Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 1999 Apr 27;96(9):5066–5071. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.9.5066

Cycles and trends in cod populations

Ottar N Bjørnstad *,†,, Jean-Marc Fromentin §, Nils Chr Stenseth *,, Jakob Gjøsæter
PMCID: PMC21817  PMID: 10220419

Abstract

Year-to-year fluctuations in fish stocks are usually attributed to variability in recruitment, competition, predation, and changes in catchability. Trends in abundance, in contrast, are usually ascribed to human exploitation and large-scale environmental changes. In this study, we demonstrate, through statistical modeling of survey data (1921–1994) of cod from the Norwegian Skagerrak coast, that both short- and long-term variability may arise from the same set of age-structured interactions. Asymmetric competition and cannibalism between cohorts generate alternating years of high and low abundance. Intercohort interactions also resonate the recruitment variability so that long-term trends are induced. The coupling of age-structure and variable recruitment should, therefore, be considered when explaining both the short- and long-term fluctuations displayed by the coastal cod populations. Resonant effects may occur in many marine populations that exhibit this combination of traits.

Keywords: age-structured dynamics, Gadus morhua, Markov chain Monte Carlo estimation, nonlinear stochastic dynamics, stock recruitment


Most commercial fish stocks vary greatly in abundance and exhibit a combination of short-term (year-to-year) fluctuations and longer term trends. Different factors are usually thought to give rise to these two types of changes in population size. Trends are usually related to external forcing on the populations, especially from human exploitation (1, 2) and large-scale environmental variation, such as changes in atmospheric and oceanographic circulation (3) or temperature (4). The year-to-year variation, in contrast, is usually explained by two different classes of hypotheses, both of which are internal to the populations in the sense of affecting vital rates, either in a density-independent or density-dependent manner. First, variable (stochastic) recruitment caused by changes in winds, currents, temperature, or resources can generate substantial variation in juvenile survivorship (39). For instance, the relative timing of the spring algal bloom and egg hatching is a critical determinant of food availability to the larvae and postlarvae. Because the spring bloom is itself determined by environmental conditions, this “match–mismatch” hypothesis offers an explicit mechanism for the effect of climate on year class strength (6, 10, 11). Second, density-dependent interactions between and within cohorts (in the form of cannibalism and competition) can generate population fluctuations (1214). Such interactions can lead to cycles or quasiperiodic oscillations (1517).

Fromentin et al. (18, 19) described patterns of abundance in a long-term survey (1919–1994) of cod (Gadus morhua L.) from the Norwegian Skagerrak coast. They demonstrated both 2- to 3-year cycles and long-term trends in these time series. The cycles were hypothesized to arise from interactions within and among cohorts (18, 20). The long-term trends appeared as a regional feature but were not clearly linked to any large-scale changes in either zooplankton abundance or climatic variables (19). In the present study we built a dynamic model for the Skagerrak cod survey data to explore the cycles and trends. The model includes stochastic reproduction and age-structured interactions and is designed to test whether this set of regulatory and disruptive processes can account for the variability in abundance. We asked whether a stochastic age-structured model alone can account for the dual pattern of cycles and trends in cod abundance. The parameters of the model were estimated from the time-series data with a Bayesian Markov chain Monte Carlo method (21, 22). Markov chain Monte Carlo allows us to estimate the parameters in the nonlinear age-structured model, including the variance and the mean of the per capita reproduction rate.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The Data.

Recruitment has long been recognized as a key parameter in year class strength of fish (5, 6). To investigate the recruitment dynamics of local cod stocks, a research survey was initiated along the Norwegian Skagerrak coast in 1919. Standardized beach seines were used to target the two juvenile cohorts of cod: the 0-group and the 1-group (see ref. 18 for details). Since then, the program has been running according to the same sampling protocol at more than 250 locations along the Southern Skagerrak coast of Norway. In the Southern part of the study area (between Kristiansand and Porsgrunn), records have been made every year for the period 1921–1994 (except for a gap during World War II) at 38 locations (see ref. 18 for details). In the Northern part of the study area (between Porsgrunn and the Norwegian-Swedish border), 31 stations provide complete records for the period 1936–1994 (except for a gap during the World War II). The total counts of the 0-group and the 1-group are shown in Fig. 1A for the Southern area and Fig. 1B for the Northern area. The temporal variability in abundance is extensive at all locations. In our analyses, we will focus on the two sets of aggregate time series. The first set is the summed count of the 0-group and the summed count of the 1-group across the 38 Southern locations. The second set is the summed series across the 31 Northern locations. The median for the 0-group and the 1-group counts in the Southern set is 458 (range, 5–1,902; maximum to minimum ratio, 380) and 39 (range, 1–164; maximum to minimum ratio, 164), respectively. The corresponding medians for the Northern set are 227 (range, 13–2,325; maximum to minimum ratio, 179) and 35 (range, 1–301; maximum to minimum ratio, 301), respectively. The four pre-World War II observations for the Northern set represent such a short continuous period that they were not used in the estimation.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The total count of 0-group and 1-group cod in the Flødevigen survey from the Southern area (sum over 38 permanent stations from 1921–1994 disrupted during World War II; A) and the Northern area (sum over 31 permanent stations from 1936–1994; B) along the Norwegian Skagerrak coast. The 0-group counts are represented by the black lines, open circles, and left axes. The gray shaded areas and the right axes represent the 1-group counts. The 38 Southern stations are located in the following fjords/coastal areas (18): Torvefjord (2), Topdalfjord (3), Høvåg (8), Bufjord (2), Flødevigen (2), Sandnesfjord (7), Søndelefjord (6), Stølefjord (2), Kilsfjord (3) and Soppekilen (3). The 31 Northern stations are located as follows: Tjøme (6), Holmestrand (6), Oslofjorden (8), Drøbak (4), and Hvaler (7).

Descriptive analyses of the 38 time series from the Southern area have identified significant short-term cycles and long-term trends in most populations (18). The two aggregate time series considered here (Fig. 1) reflect this dual pattern. Significant 2- to 3-year cycles and significant long-term trends (here taken to mean variability in the lowest frequencies observable, corresponding to 25+ years) are seen in both the Southern (for the contiguous post-World War II period) and the Northern area. The evidence for cycles is stronger in the 1-group [permutation test of the periodogram (ref. 23, Chap. 11) by using 10,000 permutations: Southern, P = 0.03; Northern, P < 0.01] than in the 0-group data (Southern P = 0.08; Northern P < 0.01). In contrast, the trends are more apparent in the 0-group (Southern, P < 0.01; Northern, P = 0.02) than in the 1-group (Southern, P = 0.16; Northern, P = 0.36). The combined high- and low-frequency variability lead to bimodal (U-shaped) spectra.

Age-Structured Model: The Cod Life Cycle.

Adults and juveniles are parapatric and do not interact strongly (2426). The two juvenile age classes (the 0-group, X and the 1-group, Y), in contrast, have similar habitat preferences (18, 24). An essential component of the dynamics is believed to be competitive and cannibalistic interactions within and between individuals of these two classes. The reduction in survival caused by juvenile interactions is thought to be density dependent. The functional form for the density dependence in cod survival appears to be approximately log linear (20, 27). The number of individuals surviving from the 0-group to the 1-group (survival during the first year) may, therefore, be approximated as

graphic file with name M1.gif 1

where β (<0) represents density-dependent reduction in survival caused by intracohort competition and γ (<0) represents density dependence caused by intercohort competition and cannibalism (Fig. 2). The former, thus, embraces the interaction among 0-group individuals, and the latter embraces the interaction between 0-group individuals and 1-group individuals. The constant c (<0) represents the level of annual density-independent survival.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(A) The estimated recruitment function of 1-group individuals from the 0-group in the preceding year as estimated from the census data from the Southern area (Fig. 1A). The significant convexity is a signature of the density-dependent juvenile mortality because of intracohort interactions (see also ref. 20). The shaded area represents the region of density independence (absence of negative density dependence). The line represents the partial regression line from the model fitted to the Southern data set (Eq. 1, Table 1). The points represent the partial residuals (corrected for the intercohort density dependence; see B). (B) Density dependence in juvenile mortality because of intercohort interactions as estimated from the census data for the Southern area. There is a significant negative effect of the 1-group abundance in the preceding year (year t − 1) on the 1-group recruitment in the succeeding year (year t). The shaded area represents the area of density independence (absence of negative density dependence). The line represents the partial regression line from the fitted model (Eq. 1, Table 1), and the points represent the partial residuals (corrected for the intracohort dependence; see A). The dotted lines represent the model fitted when four extreme observations (indicated by circles with crosses) of the 1-group count have been omitted (estimates ± 2 posterior SD: ĉ = −0.47 ± 0.33; β̂ = −0.22 ± 0.05; γ̂ = −0.18 ± 0.04).

