Skip to main content
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry logoLink to Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry
. 1998 Nov;65(5):685–692. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.65.5.685

The effect of semantic and emotional context on written recall for verbal language in high functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder

D Beversdorf 1, J Anderson 1, S Manning 1, S Anderson 1, R Nordgren 1, G Felopulos 1, S Nadeau 1, K Heilman 1, M Bauman 1
PMCID: PMC2170365  PMID: 9810938

Abstract

OBJECTIVE—Several deficits have been proposed to account for cognitive impairment in autism including an inability to comprehend the perspectives of others ("theory of mind"), an inability to process emotional information, and difficulty drawing together diverse information in context ("central coherence"). Because context (central coherence) and emotion can influence memory, a study was designed to show if autism spectrum disorder was associated with impaired utilisation of context and emotion in recall; and if impairments in theory of mind processing would influence recall in autism spectrum disorder.
METHODS—Ten high functioning subjects with autism spectrum disorder and 13 age and IQ matched controls were tested using recall tests. In the first coherence memory test, subjects listened to a series of word lists that were in varying degrees of syntactic and semantic (coherent) order and were asked to recall the words. In the second coherence memory test, subjects listened to stories consisting of sentences that were, or were not, in logical (coherent) order. In the emotional memory test, the subjects listened to sentences that were highly emotional or non-emotional. In the theory of mind test, the subjects listened to stories requiring varying levels of understanding of the perspectives of others.
RESULTS—There were no significant differences between groups in recall of coherent versus incoherent word lists, nor was there a significant difference between groups in recall of coherent versus incoherent stories. However, the control subjects recalled more of the emotional than non-emotional sentences, whereas the autism spectrum disorder group did not show such a difference. No significant difference existed in recall of stories requiring varying levels of understanding of the perspectives of others among subjects with autism spectrum disorder, and subjects with autism spectrum disorder did not differ from control subjects in the influence of theory of mind content on story recall.
CONCLUSION—The study shows that memory in high functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder is facilitated by emotional content to a lesser degree than it is facilitated by coherence. Therefore, impairments in emotional processing cannot be considered as simply an effect of the "weak central coherence" theory in autism spectrum disorder. Whereas the reasons for this emotional deficit are unknown, evidence of abnormalities of the limbic structures in autism spectrum disorder may provide an anatomical explanation.



Full Text

The Full Text of this article is available as a PDF (128.6 KB).

Selected References

These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.

  1. Baron-Cohen S. Do people with autism understand what causes emotion? Child Dev. 1991 Apr;62(2):385–395. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baron-Cohen S., Leslie A. M., Frith U. Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"? Cognition. 1985 Oct;21(1):37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baron-Cohen S. The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989 Mar;30(2):285–297. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Boller F., Cole M., Vrtunski P. B., Patterson M., Kim Y. Paralinguistic aspects of auditory comprehension in aphasia. Brain Lang. 1979 Mar;7(2):164–174. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(79)90014-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bowler D. M. "Theory of mind" in Asperger's syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992 Jul;33(5):877–893. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb01962.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Dahlgren S. O., Trillingsgaard A. Theory of mind in non-retarded children with autism and Asperger's syndrome. A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996 Sep;37(6):759–763. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01469.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Fotheringham J. B. Autism: its primary psychological and neurological deficit. Can J Psychiatry. 1991 Nov;36(9):686–692. doi: 10.1177/070674379103600913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Frith U., Happé F. Autism: beyond "theory of mind". Cognition. 1994 Apr-Jun;50(1-3):115–132. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90024-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Happé F. G. An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994 Apr;24(2):129–154. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Happé F. G. Annotation: current psychological theories of autism: the "theory of mind" account and rival theories. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994 Feb;35(2):215–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01159.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Happé F. G. Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: a test of relevance theory. Cognition. 1993 Aug;48(2):101–119. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90026-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Happé F. G. The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Dev. 1995 Jun;66(3):843–855. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hobson R. P. Methodological issues for experiments on autistic individuals' perception and understanding of emotion. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991 Nov;32(7):1135–1158. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00354.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. LeDoux J. E. Emotion: clues from the brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 1995;46:209–235. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Leslie A. M., Thaiss L. Domain specificity in conceptual development: neuropsychological evidence from autism. Cognition. 1992 Jun;43(3):225–251. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90013-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Lord C., Rutter M., Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994 Oct;24(5):659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. MILLER G. A., SELFRIDGE J. A. Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material. Am J Psychol. 1950 Apr;63(2):176–185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. O'Connor N., Hermelin B. Auditory and visual memory in autistic and normal children. J Ment Defic Res. 1967 Jun;11(2):126–131. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1967.tb00213.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ozonoff S., Pennington B. F., Rogers S. J. Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991 Nov;32(7):1081–1105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ozonoff S., Rogers S. J., Pennington B. F. Asperger's syndrome: evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991 Nov;32(7):1107–1122. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00352.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Perner J., Frith U., Leslie A. M., Leekam S. R. Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Dev. 1989 Jun;60(3):688–700. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Rumsey J. M. Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. J Autism Dev Disord. 1985 Mar;15(1):23–36. doi: 10.1007/BF01837896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Rumsey J. M., Hamburger S. D. Neuropsychological divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. J Autism Dev Disord. 1990 Jun;20(2):155–168. doi: 10.1007/BF02284715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Rumsey J. M., Hamburger S. D. Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1988 Mar;10(2):201–221. doi: 10.1080/01688638808408236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Shah A., Frith U. Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1993 Nov;34(8):1351–1364. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb02095.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Sparrevohn R., Howie P. M. Theory of mind in children with autistic disorder: evidence of developmental progression and the role of verbal ability. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995 Feb;36(2):249–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01823.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Wechsler A. F. The effect of organic brain disease on recall of emotionally charged versus neutral narrative texts. Neurology. 1973 Feb;23(2):130–135. doi: 10.1212/wnl.23.2.130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Zaitchik D. When representations conflict with reality: the preschooler's problem with false beliefs and "false" photographs. Cognition. 1990 Apr;35(1):41–68. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90036-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES