA16058).
We gratefully thank the participants. We also thank the following individuals for their contributions: project coordinators Vicki DiLillo and Laura Peterson; research assistants, Susan Aarestad, Elizabeth Street Bromet, Nicole Chaput, Angie Collier Crespin, Larisa Demshuk, Melissa Douglas, Sarah Grimes, Melissa Petri, Katheryn Pingel, Beth Putz, Elizabeth Rayl, Jan Varga-Spangler, Jessica Walker, and Laura Wielonski for conducting assessments; surgical oncologists William Farrar, William Burak, William Carson, Julian Kim, Gregory LaValle, Deborah Martinez, Michael Walker, and Lisa Yee and medical oncologists Kelly Cawley, Chris Rhoades, Arthur Sagone, Victoria Seewalt, Charles Shapiro, Michael Stanek, and James Ungerleider for OSU accural; Georita Frierson and Dr Michael Browne for conducting the factor analytic studies; and Dr Tim Crespin for manuscript comments.
Footnotes
According to previous research, the short form of the CES-D has 4 factors (1-depressed affect; 2-postitive affect; 3-somatic complaints, and 4-interpersonal problems; Kohout et al., 1993), while the PSS has 2 identified factors (1-distress; 2-coping; Hewitt et al., 1992; Martin et al., 1995; Cohen and Williamson, 1988). With a sample size of 166, 21 total items, and an item to subject ratio of 1:8, we conducted a PACE factor analysis using the program CEFA (computer exploratory factor analysis; Browne et al., 1998). An oblique rotation to a partially specified target (Browne, 1972) produced a factor structure and item loadings to test for item overlap and construct redundancy. Loadings anticipated to be zero were minimized in the rotation process and values of the remaining loadings were left unspecified. A pattern suggested by current research (6 factors: 4 for CES-D and 2 for PSS) was tested and a rotation to a solution as close to the target as possible was carried out. As an additional check, we also conducted factor analyses for 4 and 5 (i.e. underfactoring) and 7 (i.e. overfactoring) factor solutions.
The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger, 1989; Browne and Cudeck, 1993), measuring goodness of fit, for the factor solutions were as follows: 4 factors=0.072, 5 factors=0.067, 6 factors=0.058, and 7 factors=0.060. RMSEA values for the 4 and 5 factor solutions were unsatisfactory (only scores ≤ 0.05–0.06 are judged acceptable; Browne et al., 1998). While the RMSEA values for the 6 and 7 factor solutions were both acceptable, the 7 factor solution showed evidence of overfactoring as indicated by the direct quartamin rotation in which there were two moderate loadings on the seventh factor and the other factor loadings were low, negative values (approximating zero). Therefore, the variance accounted for by the seventh factor was uninterpretable.
The six-factor solution, however, demonstrated high loadings that corresponded to the target and reflected previous findings. Additionally, the confidence intervals corresponding to the target zero loadings generally overlapped with zero and were not significant. These results were not only consistent with previous research (Cohen and Williamson, 1988; Hewitt et al., 1992; Kohout et al., 1993; Martin et al., 1995) but, in fact, indicated no overlap among the factors.
References
- American Cancer Society. Cancer Facts and Figures 2001. American Cancer Society, Inc; Atlanta, GA: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Andersen BL. Biobehavioral outcomes following psychological interventions for cancer patients. J Consult Clin Psych. 2002;70:590–610. doi: 10.1037//0022-006X.70.3.590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen BL, Anderson B, deProsse C. Controlled prospective longitudinal study of women with cancer: II. Psychological outcomes. J Consult Clin Psych. 1989;57:692–697. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.57.6.692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL. Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D. Am J Prev Med. 1994;10:77–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayers A, Hoon P, Franzoni J, et al. Influence of mood and adjustment to cancer on compliance with chemotherapy among breast cancer patients. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:393–402. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90100-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baider L, De-Nour AK. Psychological distress and intrusive thoughts in cancer patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1997;185:346–348. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199705000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browne MW. Oblique rotation to a partially specified target. Brit J Math Stat Psychiat. 1972;25:207–212. [Google Scholar]
- Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing Structural Equation Models. Sage; Newbury Park, CA: 1993. pp. 136–161. [Google Scholar]
- Browne MW, Cudeck R, Tateneni K, Mels G. CEFA: Comprehensive Exploratory Factor Analysis. 1998 WWW document and computer program. Available from URL http://quantrm2.psy.ohio-state.edu/browne/
- Bukberg J, Penman D, Holland JC. Depression in hospitalized cancer patients. Psychosom Med. 1984;46:199–212. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198405000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler LD, Koopman C, Classen C, Speigel D. Traumatic stress, life events, and emotional support in women with metastatic breast cancer: cancer-related traumatic stress symptoms associated with past and current stressors. Health Psychol. 1999;18:555–560. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.18.6.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark LA, Watson D, Minneka S. Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:103–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J, Cohen P. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation for the Behavioral Sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 24:385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, Williamson GM. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In: Spacapan S, Oskamp S, editors. The Social Psychology of Health. Sage; Newbury Park, CA: 1988. pp. 31–67. [Google Scholar]
- Cole S. Assessment of differential item functioning in the Perceived Stress Scale 10. J Epidemiol Commun H. 1999;53:319–320. doi: 10.1136/jech.53.5.319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordova MJ, Andrykowski MA, Kenady DE, et al. Frequency and correlates of posttraumatic-stress-disorder-like symptoms after treatment for breast cancer. J Consult Clin Psych. 1995;63:981–986. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.63.6.981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham AJ, Edmonds CVI, Jenkins GP, et al. A randomized controlled trial of the effects of group psychological therapy on survival in women with metastatic breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology. 1998;7:508–517. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1611(199811/12)7:6<508::AID-PON376>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derogatis LR, Morrow GR, Fetting J, et al. The prevalence of psychiatric disorders among cancer patients. J Amer Med Assoc. 1983;249:751–757. doi: 10.1001/jama.249.6.751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiMatteo MR, Lepper HS, Croghan TW. Depression is a risk factor for noncompliance with medical treatment: meta-analysis of the effects of anxiety and depression on patient adherence. Arch Int Med. 2000;160:2101–2107. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.14.2101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dohrenwend B, Raphael K, Schwartz S, Stueve A, Skodol A. The structured event probe and narrative rating method for measuring stressful life events-SEPRATE. In: Dohrenwend BS, Dohrenwend BP, editors. Stressful Life Events and Their Contexts. Rutgers University Press; New Brunswick, NJ: 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Epping-Jordan JE, Compas BE, Osowiecki DM, et al. Psychological adjustment in breast cancer: processes of emotional distress. Health Psychol. 1999;18:315–326. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.18.4.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fallowfield L, Hall A, Maguire P, Baum M. Psychological outcomes of different treatment policies in women with early breast cancer outside a clinical trial. Brit Med J. 1990;301:575–580. doi: 10.1136/bmj.301.6752.575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganz K, Lerman C. Psychosocial impact of breast cancer: a critical review. Ann Behav Med. 1992;14:204–212. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg LR. The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychol Assessment. 1992;4:26–42. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg J, Scott R, Davidson P, et al. Psychological morbidity in the first year after breast surgery. Eur J Surg Oncol. 1992;18:327–331. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodwin PJ, Leszcz M, Ennis M, et al. The effect of group psychosocial support on survival in metastatic breast cancer. N Engl J Med. 2001;345:1719–1726. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa011871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grassi L, Malacarne P, Maestri A, Ramelli E. Depression, psychosocial variables and occurrence of life events among patient with cancer. J Affect Disorders. 1997;44:21–30. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(97)01445-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hann D, Winter K, Jacobsen P. Measurement of depressive symptoms in cancer patients: evaluation of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) J Psychosom Res. 1999;46:437–443. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(99)00004-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hewitt M, Breen N, Devesa S. Cancer prevalence and survivorship issues: analyses of the 1992 National Health Interview Survey. J Natl Cancer. 1999;191:1480–1486. doi: 10.1093/jnci/91.17.1480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Mosher SW. The perceived stress scale: factor structure and relation to depression symptoms in a psychiatric sample. J Psychopathol Behav. 1992;14:247–257. [Google Scholar]
- Holmes TH, Rahe RH. The social readjustment rating scale. J Psychosom Res. 1967;11:213–218. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horowitz M, Wilner N, Alvarez W. Impact of event scale: measure of subjective stress. Psychosom Med. 1979;41:209–218. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197905000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ilnyckyj A, Farber N, Cheag J, Weinerman BH. A randomized controlled trial of psychotherapeutic intervention in cancer patients. Ann R Coll Physicians Surg Can. 1994;27:93–96. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins PL, May VE, Hughes LE. Psychological morbidity associated with local recurrence of breast cancer. Int J Psychiat Med. 1991;21:149–155. doi: 10.2190/UU5G-MDJ3-U55G-33GQ. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joseph S. Psychometric evaluation of Horowitz’s Impact of Event Scale: a review. J Trauma Stress. 2000;13:101–113. doi: 10.1023/A:1007777032063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohout FJ, Berkman LF, Evans DA, Cornoni-Huntley J. Two shorter forms of the CES-D depression symptoms index. J Aging Health. 1993;5:179–193. doi: 10.1177/089826439300500202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin RA, Kazarian SS, Breiter HJ. Perceived stress, life events, dsyfunctional attitudes, and depression in adolescent psychiatric patients. J Psychopathol Behav. 1995;17:81–95. [Google Scholar]
- Matthews KA, Shumaker SA, Bowen DJ, et al. Women’s health initiative: why now? What is it? What’s new? Am Psychol. 1997;52:101–116. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.2.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maunsell E, Brisson J, Deschenes L. Psychological distress after initial treatment of breast cancer. Assessment of potential risk factors. Cancer. 1992;70:120–125. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(19920701)70:1<120::aid-cncr2820700120>3.0.co;2-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDaniel JS, Musselman DL, Nemeroff CB. Cancer and depression: theory and treatment. Psychiat Ann. 1997;27:360–364. [Google Scholar]
- McKenna M, Zevon M, Corn B, Rounds J. Psychosocial factors and the development of breast cancer: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 1999;18:520–531. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.18.5.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Leary A, Shoor S, Lorig K, Holman HR. A cognitive-behavioral treatment for rheumatoid arthritis. Health Psychol. 1988;7:527–544. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.7.6.527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pbert L, Doerfler LA, DeCosimo D. An evaluation of the perceived stress scale in two clinical populations. J Psychopathol Behav. 1992;14:363–375. [Google Scholar]
- Prieto JM, Blanch J, Atala J, et al. Psychiatric morbidity and impact on hospital length of stay among hematologic cancer patients receiving stem-cell transplantation. J Clin Oncol. 2002;20:1907–1917. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2002.07.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a new self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychiat Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Rijken M, deKruif A, Komproe IH, Roussell J. Depressive symptomatology of post-menopausal breast cancer patients: a comparison of women recently treated by mastectomy or breast conserving therapy. Eur J Surg Oncol. 1995;21:498–503. doi: 10.1016/s0748-7983(95)96898-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz MD, Lerman C, Miller SM, Daley M, Masny A. Coping disposition, perceived risk, and psychological distress among women at increased risk for ovarian cancer. Health Psychol. 1995;14:232–235. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.14.3.232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheard T, Maguire P. The effect of psychological interventions on anxiety and depression in cancer patients: results of two meta-analyses. Brit J Cancer. 1999;80:1770–1780. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6690596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steiger JH. EzPATH: A Supplementary Module for STSTAT and SYGRAPH. SYSTAT Inc; Evanston, IL: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- US Bureau of the Census. Women in the United States: A Profile. Statistical Brief. Department of Commerce; Washington, DC, US: 1995. 1996. [Google Scholar]
- VanderZee KI, Buunk BP, Sanderman R. The relationship between social comparison processes and personality. Pers Indiv Differ. 1996;20:551–565. [Google Scholar]
- van’t Spiker A, Trijsburg RW, Duivenvoorden HJ. Psychological sequelae of cancer diagnosis: a meta-analytical review of 58 studies after 1980. Psychosom Med. 1997;59:280–293. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199705000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varni JW, Katz ER, Colegrove R, Dogin M. Perceived stress and adjustment of long-term survivors of childhood cancer. J Psychosoc Oncol. 1992;12:1–16. [Google Scholar]
- Watson M, Greer S, Rowden L, et al. Relationships between emotional control, adjustment to cancer and depression and anxiety in breast cancer patients. Psychol Med. 1991;21:51–57. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700014641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson M, Haviland JS, Greer S, et al. Influence of psychological response on survival in breast cancer: a population-based cohort study. Lancet. 1999;1354:1331–1336. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(98)11392-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson D, Pennebaker JW. Health complaints, stress, and distress: exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychol Rev. 1989;96:234–254. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.96.2.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisman AD, Worden JW. The existential plight in cancer: significance of the first 100 days. Int J Psychiat Med. 1976;7:1–15. doi: 10.2190/uq2g-ugv1-3ppc-6387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]