Individuals mature at 2–3 years, and the annual survival rate, λ, of adults appears to be independent of density and relatively constant across years (25). The average annual adult survival was estimated to be 0.34 ±0.02 (range, 0.44–0.32) in a mark–recapture study involving 40,000 marked individuals during 1986–1989 (28). Fishing mortality is known to have been higher during the late 1980s (28); an adult survival of approximately 0.4 is therefore likely to be representative for these populations. A constant adult survival of 0.4 implies that the group 2, Z, and group 3, W, together make up more than 80% of the spawning stock. In agreement with this, Gjøsæter et al. (25) found these two age groups to make up approximately 75% of the spawning stock of local populations. Therefore, the two dominant adult classes are given by

graphic file with name M2.gif 2

where λ is set to 0.4, and the reproductive stock in year t may be approximated as Zt + Wt.

Spawning is usually in March in the Norwegian Skagerrak cod, and the eggs hatch into larvae after a few weeks. The larvae metamorphose into young fish (the 0-group individuals) in May/June. After a pelagic phase, the 0-group individuals settle and feed on the bottom when they are about 3–5 cm long (around August). Thus, the numbers of 0-group individuals counted in the fall arise from reproduction by the adult stock in the spring of the same year. Approximating the stock by the 2- and 3-year olds, the number of the 0-group recruits, X, will be

graphic file with name M3.gif 3

where eαt is the reproduction rate. We define the reproductive process as the transition from eggs through fish larvae to pelagic and finally demersal 0-group individuals (thus including the settlement on the bottom). This extended reproductive process is highly variable between years. To allow for variability between years in reproduction, we assume that αt is drawn from a Gaussian distribution with mean ᾱ and variance σα2. This leads to a per capita reproductive rate, eαt, that is log-normally distributed across years (27, 29, 30). To understand the dynamic effects and importance of reproductive variability, we also consider the contrasting situation: αt is constant across years (denoted αconst), and estimate the parameters under this assumption.

Under the heuristic assumption of constant recruitment, equations 1–3 describe a deterministic nonlinear dynamic system that produces cyclic or dampened cyclic dynamics, depending on the absolute and relative strength of inter- and intracohort interactions. Limit cycles will arise if the intercohort interaction is substantially larger than intracohort interaction. For adult survival of 0.4, limit cycles arise if the intercohort interaction γ < −0.6 + 0.4β (unpublished numerical results). With deterministic reproduction, all variability in the data is because of measurement errors inherent in the counting of individuals (approximated by Poisson variability) and because of the intrinsic dynamic (deterministic) variability (31). Statistically speaking, the census counts arise from a nonlinear and non-Gaussian process with fixed coefficients (32): αconst, β, γ, and c.

When the reproductive rate is affected by changing environmental conditions, there is an additional level of variability: the process-inherent stochasticity (see, for example, refs. 30 and 33). Under the stochastic recruitment model, the census counts arise from a stochastic nonlinear and non-Gaussian process with one random coefficient (32), αt (which we assume to be approximately normally distributed with mean ᾱ and variance σα2).

For a known λ, the parameters αconst, ᾱ, σα2, β, γ, and c will be fully specified by the census data for 0-group and 1-group (Fig. 1). The parameters can be estimated by using the Markov chain Monte Carlo method implemented in bugs, version 0.6 (21, 22, 34). We base our estimates (and 95% credible intervals (ci)—the Bayesian alternative to the confidence interval) on 100,000 iterations of the Gibbs sampler after a burn-in of 10,000 iterations.

RESULTS

The parameter estimates demonstrate that there is both inter- and intracohort density dependence in the survival of the juvenile cod (Table 1). The parameters β and γ are negative and their 95% ci does not include 0. The parameters for the Southern and Northern areas are very similar. The baseline juvenile survival, ec, does not differ significantly between the two areas and is estimated to be about 0.6 (Southern, 0.56; Northern, 0.68; Table 1). The estimated age-structured interactions greatly reduce the survival. In years of median abundance of juveniles (for comparison, we use the grand median, which is 37 for 1-group and 333 for the 0-group), the survival of juveniles is estimated to be approximately 0.1 (Southern, 0.10, ci = {0.09, 0.11}; Northern: 0.15, ci = {0.14, 0.15}). Fig. 2 shows the model of the 1-group abundance for the Southern area. When the four most outlying observations are omitted from the analyses, the between-cohort interaction is of comparable strength to the within-cohort interaction (β ≈ γ ≈ −0.2).

Table 1.

The parameters in the ecological model (equations 1–3) were estimated from the survey data of the Southern and Northern areas using the Gibbs sampler as implemented in bugs, version 0.6 (22, 34)

Parameter Southern area
Northern area
Estimate 95% ci Estimate 95% ci
αconst 2.73 {2.72, 2.74} 2.13 {2.12, 2.15}
2.58 {2.32, 2.84} 1.93 {1.59, 2.27}
σα2 1.12 {0.79, 1.59} 0.63 {0.48, 0.84}
β −0.26 {−0.31, −0.21} −0.22 {−0.26, −0.17}
γ −0.06 {−0.09, −0.02} −0.07 {−0.11, −0.04}
c −0.58 {−0.91, −0.26} −0.39 {−0.71, −0.06}
λ Fixed at 0.4 N/A

A Poisson error was assumed for the counts of the 0- and 1-groups. Annual adult survival, λ, was fixed at 0.4. The reproductive parameter, α, was either assumed to be constant (αconst) or randomly distributed with variance, σα2, around a time-invariant mean, ᾱ. The parameters for intracohort (β) and intercohort (γ) density dependence and basal survival (c) were assumed to be time invariant. All parameters were assigned noninformative priors (22, 34). Estimates and 95% ci were based on 100,000 iterations of the Gibbs sampler after a burn-in of 10,000 iterations. N/A, not applicable. 

The per capita reproductive parameter in the constant recruitment model, eαconst, predicts that an adult produces approximately 10 eggs that successfully hatch, metamorphose, and enter the 0-group (Southern, 15; Northern, 8). The constant recruitment model predicts dampened oscillations in abundance, because the intercohort interactions are too weak to induce stable limit cycles (see “Age-Structured Model”). The spectral density of the model (including the transient period) is L-shaped (with a peak at 2 years). Arguably, this model may be seen as capturing the short-term fluctuations in cod abundance (the 2- to 3-year periodicity). It does, however, fail to account for any low-frequency behavior. Some essential part of the cod dynamics appears to be missing from the deterministic model (35).

Allowing the reproduction to be variable does not affect estimates of the interaction strengths. It does, however, demonstrate significant interannual variability in the per capita reproductive rate (Table 1). The mean reproductive rate does not differ significantly from the constant reproduction model. The reproductive variance, σα2, is approximately unity and is significantly larger than 0 in both areas (Table 1). The coefficient of variation in αt is estimated to be 0.41 (with ci = {0.34, 0.51}) for the Southern area and 0.63 (with ci = {0.48, 0.84}) for the Northern area. Typical years may be considered those that fall within 1 SD of the mean. The estimated variability is such that the per capita production of eggs that successfully hatch, metamorphose, and enter the 0-group may vary by a factor of 5 between two such typical years (ᾱ ± σα: Southern, {4.6, 38.0}; Northern, {3.1, 15.0}).

The stochastic model has a spectral density that differs nontrivially from its deterministic counterpart (Fig. 3A). As in the deterministic model, there is a strong cyclic component corresponding to a 2- to 3-year oscillation. However, there is an additional low-frequency component to the dynamics. The bimodal spectrum of realizations of the stochastic model (Fig. 3A) bears resemblance to that of the data. Repeated simulations (across 50 iterations corresponding to the numbers of contiguous years in the data) and spectral estimation from the stochastic model reveal that the model can account for the bimodal spectrum of the coastal cod populations. Thus, both the cyclicity and the low-frequency variability observed in the cod dynamics can be predicted from the model. The observed spectrum in the data is enveloped by the 95% distribution of spectra from the stochastic model (Fig. 3). When the model is run for 5,000 iterations with the Northern parameter estimates (Table 1), the dominant period is always the low-frequency behavior with an apparent period of more than 100 years (median across 1,000 simulations, 175 years; interquartile range, {101, 370}). The subdominant period is the 2-year cycle (median, 2.1 years; interquartile range, {2.0, 2.2}). The model for the Southern area has a similar behavior (median dominant period, 153 years; interquartile range, {88, 313}; median subdominant period, 2.2 years; interquartile range, {2.1, 2.3}).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

(A) The spectral density of the 0-group count (for the post-World War II Southern data) is represented by the thick white line. It has significant peaks in the low- and the high-frequency ends of the spectrum (see text). The shaded area represents the 95% envelope for the spectrum of the optimally parameterized age-structured model (Table 1). The envelope is estimated across 1,000 simulations each of 50 years after discarding a transient period of 100 years. The model spectrum has a distinct bimodal shape that embraces the spectrum of the 0-group counts (thick white line). The spectra of three of the 1,000 realizations of the stochastic model are shown as thin black lines (labeled B, C, and D). (BD) The time trajectories of the three stochastic realizations whose spectra are shown in A. The smooth lines represent running averages to highlight apparent long-term trends (the dynamics are really stationary).

DISCUSSION

We studied a simple model for cod dynamics that included density-dependent interactions and interannual variation in reproductive rates. The parameters in the model were estimated from survey data from the Norwegian Skagerrak coast between 1921 and 1994. We demonstrated here a significant inter- and intracohort density dependence in juvenile mortality, and estimated the magnitude of the recruitment variation. By using the parameter estimates, we showed that the model will give rise to short-term cycles in abundance superimposed on long-term trends when there is reproductive variability.

The estimated strength of the density dependence in juvenile survival of cod (Table 1) is comparable to estimates previously reported for other cod populations (27). The estimated per capita production of juveniles (approximately 8–15 0-group juveniles per adult) also compares well with previous reports (36, 37). One of the main features of the demography of the Norwegian Skagerrak cod is a low age of maturation (2–3 years) and a short lifespan (<5 years) (18, 25). A cycle in abundance caused by interactions in age-structured populations is typically related to time to maturation (ref. 38, but see ref. 17). Any cycles are therefore likely to have longer period in Atlantic cod populations that have longer lifespans and slower maturation rates. The qualitative aspects of the dynamics, i.e., the possibility of a bimodal spectrum with both cycles and trends, are, however relatively robust to time to maturation and lifespan (unpublished numerical analyses). Log-linear density dependence in the mean reproductive rate or adult survival rate does not affect the type of dynamics observed (26). The estimation is insensitive to the assumption of an adult survival rate of 0.4. Assuming higher or lower survival in the estimation will result in altered (lower or higher, respectively) estimates of the mean per capita reproductive rates, but no other estimates are affected. The predicted dynamics are unchanged. The critical feature appears to be the combination of variability in reproduction and asymmetric intercohort interactions.

Simple deterministic (35) or stochastic (30, 33) models of fluctuating populations typically predict that spectra will be dominated by high frequencies (to be “blue shifted”) (35, 39, 40). The discrepancy between the prediction of these simple models and the “multicolored spectra” (i.e., also containing low-frequency signals) of real populations has received recent attention (35, 3941). Spatial dynamics (40) or long-term stochastic forcing (refs. 42 and 43, but see ref. 41) has been invoked to account for the discrepancy. This study shows that stochastic (white noise) recruitment can be echoed through the age structure to produce a two-peaked spectrum (cycle and trend) from a single set of density-dependent mechanisms. Disruptive forces (such as stochastic reproduction) have previously been shown to be capable of sustaining transient periodicity in population models (4446). Here we show that disruptive forces can interact with regulatory mechanisms to produce dynamics that differ qualitatively from both the purely stochastic and the purely deterministic analogs.

The effect that (uncorrelated) stochasticity in reproductive rates can generate low-frequency variability in dynamics may appear counterintuitive because the deterministic age-structured model exhibits high-frequency cycles only. Stenseth et al. (26) investigated a simplified version of the model (equations 1–3) with only one adult age class and showed that this may be written in delay coordinates to obtain a model of the form:

graphic file with name M4.gif 4

where, xt = log(Xt), η is a constant, and the other symbols are as defined above. The random component of this model, αt − γαt−1, represents an explicit formulation of how random reproduction, {αt}, is dynamically echoed in time (recall that γ < 0). Ecologically speaking, the effect is caused by the intercohort interaction (γ) in the age-structured dynamics operating as a “resonator” for the variability in recruitment.** Statistically speaking, however, the random component represents a noncentered moving average process. Moving average processes are generally capable of inducing low-frequency behavior (49). Therefore, this effect is the dynamic cause of the presence of low-frequency signals in the model trajectory. Because the intercohort interaction is estimated to be fairly weak and the reproductive variance fairly high, the resonant effect is relatively weak in the model realizations (Fig. 3). To illustrate the effect more fully, we simulate a system with strong interactions (β = −0.3, γ = −0.9) and low reproductive variance (ᾱ = 2, σα2 = 0.01). This set of parameters yields a low coefficient of variation in the per capita reproduction rate (the coefficient of variation of eαt is 0.1). Fig. 4 shows 100 years of simulation (after a transient period of 50 years) of two stochastic realizations and one deterministic realization (αconst = 2). The low-frequency oscillations arising from the resonance is subdominant in power to the 2-year cycle for this set of parameters but nevertheless highly conspicuous.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

The simulated 0-group count from stochastic (A and B) and deterministic (C) realizations of the age-structured model with strong intra- and intercohort interactions (β = −0.3, γ = −0.9). The parameters used for survival and reproduction are: c = 0, λ = 0.4, α = 2, σα2 = 0.01, αconst = 2 (see text). The 100 years of simulation are given after a transient period of 50 years.

The age-structured model may account for both the cycles and trends in abundance of the Skagerrak cod populations. However, external factors such as pollution, harvesting, and climatic changes should not be ruled out. Such factors have already been shown to account for some of the variability in fish populations (13, 19, 50). Community interactions are also likely to be of importance (13). Our main aim here is to point to an interesting alternative source of low-frequency behavior that deserves future study. Future studies must be undertaken to understand which of these causal mechanisms are the most important in governing the abundance of the cod populations along the Norwegian Skagerrak cod.

Asymmetric interactions between age or stage classes are common in many animal life cycles (51) and are a major source of nonlinearity and of dynamic complexity in deterministic population models (16, 52, 53). We show that when age-structured dynamics occur in a varying environment, both cycles and long-term trends may arise. Nonlinear interactions combined with stochastic reproduction can induce resonant effects that represent a neglected level of dynamic richness. Separating anthropogenic and environmentally induced trends from this intrinsic low-frequency variability is a challenge that will require more attention.

Acknowledgments

The late Rangvald Løversen and Aadne Sollie carried out the survey every year for more than 70 years. Tore Johannessen provided help with the data. Rolf A. Ims, Fiorenza Micheli, and three anonymous reviewers commented on the manuscript. We received financial support from the Norwegian Science Foundation (O.N.B. and N.C.S.), the University of Oslo (N.C.S.), and the Marine Sciences and Technologies program of the European Union (J.M.F.).

ABBREVIATION

ci

Bayesian credible intervals

Footnotes

**

This mechanism is not a form of stochastic resonance in which the signal to noise ratio increases with increasing noise levels (47, 48). The signal to noise ratio of the present model decreases monotonically with reproductive variance.

References

  • 1.Myers R A, Hutchings J A, Barrowman N J. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1996;138:293–308. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Cook R M, Sinclair A, Stefansson G. Nature (London) 1997;385:521–522. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dickson R R, Brander K. Fish Oceanogr. 1993;2:124–153. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ottersen G, Sundby S. Fish Oceanogr. 1995;4:278–292. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hjort J. Rapp P-v Réun Cons Int Explor Mer. 1914;20:1–228. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cushing D H. Population Production and Regulation in the Sea: A Fisheries Perspective. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge Univ. Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sundby S, Bjorke H, Soldal A V, Olsen S. Rapp P-v Réun Cons Perm Int Explor Mer. 1989;191:351–358. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ellersten B, Fossum P, Solemdal P, Sundby S. Rapp P-v Réun Cons int Explor Mer. 1989;191:209–219. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sundby S, Fossum P. J Plankton Res. 1990;12:1153–1162. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.May R C. In: The Early Life History of Fish. Blaxter J H S, editor. New York: Springer; 1974. pp. 3–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cushing D H. Adv Mar Biol. 1990;26:249–293. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fortier L, Villeneuve A. Fish Bull. 1996;94:268–281. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Caley M J, Carr M H, Hixon M A, Hughes T P, Jones G P, Menge B A. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1996;27:477–500. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bailey K M, Houde E D. Adv Mar Biol. 1989;26:1–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cushing J M. Theor Popul Biol. 1992;42:347–361. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cushing J M. J Math Biol. 1994;32:705–729. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mertz G, Myers R A. Math Biosci. 1996;131:157–171. doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(95)00036-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fromentin J-M, Stenseth N C, Gjøsæter J, Bjørnstad O, Falk W, Johannessen T. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1997;55:209–222. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Fromentin J-M, Stenseth N C, Gjøsæter J, Johannesen T, Planque B. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1998;162:265–278. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bjørnstad O N, Fromentin J-M, Stenseth N C, Gjøsæter J. Ecology. 1999;80:1278–1288. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Best N G, Spiegelhalter D J, Thomas A, Brayne C E G. J R Stat Soc A. 1996;159:323–342. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gilks, W. R., Richardson, S. & Spiegelhalter, D. J. (1996) (Chapman & Hall, London).
  • 23.Manly B F J. Randomization, Bootstrap and Monte Carlo Methods in Biology. London: Chapman & Hall; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dalley E L, Anderson J T. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 1997;54:168–176. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gjøsæter J, Enersen K, Enersen S E. Fisken Havet. 1996;23:1–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Stenseth, N. C., Bjørnstad, O. N., Falck, W., Fromentin, J.-M., Gjøsæter, J. & Gray, J. S. (1999) Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B, submitted.
  • 27.Myers R A, Cadigan N G. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 1993;50:1576–1590. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Julliard, R., Stenseth, N. C., Gjøsæter, J., Lekve, K., Fromentin, J.-M. & Danielsen, D. S. (1999) Ecol. Appl., submitted.
  • 29.Cushing J M, Dennis B, Desharnais R A, Costantino R F. Ecol Modell. 1996;92:111–119. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Engen S, Lande R. Math Biosci. 1996;132:169–183. doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(95)00054-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sugihara G, May R M. Nature (London) 1990;344:734–741. doi: 10.1038/344734a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Longford N T. Random Coefficient Models. Oxford: Clarendon; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dennis B, Patil G P. Math Biosci. 1984;68:187–212. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Spiegelhalter D J, Thomas A, Best N G, Wilks W R. bugs. Institute of Public Health, Cambridge, U.K.: MRC Biostatistics Unit; 1995. Version 0.6. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cohen J E. Nature (London) 1995;378:610–612. doi: 10.1038/378610a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Myers, R. A., Mertz, G. & Barrowman, N. J. (1996) Int. Counc. Explor. Sea Tech. Rep. D 11.
  • 37.Myers R A, Barrowman N J, Hutchings J A, Rosenberg A A. Science. 1995;269:1106–1108. doi: 10.1126/science.269.5227.1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Nisbet R M, Onyiah L C. J Math Biol. 1994;32:329–344. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Sugihara G. Nature (London) 1995;378:559–560. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.White A, Begon M, Bowers R G. Proc R Soc London Ser B. 1996;263:1731–1737. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kaitala V, Ylikarjula J, Ranta E, Lundberg P. Proc R Soc London Ser B. 1997;264:943–948. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Pimm S L, Readfern A. Nature (London) 1988;334:613–614. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Halley J M. Trend Ecol Evol. 1996;11:33–37. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(96)81067-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kaitala V, Ranta E, Lindstrom J. Ann Zool Fenn. 1996;33:275–282. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Higgins K, Hastings A, Sarvela J N, Botsford L W. Science. 1997;276:1431–1435. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Stenseth N C, Bjørnstad O N, Saitoh T. Proc R Soc London Ser B. 1996;263:1117–1126. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1996.0164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wiesenfeld K, Moss F. Nature (London) 1995;373:33–36. doi: 10.1038/373033a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Huppert A, Stone L. Am Nat. 1998;152:447–459. doi: 10.1086/286181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Priestley M B. Spectral Analysis and Time Series. London: Academic; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hutchings J A. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 1996;53:943–962. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Tuljapurkar S, Caswell H. Structured-Population Models in Marine, Terrestrial, and Freshwater Systems. New York: Chapman & Hall; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Higgins K, Hastings A, Botsford L W. Am Nat. 1997;149:247–269. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Costantino R F, Desharnais R A, Cushing J M, Dennis B. Science. 1997;275:389–391. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5298.389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES