Abstract
Voltage-dependent inhibition of N- and P/Q-type calcium channels by G proteins is crucial for presynaptic inhibition of neurotransmitter release, and may contribute importantly to short-term synaptic plasticity. Such calcium-channel modulation could thereby impact significantly the neuro-computational repertoire of neural networks. The differential modulation of N and P/Q channels could even further enrich their impact upon synaptic tuning. Here, we performed in-depth comparison of the G-protein inhibition of recombinant N and P/Q channels, expressed in HEK 293 cells with the m2 muscarinic receptor. While both channel types display classic features of G-protein modulation (kinetic slowing of activation, prepulse facilitation, and voltage dependence of inhibition), we confirmed previously reported quantitative differences, with N channels displaying stronger inhibition and greater relief of inhibition by prepulses. A more fundamental, qualitative difference in the modulation of these two channels was revealed by a modified tail-activation paradigm, as well as by a novel “slope” analysis method comparing time courses of slow activation and prepulse facilitation. The stark contrast in modulatory behavior can be understood within the context of the “willing–reluctant” model, in which binding of G-protein βγ subunits to channels induces a reluctant mode of gating, where stronger depolarization is required for opening. Our experiments suggest that only N channels could be opened in the reluctant mode, at voltages normally spanned by neuronal action potentials. By contrast, P/Q channels appear to remain closed, especially over these physiological voltages. Further, the differential occurrence of reluctant openings is not explained by differences in the rate of G-protein unbinding from the two channels. These two scenarios predict very different effects of G-protein inhibition on the waveform of Ca2+ entry during action potentials, with potentially important consequences for the timing and efficacy of synaptic transmission.
Keywords: α1A, α1B, channel modulation, heterologous expression, short-term synaptic plasticity
INTRODUCTION
High voltage-activated calcium channels (N, L, P/Q and R type) are present in central and peripheral neurons, where they gate Ca2+ influx to mediate and regulate such essential Ca2+-dependent processes as gene expression, membrane excitability, and synaptic transmission (Tsien and Tsien 1990; Bito et al. 1997). In tune with their vital role in cellular control, calcium channels are modulated by an array of intracellular signaling pathways, providing powerful means to fine tune biological function by controlling Ca2+ influx.
One important and widespread form of calcium channel modulation is voltage-dependent inhibition of N- and P/Q-type channels by receptor-coupled, heterotrimeric G proteins (Wanke et al. 1987; Ikeda and Schofield 1989; Lipscombe et al. 1989; Elmslie et al. 1990; Zhu and Ikeda 1994). Direct binding of G-protein βγ subunits (Gβγ) to distinct domains on the channels is thought to produce the channel inhibition (Herlitze et al. 1996; Ikeda 1996; Zhang et al. 1996; De Waard et al. 1997; Qin et al. 1997; Zamponi et al. 1997; Furukawa et al. 1998a,Furukawa et al. 1998b; Zamponi and Snutch 1998; Canti et al. 1999). As N- and P/Q-type channels figure prominently in triggering neurotransmitter release (Luebke et al. 1993; Takahashi and Momiyama 1993; Wheeler et al. 1994; Wu and Saggau 1994; Reid et al. 1998), this type of modulation is a dominant mechanism for presynaptic inhibition of synaptic transmission (Wu and Saggau 1997). Such modulation may also contribute to short-term synaptic plasticity by virtue of the transient relief of G-protein inhibition with depolarization (Elmslie et al. 1990; Miller 1990; Wu and Saggau 1994; Dittman and Regehr 1996; Brody et al. 1997; Brody and Yue 1999). Even more intriguing is the potential for differential G-protein modulation of N- and P/Q-type channels to produce selective regulation of synaptic efficacy at spatially distinct sites, as varying proportions of N- and P/Q-type channels mediate synaptic transmission at different synapses (Luebke et al. 1993; Takahashi and Momiyama 1993; Wheeler et al. 1994; Wu and Saggau 1994; Reid et al. 1998). In fact, some quantitative differences in the strength of G-protein modulation of N- and P/Q-type channels have been documented (Mintz and Bean 1993; Bourinet et al. 1996; Zhang et al. 1996; Currie and Fox 1997), with the general trend that N-type channels are more strongly modulated than P/Q-type channels (but see Meza and Adams 1998). As well, the contrasts in the extent of modulation may relate to certain quantitative differences in the kinetics of Gβγ interaction with N- and P/Q-type channels (Zhang et al. 1996). However, it is unknown whether there are fundamental qualitative differences in the modulation of N- and P/Q-type channels, and there remain substantial gaps in the comparison of G-protein modulation of these two channels, particularly as determined under uniform experimental conditions.
Here, the overall goal was to undertake in-depth analyses of the G-protein inhibition of N- and P/Q-type calcium channels, exploiting reconstitution of such modulation in HEK 293 cells transfected with recombinant channels and receptors. To articulate the specific scope of the experiments and results, we first review the “willing-reluctant” model of voltage-dependent G-protein inhibition of neuronal calcium channels (Bean 1989; Elmslie et al. 1990), a leading mechanistic framework for rationalizing the key functional features of this form of modulation. According to this model (Fig. 1), uninhibited channels exist in a “willing” mode of gating in which depolarization induces ready opening of the channel in state O, and activated G proteins inhibit the channel by favoring occupancy of a “reluctant” mode of gating where longer and/or stronger depolarizations are required to open in state O′ (G-protein inhibited). As activation of current entails voltage-dependent movement of channels through a series of closed states (C in the willing mode, C′ in the reluctant mode) to their respective open states (O and O′), the contrasting propensities to open in the two modes reflect quantitative differences in their respective activation pathways (Jones et al. 1997). Elmslie et al. 1990 explicitly incorporated the idea that conversions between willing and reluctant modes of gating reflect binding and unbinding of activated G proteins to and from the channel. The voltage dependence of G-protein inhibition, alluded to above in relation to the tendency for depolarization to transiently relieve inhibition, is explained by the proposed state dependence of Gβγ binding to the channel (Boland and Bean 1993): Gβγ is presumed to have a high affinity for channels in deep closed conformations towards the left of each mode, and a lower affinity for conformations near or in the open states (O and O′). Hence, by driving channels towards different positions along the activation pathway, changes in voltage alter Gβγ affinity and thereby redistribute channels between willing and reluctant modes.
Classic features of voltage-dependent inhibition are then explained in the following manner. (a) The slowing of the activation process induced by G proteins (“kinetic slowing”) (Luebke and Dunlap 1994) corresponds to depolarization driving reluctant channels towards conformations near the reluctant open state (O′), whereupon Gβγ dissociates slowly and allows initially reluctant channels to open in the willing mode with higher open probability (Elmslie et al. 1990; Boland and Bean 1993; Patil et al. 1996; Jones and Elmslie 1997). In this scenario, the gradual time course of Gβγ unbinding accounts for the slow phase of activation. (b) The relative extent of inhibition is generally diminished with increasing depolarization (Bean 1989; Boland and Bean 1993), possibly because either the Gβγ dissociation described above is more complete, or reluctant channels are forced open to a greater extent. (c) The transient relief of inhibition measured during a test pulse that follows a large depolarizing prepulse (“prepulse facilitation”) (Forscher and Oxford 1985; Marchetti et al. 1986; Elmslie et al. 1990; Boland and Bean 1993; Mintz and Bean 1993; Hille 1994) could occur because the prepulse brings about Gβγ dissociation, which is only partially reversed during a short interpulse. Hence, the population of channels is still biased towards the willing mode during the ensuing test pulse, resulting in facilitation of current.
The reluctant–willing model points to a crucial difference that could exist among different channels in regard to the prevalence of reluctant openings. In some channel types, the kinetic structure of the channel might enforce a “preferential exchange” scenario (Patil et al. 1996), in which Gβγ unbinding always predominates before appreciable opening of reluctant channels. In other channel types, a “permissive exchange” scenario may hold (Patil et al. 1996), where sufficiently strong depolarization is capable of driving reluctant channels to open appreciably in O′ before bound Gβγ subunits have time to unbind (Elmslie et al. 1990; Boland and Bean 1993). These two scenarios predict fundamentally different effects of G-protein inhibition on the waveform of Ca2+ entry during action potentials (Brody et al. 1997), with potentially important physiological consequences for the timing and efficacy of synaptic transmission (Bertram et al. 1996; Borst and Sakmann 1998; Sabatini and Regehr 1999).
In this study, we provide evidence for a fundamental qualitative difference in the G-protein modulation of N- versus P/Q-type calcium channels. Using whole-cell voltage-clamp techniques and novel analyses, we provide compelling arguments that reluctant openings readily occur in N-type channels at physiologically relevant voltages. By contrast, occupation of the reluctant open state appears to be rare for P/Q-type channels.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Expression of Recombinant Channels and Receptors
Low passage number HEK 293 cells (<20 passages) were transiently transfected with 8 μg each of cDNAs encoding calcium channel α1A (rat brain, rbA-1) (Stea et al. 1994) or α1B (human, α1B-1) (Williams et al. 1992), rat brain β2a (Perez-Reyes et al. 1992), and rat α2bδ subunits (Tomlinson et al. 1993), together with 2 μg cDNA encoding the m2 muscarinic receptor (Peralta et al. 1987), using the calcium phosphate precipitation method. All constructs were subcloned into cytomegalovirus expression plasmids. HEK 293 cells were maintained as previously described (Brody et al. 1997).
Electrophysiology
Whole cell currents were obtained at room temperature, 2–3 d after transfection, by standard patch clamp methods with an Axopatch 200A (Axon Instruments). Cells were continuously perfused with a bath solution containing (mM): 150 tetraethylammonium methane sulfonate (TEA-MeSO3), 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, and 10 HEPES, pH 7.4 adjusted with TEA-OH. Where noted, 50 μM carbachol was included in the superfusing medium to activate G-proteins via the transfected m2 muscarinic receptor. Internal solution contained (mM): 135 Cs-MeSO3, 5 CsCl, 10 EGTA, 1 MgCl2, 4 MgATP, 0.3 LiGTP, and 10 HEPES, pH 7.3 adjusted with CsOH. Pipette series resistances were typically <1 MΩ after 70–80% compensation. In protocols in which tail currents were analyzed, signals were filtered at 10 kHz (four-pole Bessel) and sampled at 20-μs intervals. In protocols in which step currents alone were analyzed, signals were filtered at 2 kHz and sampled every 40 μs. Data traces were acquired at a repetition interval of 30 s. All voltages were corrected for a liquid junction potential of −11 mV before recording. Leaks and capacitance transients were subtracted by a P/8 protocol. For displayed traces, we often subtracted a smooth curve fitted to the leak currents. Data were analyzed using custom-written MATLAB software (MathWorks). Pooled data are presented as mean ± SEM.
RESULTS
Reconstitution of Voltage-dependent G-Protein Inhibition of N- and P/Q-type Calcium Channels
Transfection of HEK 293 cells with cDNA encoding the N-type (α1B/β2a/α2δ) or the P/Q-type (α1A/β2a/α2) calcium channel, along with the m2 muscarinic receptor, enabled the recording of robust Ca2+ currents that could be modulated by the muscarinic agonist carbachol (CCh). Application of a supramaximal dose of CCh (50 μM) resulted in the inhibition of both N- and P/Q-type Ca2+ currents (Fig. 2 and Fig. 3) in a manner that recapitulated classic features of voltage-dependent inhibition, as observed in neurons (Bean 1989; Kasai 1992) and heterologous systems expressing recombinant channels (Patil et al. 1996). First, application of CCh produced kinetic slowing of channel activation (Luebke and Dunlap 1994), as evident from comparison of exemplar currents in the absence (−CCh) and presence (+CCh) of carbachol (Fig. 2 and Fig. 3, left). By contrast to control currents, which primarily activated according to a single rapid component, inhibited traces showed clearly distinguishable fast and slow components of activation. Such kinetic slowing resulted in the degree of inhibition being greatest when measured shortly after the start of depolarization, after appreciable activation of the fast, but not slow component. Henceforth, inhibition of test-pulse current was therefore quantified via peak current determined in the interval between 3 and 5 ms from the start of voltage steps, a parameter that conveniently approximates the amplitude of the fast-activating component. A second feature was the full reversibility of inhibition during maintained depolarization, as demonstrated by the convergence of inhibited and control traces at the end of depolarizing steps to sufficiently positive potentials. This finding implies the reconstitution of a purely voltage-dependent form of G-protein inhibition of both channel types. Convergent, voltage-independent mechanisms of G-protein inhibition are also present in certain preparations (Hille 1992; Luebke and Dunlap 1994); their absence in HEK 293 cells simplifies subsequent in-depth analysis of the voltage-dependent mechanism alone. Another perspective on the voltage-dependent nature of inhibition came from comparison of current–voltage relations ±CCh (Fig. 2 A and 3 A, right). Inhibition of current was largest at low or moderate depolarizations, but essentially absent at voltages greater than +40 mV (Bean 1989; Boland and Bean 1993). Finally, although the relief of inhibition by depolarization was transient, the relief did persist for some milliseconds after large depolarizing prepulses (Fig. 2 B and 3 B, left), in a fashion that recapitulated prepulse facilitation as observed in neurons (Grassi and Lux 1989; Elmslie et al. 1990). Under control conditions (−CCh), the amplitude of the fast-activating component of test-pulse current was barely larger after a 30-ms prepulse to +100 mV (○) than without a prepulse (•), consistent with minimal basal activation of G proteins. However, in the presence of CCh, the prepulse produced marked facilitation of test-pulse currents (○), with fast-activating components nearly as large as observed without CCh (Fig. 2 B and 3 B, right). Interestingly, basal inhibition was completely absent upon washout of CCh (Fig. 2 B and 3 B, right) (Roche and Treistman 1998).
Although these results suggested that G-protein inhibition of the recombinant N-type channel was broadly similar to that of the P/Q-type channel, closer examination of the exemplar data (Fig. 2 and Fig. 3) pointed to quantitative differences that might exist in the degree of inhibition and facilitation, as observed elsewhere (Mintz and Bean 1993; Bayliss et al. 1995; Bourinet et al. 1996; Zhang et al. 1996; Currie and Fox 1997; Roche and Treistman 1998). Population data from a number of cells firmly established this impression. N-type currents evoked by step depolarizations to +10 mV were inhibited by 48.8 ± 1.3% (n = 13), compared with 34.5 ± 2.4% (n = 9; P < 0.005, one-tailed unpaired t test) for P/Q-type channels, comparable with the degree of inhibition observed in native preparations (Bean 1989; Mintz and Bean 1993; Currie and Fox 1997). The stronger inhibition resulted in greater prepulse facilitation of N-type channels (2.69 ± 0.31-fold, n = 7) compared with P/Q-type channels (1.50 ± 0.07-fold, n = 6; P < 0.005, one-tailed unpaired t test), as determined by the voltage protocols in Fig. 2 B and 3 B.
Tail G-V Relations Hint at Fundamental Differences in the Prevalence of “Reluctant Openings” in N- and P/Q-type Channels
Having established the overall modulatory behavior of N- and P/Q-type channels, we turned to experiments that suggested a more fundamental, qualitative difference in the prevalence of reluctant openings manifested by these channels. Tail G-V protocols usually feature relatively long (10–20 ms) voltage steps preceding tail currents. With such relatively long depolarizing steps, the reluctant–willing model predicts two mechanisms by which the tail G-V curve with CCh should be right-shifted with respect to the control curve without CCh (Bean 1989; Mintz and Bean 1993; Patil et al. 1996). First, strong depolarization is required for Gβγ to unbind from reluctant channels (Fig. 4 A, pathway a), permitting subsequent high probability opening in the willing mode. Second, channels in the reluctant mode might exhibit reluctant openings (O′) within the same mode (Fig. 4 A, pathway b), but only with strong depolarization. Both mechanisms predict that greater depolarization is required to open channels that are initially in the reluctant mode, compared with that required to open initially willing channels.
If the duration of the voltage step preceding tail currents is reduced to 3 ms (Fig. 4), then the reluctant–willing model predicts only one mechanism by which carbachol would right-shift the G-V curve. Given that the measured time constant for exchange between reluctant and willing modes is >10 ms for voltages up to +50 mV (see Fig. 10), relatively little depolarization-driven conversion from reluctant to willing modes should occur within 3 ms, thus minimizing the first mechanism for right-shifting G-V curves (Fig. 4 A, pathway a). The only remaining mechanism requires that strong depolarization be capable of forcing reluctant channels to open in O′ (Fig. 4 A, case 1, pathway b). If depolarization cannot induce such reluctant openings, then initially reluctant channels would not open appreciably by any pathway (Fig. 4 A, case 2). In the latter case, the overall G-V curve with CCh would be a scaled-down version of the uninhibited G-V curve, where the reduction in amplitude scales directly with the carbachol-induced decrease in the number of initially willing channels. Fig. 4 B summarizes the results of this test for reluctant openings, as applied to N-type channels. With 3-ms depolarizations preceding tail currents, the tail G-V appears right-shifted and smaller in amplitude with CCh than without (Fig. 4 B, middle). The actual test comes with normalization of the G-V curves ±CCh relative to each other (Fig. 4 B, right). To satisfy the test prerequisite that interchange between reluctant and willing modes be minimal, the curves were normalized at +50 mV, and interpretation was limited to more negative potentials. After such normalization, the normalized G-V curve with CCh displayed a prominent rightward shift in the relevant voltage range (Fig. 4 B, right), clearly contradicting the predicted outcome for no reluctant openings, and instead supporting their robust presence. In striking contrast, tail G-V curves for control and G-protein–inhibited P/Q-type channels essentially coincided after normalization at +50 mV (Fig. 4 C), suggesting the virtual absence of reluctant openings. This remarkable difference in the behavior of normalized G-V curves for N- and P/Q-type channels provided the first suggestion of a fundamental qualitative difference in the G-protein inhibition of N- versus P/Q-type channels: it appeared that inhibited N-type, but not P/Q-type channels could open in the reluctant mode.
The strength of this suggestion was critically dependent on the extent to which exchange between reluctant and willing modes was minimized by the 3-ms duration of depolarizing steps. Furthermore, the experimental design in Fig. 4 did not provide a practical approach to estimate the relative prevalence of reluctant openings at various voltages, as would be required to gauge the relevance of such openings at physiological potentials. These limitations motivated the development of a complementary strategy for determining the prevalence of reluctant openings.
“Slope-Analysis” Method for Detection of Reluctant Openings
As a starting point for developing the complementary strategy, we applied a voltage protocol similar to one initially described by Boland and Bean 1993. Fig. 5 A shows the specific paradigm, which compares the kinetics of N-type channel activation with the time evolution of prepulse facilitation, all during exposure to carbachol. Activation was gauged by the amplitude of the tail current (I tail) at −30 mV, following 100-mV prepulses of increasing duration. Facilitation was gauged by the increase in the fast-activating phase of the current (I peak) elicited by an ensuing test pulse to +10 mV. The time courses of the two processes are shown in Fig. 5 B, where normalized versions of these parameters, I tail,norm (•) and I peak,norm (○), reflect activation and facilitation, respectively. The I tail,norm curve indicated a roughly biexponential time course of activation (smooth curve), the slow phase of which is a familiar feature of kinetic slowing. The time course of facilitation could be represented by a constant offset plus a single exponential with τ = 5.5 ms. If channels in the reluctant mode cannot open, even with the 100-mV depolarization during the prepulse, then the slow phase of activation and the time course of facilitation (Fig. 5 B) would be expected to coincide precisely in amplitude and time course, because both plots reflect unbinding of G proteins, and only willing channels can open. As shown in Fig. 5 B, however, the plots for these two processes are clearly disjoint (Boland and Bean 1993), which argues for the occurrence of reluctant openings in N-type channels during prepulse depolarization to +100 mV.
Quantitative conceptualization of the experiment in Fig. 5 enabled us to generalize the approach, leading to a slope analysis method for detecting reluctant openings at different voltages. To articulate this novel method, we first developed equations that provide a first-order description of the processes of facilitation and activation. Fig. 6A–C, shows fits of these equations to the N-type channel data in Fig. 5, using a format that aids intuitive understanding of the functional forms. The normalized time course of facilitation (I peak,norm) is shown in Fig. 6 A, and can be described as:
1 |
where A initial,peak and A slow,peak are the magnitudes of the initial constant offset and slow-exponential component of the curve, D is the prepulse duration, and τ is presumably the time constant for conversion of initially reluctant channels into the willing mode. If we assume that the modest test potential (+10 mV) used to elicit I peak does not evoke appreciable reluctant openings, it follows that the amplitude of the initial offset would scale directly with the fraction of initially willing channels at the holding potential (F W), normalized by the fraction of willing channels that would be observed with prolonged depolarization at the prepulse voltage V pre. The normalization factor would then be F W + [F R − F Rmin(V pre)], where the term in parentheses is the increase in the fraction of willing channels over the course of maintained depolarization to V pre, F R is the fraction of initially reluctant channels at the holding potential, and F Rmin(V pre) is the fraction of channels that would remain in the reluctant mode during maintained depolarization to V pre. As such, F W and F R depend on the magnitude of the steady state inhibition at the holding potential (always −100 mV), and F Rmin depends on the steady state inhibition at various step potentials. The initial offset amplitude is therefore:
2 |
where P o(Vtest) is the steady state open probability of willing channels at Vtest. Because the amplitudes of the initial offset and slow-exponential component sum to unity, the slow-exponential amplitude must be 1 − A initial,peak, which is:
3 |
The contributions of these two amplitudes are represented graphically in Fig. 6 A.
Similarly, after the fast phase of activation has occurred, the normalized time course of channel activation can be represented as (Fig. 6 B):
4 |
where A fast,tail is the amplitude of the fast-activating component and A slow,tail is the amplitude of the slow-activating component. During strong depolarization, A fast,tail may have contributions from the opening of both willing and reluctant channels, as specified by:
where P o(V pre) is the steady state open probability of willing channels at V pre, and P o′(V pre) is the steady state open probability of reluctant channels at V pre. i and ii reflect the respective normalized contributions of initially willing and reluctant channels to overall opening in the fast-activating phase, and the magnitudes of these two terms are represented graphically in Fig. 6 B. The scaling factor S in the denominator reflects the overall open probability that would be expected during maintained depolarization to V pre. S therefore includes not only two terms corresponding to the contributions of initially willing and reluctant channels [F W P o(V pre) + F R P o′(V pre)], but an additional term representing the increased open probability brought about by that fraction of channels that converts from reluctant to willing modes {[F R − F Rmin(V pre)] × [P o(V pre) − P o′(V pre)]}. The slow component of I tail,norm reflects the same increase in open probability brought about by that fraction of channels that converts from reluctant to willing modes. The amplitude of the slow component, represented graphically in Fig. 6 B, iii, is easily calculated from 1 − A fast,tail as:
6 |
The basis of the slope analysis method for determining the occurrence of reluctant openings came from the realization that a linear relationship exists between activation (I tail,norm) and facilitation (I peak,norm). This can be shown by combining and , yielding:
7 |
It was gratifying that the plot of experimentally determined I tail,norm as a function of I peak,norm actually formed a straight line (Fig. 6 C), after the anticipated initial deviation during the fast-activating phase of I tail,norm (which is not modeled in ).
The slope of the linear phase of the curve (A slow,tail /A slow,peak) provides the crucial information regarding the prevalence of reluctant openings at V pre. This can be demonstrated by considering what happens to the slope in two polar instances, as simulated in Fig. 6D–G. In either instance, the time course of facilitation, as gauged by the plot of I peak,norm versus prepulse duration (D), will not change from that plotted in Fig. 6 A, because the experimental paradigm is designed to selectively detect willing openings. However, the kinetics of activation, as represented in plots of I tail,norm versus prepulse duration (D), will change dramatically depending on the prevalence of reluctant openings in the prepulse. First, in the case where reluctant openings fail to occur (i.e., when P o′ = 0) (Fig. 6D and Fig. E), term ii in the plot of normalized activation vanishes (Fig. 6 D, and Eq. 5), so that the kinetics of activation at V pre (Fig. 6 D) becomes essentially identical to that for facilitation (Fig. 6 A). Hence, in the absence of reluctant openings, the linear phase in the plot of I tail,norm versus I peak,norm will fall on the line of identity, with a slope of unity (Fig. 6 E). This outcome can be verified explicitly by inspection of and , which reveals that if P o′ = 0 ():
8 |
so that the slope of the plot of I tail,norm versus I peak,norm becomes unity. Furthermore, inspection of and 5 reveals that when P o′ = 0:
9 |
such that the y intercept term in now vanishes. At the other extreme, if reluctant channels open just as well as willing ones (i.e., if P o′ = P o) (Fig. 6F and Fig. G), then A slow,tail = 0 (), so that term iii in Fig. 6 F vanishes. In this circumstance, the linear phase of the plot of I tail,norm versus I peak,norm will be a flat line, with a value of unity and a slope of zero (Fig. 6 G).
Overall, the key insight from these equations is this: the presence of reluctant openings, where 0 < P o′ < P o, will result in slope values between 0 and 1, with lower slope values corresponding to greater occurrence of reluctant openings. The slope analysis for the actual N-type channel data in Fig. 6 C yields a slope of 0.49, indicating substantial occurrence of reluctant openings. In fact, the slope can be converted into an even more direct measure of the relative prevalence of reluctant openings, under the condition that the steady state fraction of reluctant channels F Rmin(V pre) = 0. In this case, , , and can be simplified to:
10 |
This remarkable result should hold for voltages ≥10 mV, where the convergence of inhibited and uninhibited traces by the end of 300-ms pulses (Fig. 2 and Fig. 3) indicates that F Rmin ∼ 0. Application of this relation to the slope analysis of actual N-type channel data (Fig. 6 C) yields a relative open probability for reluctant channels of P o′/P o = 1 − 0.49 = 0.51 at +100 mV. Slope analysis may be applied to experiments with different prepulse voltages, enabling us to determine the propensity for reluctant channels to open at a variety of voltages.
Voltage Dependence of Reluctant Openings in N- and P/Q-type Channels
Fig. 7 summarizes the application of slope analysis to determine the occurrence and voltage dependence of reluctant openings in G-protein–inhibited N-type channels. Each of three rows corresponds to a different prepulse voltage (+50 mV in Fig. 7, A–C; +100 mV in D–F; and +150 mV in G–I). In all three instances, the occurrence of reluctant openings could be qualitatively inferred from the clear deviation of I tail,norm − I peak,norm relations from the line of identity (Fig. 7C, Fig. F, and Fig. I), with slope values of 0.7 (+50 mV, ○), 0.48 (+100 mV, ▵) and 0.32 (+150 mV, □). The population data shown below in Fig. 9 A (○) entirely confirmed these findings, with increasing voltages producing progressively more reluctant openings, and the relative open probability of reluctant channels reaching >50% at saturating depolarization. Interestingly, reluctant openings appeared to be appreciable at voltages as low as +25 mV. This would place them in a range where they could potentially contribute to physiological Ca2+ influx, as neuronal action potentials reach potentials as high as 30–35 mV (Borst et al. 1995).
In sharp contrast, slope analysis of P/Q-type channels revealed an entirely different picture. Fig. 8 summarizes the results, using a format identical to that applied to N-type channels (Fig. 7). The absence of reluctant openings can be qualitatively appreciated from the almost complete concurrence of slow activation and facilitation at the various prepulse voltages (Fig. 8B, Fig. E, and Fig. H), as well as from I tail,norm − I peak,norm relations that closely hugged the line of identity (C, F, and I). Data collected over a range of prepulse voltages in multiple cells confirmed the virtual absence of reluctant openings in P/Q-type channels (Fig. 9 A, ▪). No reluctant openings could be detected at voltages up to +50 mV, and only a hint of their existence starts to appear at voltages of +100 mV or greater. Thus, in contrast to N-type channels, reluctant P/Q-type channels seem unlikely to contribute to Ca2+ influx during physiological action potentials.
It has been noted previously (Elmslie et al. 1990; Boland and Bean 1993) that in G-protein–inhibited N-type channels, tail currents recorded after a brief (1–2 ms) large depolarizing prepulse deactivate faster than those recorded after a longer duration prepulse (during which appreciable facilitation occurs). This result has been interpreted as evidence that G-protein–inhibited N-type channels could open to a reluctant open state. As a final check on the suggestion that reluctant openings occur readily in N-type, but not P/Q-type channels, we compared the kinetics of tail currents measured at −30 mV after prepulses of 1- or 20-ms duration to +100 mV, all during G-protein inhibition. In agreement with these previous studies, tail currents of inhibited N-type channels recorded after a 20-ms prepulse depolarization, deactivated at a much slower rate compared with tail currents measured after a 1-ms prepulse (Fig. 9 B, left). We further quantified the change in shape by measuring the difference in tail current width at half-maximal amplitude, which increased by 0.73 ± 0.13 ms (n = 3; P < 0.05, two-tailed paired t test). As a control, tail currents recorded from unmodified N channels (2 mM GDPβS included in patch pipette) displayed no appreciable lengthening, with a half-width difference of 0.11 ± 0.10 ms (n = 3; N.S., P > 0.3, two-tailed paired t test). By contrast to inhibited N channels, tail currents from G-protein–modified P/Q channels deactivated at similar rates, irrespective of whether they were recorded after prepulse durations of 1 or 20 ms (Fig. 9 B, right; half-width difference of 0.04 ± 0.02 ms, n = 3; NS, P > 0.1, two-tailed paired t test). Thus, these tail kinetics data are consistent with the idea of a differential occurrence of reluctant openings in N- and P/Q-type channels, and therefore provide an important verification of the slope analysis method.
We considered two potential sources of artifacts that could potentially confound the interpretation of the data obtained using slope analysis. First, the faster deactivation of P/Q channels compared with N channels could induce an error if the voltage clamp was not fast enough to resolve the true peak amplitude of the P/Q-channel tail current. We addressed this issue by applying slope analysis to N-channel tail currents recorded at a more hyperpolarized voltage (−50 mV), where the N-channel deactivation rate is similar to P/Q-channel deactivation at −30 mV (data not shown). The slope values obtained from this analysis were identical to those obtained with tail currents recorded at −30 mV, thereby ruling out this potential source of error. Second, the smaller degree of inhibition of P/Q channels raised the possibility that the slope analysis method might not be sensitive enough to detect reluctant openings in P/Q channels, even if they did exist. This possibility is excluded because there are individual cases in which N channels are inhibited to a similar degree as P/Q channels, and slope analysis applied to these individual examples (not shown) resulted in slope values that did not deviate from the average values shown in Fig. 9 A.
Kinetics of Gβγ Interaction with N-and P/Q-type Channels
What could be the molecular mechanism underlying this difference in reluctant openings between N- and P/Q-type channels? One hypothesis is that Gβγ dissociates more quickly from P/Q- than from N-type channels (as opposed to actual differences in the activation pathways). In this case, P/Q-type channels might be capable of expressing reluctant openings if Gβγ stayed bound long enough for such openings to occur. Since the processes of kinetic slowing and prepulse facilitation are believed to reflect unbinding of Gβγ, we investigated this possibility by comparing the time constants of slow activation (for voltages up to +30 mV) or facilitation (for voltages +50 mV or greater) between inhibited N- and P/Q-type channels (Fig. 10). Time constants for slow activation were obtained from simultaneous fits of inhibited and uninhibited traces (Fig. 10 A) as described in the legend. As there is little steady state inhibition at voltages ≥10 mV (Fig. 2 and Fig. 3), time constants are presumably equivalent to Gβγ dissociation rates over this range of potentials. As postulated, the profile of time constants was strikingly different between the two channel types. For the N-type channel, the time constant was comparatively slow and markedly voltage dependent, dropping precipitously from a value of 125 ± 3.9 ms at −10 mV, to a value of 7.5 ± 1.0 ms at +100 mV (Fig. 10 B). For potentials ≤20 mV, time constants for P/Q-type channels were considerably faster (15.9 ± 2.9 ms at −10 mV) and less voltage dependent than for N-type channels. Contrary to expectations of the simple hypothesis, however, time constants for the two channels converged and were virtually identical at potentials +30 mV or greater (Fig. 10 B), with a P/Q-type channel time constant of 6.1 ± 0.6 ms at + 100 mV. This convergence of time constants discounts differences in the kinetics of Gβγ interaction with channels as a possible explanation for the differential occurrence of reluctant openings, since it is precisely at these voltages (+30 mV or greater) that reluctant openings are preferentially detected in N-type channels by the slope analysis method. Hence, under the assumption that slow activation and prepulse facilitation reflect Gβγ unbinding from the channel, the differential occurrence of reluctant openings is likely to reflect a genuine difference in the reluctant mode activation pathways of the two channel types (Fig. 10 C), rather than differences in Gβγ dissociation rates.
DISCUSSION
We have performed an in-depth comparison of the G-protein modulation of N- and P/Q-type calcium channels, here reconstituted in HEK 293 cells transfected with recombinant channels and receptors. As previously demonstrated for recombinant N-type channels expressed in this system (Patil et al. 1996), recombinant P/Q-type channels were also subject to a purely voltage-dependent form of G-protein modulation upon activation of transfected m2 muscarinic receptors. Overall, modulation was broadly similar for the two channel types, though N-type channels were inhibited to a greater extent, and exhibited larger prepulse facilitation than P/Q-type channels. The key new findings were revealed by a modified tail G-V experiment and a novel slope-analysis method, both of which indicated a more fundamental qualitative difference in the G-protein modulation of these channels. Whereas N-type channels could open in the reluctant mode of gating at physiologically relevant voltages, P/Q-type channels gave no clear indication of such reluctant openings, particularly in the physiological range of potentials ≤50 mV. In accord with previous terminology (Patil et al. 1996), our results suggest a permissive exchange model for G-protein inhibition of N-type channels, and a preferential exchange model for inhibition of P/Q-type channels. Finally, the differential occurrence of reluctant openings does not appear to result from distinctions in the rates of Gβγ unbinding, but probably reflects intrinsic differences in the activation pathways of the two channel types.
Here we explore both biophysical and physiological dimensions of the results. First, we consider the implications for basic mechanisms of G-protein inhibition of channels. Second, we critique the data supporting differences in the prevalence of reluctant openings between channel types. Finally, we elaborate on the physiological impact of differential modulation of N- versus P/Q-type channels.
Mechanisms Underlying Differential N- and P/Q-type Channel Modulation
We have argued that the sharp contrast in the prevalence of reluctant openings between N- and P/Q-type channels arises from differences in reluctant mode activation pathways, rather than from differences in the rates of Gβγ dissociation from channels. We inferred that such dissociation rates were similar for voltages ≥30 mV, based on the similarity of time constants for equilibration between willing and reluctant modes of gating (Fig. 10 B). Because the two channel types manifest a clear distinction in the prevalence of reluctant openings over these voltages (Fig. 9), we concluded that this difference was unlikely to arise from differing Gβγ dissociation rates.
It would be reassuring to our conclusion if others had observed the same voltage-dependent pattern of time constants shown in Fig. 10 B. In accord with our results, previous experiments with neurons and heterologous expression systems indicate that facilitation of N- and P/Q-type channels proceeds at similar rates for prepulse voltages ≥100 mV (Currie and Fox 1997; Meza and Adams 1998; Roche and Treistman 1998). By contrast, another study comparing G-protein inhibition of N-type (α1B) and P/Q-type (α1A) channels expressed in Xenopus oocytes found that the kinetics of facilitation were consistently about twofold faster for α1A than for α1B over a similar range of prepulse potentials (+60 and 150 mV; Zhang et al. 1996). At more modest depolarizations, the time constant of intermodal exchange has rarely been investigated, in part because of the overlap between the kinetics of slow activation and voltage-dependent inactivation. Here, the use of the calcium-channel β2a subunit dramatically slowed voltage-dependent inactivation (e.g., Jones et al. 1998; Patil et al. 1998), thus facilitating well-resolved exponential fits to the slow phase of current activation at modest voltages. This strategy revealed that over a broad range of more negative voltages, the kinetics of reluctant → willing mode conversion was considerably faster (approximately eightfold faster at −10 mV) for P/Q- compared with N-type channels. Because of the markedly greater voltage dependence of such kinetics in the N-type channel, the time constants for the two channel types converged at more positive voltages. The strong voltage dependence of N-type channel kinetics over a range of modest potentials is in overall agreement with previous analysis in NG108-15 cells (Kasai 1992). In short, previous results generally support the profile of time constants observed in Fig. 10 B, although this study is the first to explicitly compare the kinetics of N- and P/Q-type channels over such a broad voltage range.
The configuration of time constants for intermodal exchange (Fig. 10 B) bears not only upon potential differences in the activation pathways of N- and P/Q-type channels, but also upon a possible explanation for the lower degree of G-protein inhibition of P/Q-type channels (Mintz and Bean 1993; Bayliss et al. 1995; Bourinet et al. 1996; Zhang et al. 1996; Currie and Fox 1997; Roche and Treistman 1998). The clearly faster time constants for P/Q-type channel facilitation at modest depolarizations point to more rapid Gβγ dissociation, which would result in weaker steady state inhibition, assuming equal association rates between channel types. Zhang et al. 1996 were the first to advance this explanation for the weaker inhibition of P/Q-type channels, but others argued against it based on the similarity of facilitation kinetics at voltages ≥100 mV (Currie and Fox 1997; Meza and Adams 1998; Roche and Treistman 1998). Our observations reconcile the differing perspectives, and support the explanation of Zhang et al. 1996 at moderate potentials with relevance to physiological behavior.
Authenticity of Suggested Differential Prevalence of Reluctant Openings in N- and P/Q-type Channels
A major outcome of this study is the suggestion that the N- but not P/Q-type channel can open in the reluctant mode of gating (reluctant openings). Although the tail G-V experiment and slope-analysis method provide arguably compelling support for this suggestion, the evidence is nevertheless indirect and dependent on several assumptions. We review these assumptions, so as to gauge the strength of our suggestion and to facilitate comparison with future, more direct experiments.
Regarding the tail G-V experiments (Fig. 4), we mentioned that mechanistic inferences from the 3-ms activation protocol were dependent upon the extent to which intermodal exchange was minimized by the short duration of depolarizing steps. This assumption appears reasonable given that time constants for reluctant → willing mode conversion were at least three- to fourfold longer for voltages up to +50 mV (Fig. 10 B). The assumptions underlying the slope-analysis method are more subtle. First, we postulated that no reluctant openings occurred during the modest 10-mV depolarization in the test pulse (e.g., Fig. 5 A). If this were not true, then the expression for A slow,peak, currently given in , should be revised to the form currently applied to A slow,tail (), except that all P o′(V pre) terms should be substituted with P o′(Vtest), where Vtest is the potential during the test pulse. The net result is that the slope we measure (= A slow,tail ÷ modified A slow,peak) would underestimate the true slope in the absence of reluctant openings during the test pulse (= A slow,tail ÷ true A slow,peak). This would lead to an underestimate in the relative open probability of reluctant channels [P o′(V pre)/P o(V pre)]. If such underestimates are present to a greater degree in P/Q- versus N-type channels, the inferred difference in the prevalence of reluctant openings could be diminished. However, the results of the tail G-V experiments would still support a strong difference in the prevalence of reluctant openings between channel types. Second, the equations describing activation presume that if reluctant channels can open during a prepulse, their conditional probability of occurrence, given occupancy within the reluctant mode, has reached steady state during the course of the slow phase of activation. If this presumption was false, then the time constants for the slow phases of activation and facilitation would be expected to differ, resulting in nonlinearity that was not apparent in experimentally determined I tail,norm − I peak,norm relations (e.g., Fig. 6 C). Finally, the equations describing activation do not consider the possibility that reluctant openings may manifest a smaller unitary conductance (Kuo and Bean 1993), although fragmentary single-channel experiments have so far failed to confirm such a difference in unitary conductance (Patil et al. 1996).
While the assumptions underlying our whole-cell analysis seem quite reasonable, the present experiments urgently underscore the need for single-channel experiments to directly establish the core dichotomy in the expression of reluctant openings. In two previous single-channel studies of N-type channels, the major effect of G-protein activation was to delay the time to first opening (Carabelli et al. 1996; Patil et al. 1996), without obvious evidence of reluctant openings. One preliminary study of single N-type channels (Lee and Elmslie 1997) did report gating consistent with reluctant opening. No comparable studies of single P/Q-type channels have appeared. One drawback of most of these studies, however, is that due to the surface charge screening effects of the high Ba2+ concentrations used, the voltages at which G-protein effects were studied corresponded to physiological voltages <0 mV. The voltage dependence of reluctant openings in inhibited N-type channels, as presented here (Fig. 9), makes it clear that detection of single N-type channel reluctant openings would not be expected to occur under the conditions used in the two full-length studies above. As unitary current amplitude diminishes with increasing depolarization, it will be challenging to resolve single-channel events at the positive potentials where appreciable reluctant openings would be predicted. We are, however, encouraged by our preliminary single-channel evidence that reluctant openings can be detected in G-protein–inhibited N-type channels at higher voltages, using quartz pipette technology to aid resolution (Levis and Rae 1998; Colecraft et al. 1999).
Physiological Implications of Differential Occurrence of Reluctant Openings
N- and P/Q-type channels have been immunocytochemically identified within central and peripheral neurons, and the channels show distinct subcellular locations in dendritic spines, cell bodies, and nerve terminals (Westenbroek et al. 1992, Westenbroek et al. 1998). Using selective toxin blockade of distinct types of calcium channels, complementary studies have focused on the functional contribution of different calcium channels to synaptic transmission at a variety of synapses. Both N-and P/Q-type channels are found to contribute to synaptic transmission, albeit to different degrees in different synapses (Luebke et al. 1993; Takahashi and Momiyama 1993; Wheeler et al. 1994; Wu and Saggau 1994; Reid et al. 1998). Some neurons even show the extreme case, where synaptic transmission is entirely mediated by either N- or P/Q-type channels (Poncer et al. 1997). Against this background of variably distributed N- and P/Q-type channels, differential modulation would provide a means to fine tune synaptic transmission tailored to the needs of spatially distinct synapses.
The selective occurrence of reluctant openings at physiologically relevant voltages in N- but not P/Q-type channels represents a new dimension of differential modulation that could contribute to such functional tuning. The presence or absence of reluctant openings during G-protein inhibition could affect the waveform of Ca2+ entry during action potentials in fundamentally different ways (Brody et al. 1997). If reluctant openings are not evoked during an action potential, then G-protein inhibition would simply limit the number of willing channels. In this case, the inhibited waveform of Ca2+ entry would preserve its shape, and represent a scaled-down version of the control waveform. By contrast, if both reluctant and willing openings are evoked during an action potential, then the inhibited waveform would reflect the properties of gating transitions surrounding both willing and reluctant open states. One might then predict that the waveform of Ca2+ entry during G-protein inhibition would not only be smaller, but altered in shape. Changes in the shape of the Ca2+ entry waveform could have substantial physiological consequences for the timing of synaptic transmission (Sabatini and Regehr 1999). Furthermore, Ca2+ influx through a single channel may trigger vesicle release under some conditions (Augustine et al. 1991; Stanley 1993; Bertram et al. 1996), while multiple channels may contribute in others (Borst and Sakmann 1998). Alterations in the amplitude and/or shape of the macroscopic Ca2+ entry waveform, as well as differences in single-channel open and closed times, may tilt the balance between single- and overlapping-channel regimes of neurotransmitter release, with attendant modulation of synaptic efficacy.
Given the potential functional impact of reluctant openings, it will be critical to determine whether they actually do occur in N-type channels driven by physiological action potentials. Although the present results suggest that such openings occur at physiologically relevant potentials, it remains to be shown whether they can activate fast enough to occur during the brief 1–4-ms duration of action potentials. The outcome will be critically dependent on the first latency of inhibited channels. Future work using action potential waveforms (Brody et al. 1997) will be essential in answering the remaining unknown.
The full extent of the physiological impact of differential G-protein modulation may extend well beyond the simple two-channel dichotomy presented here. For simplicity, we have used only the β2a auxiliary subunit, although there are at least four types of β subunits with multiple splice variants (Perez-Reyes et al. 1992). Different β subunits may impart varying propensities for reluctant opening. Moreover, different splice variants of the various α1A and α1B backbones for N- and P/Q-type channels may also figure in differential G-protein modulation (Lin et al. 1997). For example, an alternatively spliced variant of the P/Q-type (α1A) subunit, termed α1A-b, has been identified. This splice variant differs from the primary sequence of the α1A subunit used here at three sites, one of which is the presence of a single valine insertion in the I–II linker of the new clone (Bourinet et al. 1999). Of particular relevance was the finding that the presence of this valine resulted in currents that were more strongly modulated by G proteins, reminiscent of N-type channel behavior. It will be interesting to determine whether reluctant openings may occur differentially between alternatively spliced variants of the P/Q-type channel.
Acknowledgments
We thank SIBIA Neurosciences for the human α1B-1 clone, T.P. Snutch for the α1A-a and α2bδ clones, E. Perez-Reyes for the β2a clone, Ernst Peralta for the m2 clone, and J.G Mulle and Devi Rathod for technical assistance.
This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (D.T. Yue) and an NIH Medical Scientist Training Program Award (P.G. Patil).
Footnotes
Abbreviations used in this paper: CCh, carbachol; Gβγ, G-protein βγ subunits.
References
- Augustine G.J., Adler E.M., Charlton M.P. The calcium signal for transmitter secretion from presynaptic nerve terminals. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1991;635:365–381. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1991.tb36505.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayliss D.A., Umemiya M., Berger A.J. Inhibition of N- and P-type calcium currents and the after-hyperpolarization in rat motoneurones by serotonin. J. Physiol. 1995;485:635–647. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bean B.P. Neurotransmitter inhibition of neuronal calcium currents by changes in channel voltage dependence. Nature. 1989;340:153–156. doi: 10.1038/340153a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertram R., Sherman A., Stanley E.F. Single-domain/bound calcium hypothesis of transmitter release and facilitation. J. Neurophysiol. 1996;75:1919–1931. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.5.1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bito H., Deisseroth K., Tsien R.W. Ca2+-dependent regulation in neuronal gene expression. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1997;7:419–429. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(97)80072-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boland L.M., Bean B.P. Modulation of N-type calcium channels in bullfrog sympathetic neurons by luteinizing hormone-releasing hormonekinetics and voltage dependence. J. Neurosci. 1993;13:516–533. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-02-00516.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borst J.G., Sakmann B. Calcium current during a single action potential in a large presynaptic terminal of the rat brainstem. J. Physiol. 1998;506:143–157. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.143bx.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borst J.G.G., Helmchen F., Sakmann B. Pre- and postsynaptic whole-cell recordings in the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body of the rat. J. Physiol. 1995;489:825–840. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp021095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourinet E., Soong T.W., Stea A., Snutch T.P. Determinants of the G protein–dependent opioid modulation of neuronal calcium channels. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1996;93:1486–1491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.4.1486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourinet E., Soong T.W., Sutton K., Slaymaker S., Mathews E., Monteil A., Zamponi G.W., Nargeot J., Snutch T.P. Splicing of alpha 1A subunit gene generates phenotypic variants of P- and Q-type calcium channels. Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2:407–415. doi: 10.1038/8070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody D.L., Patil P.G., Mulle J.G., Snutch T.P., Yue D.T. Bursts of action potential waveforms relieve G-protein inhibition of recombinant P/Q-type Ca2+ channels in HEK 293 cells. J. Physiol. 1997;499:637–644. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1997.sp021956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody D.L., Yue D.T. Evidence that relief of G-protein inhibition of neuronal calcium channels contributes to short-term synaptic plasticity Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 25 1999. 802(Abstr.) [Google Scholar]
- Canti C., Page K.M., Stephens G.J., Dolphin A.C. Identification of residues in the N terminus of alpha1B critical for inhibition of the voltage-dependent calcium channel by Gbeta gamma. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:6855–6864. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-16-06855.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carabelli V., Lovallo M., Magnelli V., Zucker H., Carbone E. Voltage-dependent modulation of single N-Type Ca2+ channel kinetics by receptor agonists in IMR32 cells. Biophys. J. 1996;70:2144–2154. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(96)79780-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colecraft H.M., Brody D.L., Yue D.T. Single-channel resolution of reluctant openings in G-protein–inhibited Ca2+ channels Biophys. J. 76 1999. A258(Abstr.) [Google Scholar]
- Currie K.P., Fox A.P. Comparison of N- and P/Q-type voltage-gated calcium channel current inhibition. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:4570–4579. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-12-04570.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Waard M., Liu H., Walker D., Scott V.E., Gurnett C.A., Campbell K.P. Direct binding of G-protein betagamma complex to voltage-dependent calcium channels. Nature. 1997;385:446–450. doi: 10.1038/385446a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dittman J.S., Regehr W.G. Contributions of calcium-dependent and calcium-independent mechanisms to presynaptic inhibition at a cerebellar synapse. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:1623–1633. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-05-01623.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elmslie K.S., Zhou W., Jones S.W. LHRH and GTP-gamma-S modify calcium current activation in bullfrog sympathetic neurons. Neuron. 1990;5:75–80. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(90)90035-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forscher P., Oxford G. Modulation of calcium channels by norepinephrine in internally dialyzed avian sensory neurons. J. Gen. Physiol. 1985;85:743–763. doi: 10.1085/jgp.85.5.743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furukawa T., Miura R., Mori Y., Strobeck M., Suzuki K., Ogihara Y., Asano T., Morishita R., Hashii M., Higashida H. Differential interactions of the C terminus and the cytoplasmic I-II loop of neuronal Ca2+ channels with G-protein alpha and beta gamma subunits. II. Evidence for direct binding J. Biol. Chem. 273 1998. 17595 17603a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furukawa T., Nukada T., Mori Y., Wakamori M., Fujita Y., Ishida H., Fukuda K., Kato S., Yoshii M. Differential interactions of the C terminus and the cytoplasmic I-II loop of neuronal Ca2+ channels with G-protein alpha and beta gamma subunits. I. Molecular determination J. Biol. Chem. 273 1998. 17585 17594b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grassi F., Lux H.D. Voltage-dependent GABA-induced modulation of calcium currents in chick sensory neurons. Neurosci. Lett. 1989;105:113–119. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(89)90021-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herlitze S., Garcia D.E., Mackie K., Hille B., Catterall W.A. Modulation of Ca2+ channels by G-protein beta-gamma subunits. Nature. 1996;380:258–262. doi: 10.1038/380258a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hille B. G protein–coupled mechanisms and nervous signaling. Neuron. 1992;9:187–195. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90158-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hille B. Modulation of ion-channel function by G-protein–coupled receptors. Trends Neurosci. 1994;17:531–536. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90157-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikeda S.R. Voltage-dependent modulation of N-type calcium channels by G-protein beta-gamma subunits. Nature. 1996;380:255–258. doi: 10.1038/380255a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikeda S.R., Schofield G.G. Somatostatin blocks a calcium current in rat sympathetic ganglion neurones. J. Physiol. 1989;409:221–240. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones L.P., Patil P.G., Snutch T.P., Yue D.T. G-protein modulation of N-type calcium channel gating current in human embryonic kidney cells (HEK 293) J. Physiol. 1997;498:601–610. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1997.sp021886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones L.P., Wei S.K., Yue D.T. Mechanism of auxiliary subunit modulation of neuronal α1E calcium channels. J. Gen. Physiol. 1998;112:125–143. doi: 10.1085/jgp.112.2.125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones S.W., Elmslie K.S. Transmitter modulation of neuronal calcium channels. J. Membr. Biol. 1997;155:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s002329900153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasai H. Voltage- and time-dependent inhibition of neuronal calcium channels by a GTP-binding protein in a mammalian cell line. J. Physiol. 1992;448:189–209. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1992.sp019036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo C.C., Bean B. G-protein modulation of ion permeation through N-type calcium channels. Nature. 1993;365:258–262. doi: 10.1038/365258a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee H.K., Elmslie K.S. Characterization of N-channel gating recorded in the presence of norepinephrine Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 23 1997. 856(Abstr.) [Google Scholar]
- Levis R.A., Rae J.L. Low-noise patch-clamp techniques. Methods Enzymol. 1998;293:218–266. doi: 10.1016/s0076-6879(98)93017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin Z., Haus S., Edgerton J., Lipscombe D. Identification of functionally distinct isoforms of the N-type Ca2+ channel in rat sympathetic ganglia and brain. Neuron. 1997;18:153–166. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)80054-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipscombe D., Kongsamut S., Tsien R. Alpha-adrenergic inhibition of sympathetic neurotransmitter release mediated by modulation of N-type calcium-channel gating. Nature. 1989;340:639–642. doi: 10.1038/340639a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luebke J.I., Dunlap K. Sensory neuron N-type calcium currents are inhibited by both voltage-dependent and -independent mechanisms. Pflügers Arch. 1994;428:499–507. doi: 10.1007/BF00374571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luebke J.I., Dunlap K., Turner T. Multiple calcium channel types control glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the hippocampus. Neuron. 1993;11:895–902. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90119-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marchetti C., Carbone E., Lux H.D. Effects of dopamine and noradrenaline on Ca channels of cultured sensory and sympathetic neurons of chick. Pflügers Arch. 1986;406:104–111. doi: 10.1007/BF00586670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meza U., Adams B. G-protein–dependent facilitation of neuronal alpha1A, alpha1B, and alpha1E Ca channels. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:5240–5252. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-14-05240.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller R.J. Receptor-mediated regulation of calcium channels and neurotransmitter release. FASEB J. 1990;4:3291–3299. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mintz I.M., Bean B.P. GABAB receptor inhibition of P-type Ca2+ channels in central neurons. Neuron. 1993;10:889–898. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90204-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patil P.G., Brody D.L., Yue D.T. Preferential closed-state inactivation of neuronal calcium channels. Neuron. 1998;20:1027–1038. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80483-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patil P.G., de Leon M., Reed R.R., Dubel S., Snutch T.P., Yue D.T. Elementary events underlying voltage-dependent G-protein inhibition of N-type calcium channels. Biophys. J. 1996;71:2509–2521. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(96)79444-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peralta E.G., Ashkenazi A., Winslow J.W., Smith D.H., Ramachandran J., Capon D.J. Distinct primary structures, ligand-binding properties and tissue-specific expression of four human muscarinic acetylcholine receptors. EMBO (Eur. Mol. Biol. Organ.) J. 1987;6:3923–3929. doi: 10.1002/j.1460-2075.1987.tb02733.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perez-Reyes E., Castellano A., Kim H.S., Bertrand P., Baggstrom E., Lacerda A.E., Wei X.Y., Birnbaumer L. Cloning and expression of a cardiac/brain beta subunit of the L-type calcium channel. J. Biol. Chem. 1992;267:1792–1797. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poncer J.C., McKinney R.A., Gahwiler B.H., Thompson S.M. Either N- or P-type calcium channels mediate GABA release at distinct hippocampal inhibitory synapses. Neuron. 1997;18:463–472. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81246-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin N., Platano D., Olcese R., Stefani E., Birnbaumer L. Direct interaction of Gβγ with a C-terminal Gβγ-binding domain of the Ca2+ channel alpha1 subunit is responsible for channel inhibition by G protein–coupled receptors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1997;94:8866–8871. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.16.8866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid C.A., Bekkers J.M., Clements J.D. N- and P/Q-type Ca2+ channels mediate transmitter release with a similar cooperativity at rat hippocampal autapses. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:2849–2855. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-08-02849.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roche J.P., Treistman S.N. The Ca2+ channel beta3 subunit differentially modulates G-protein sensitivity of alpha1A and alpha1B Ca2+ channels. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:878–886. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-03-00878.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabatini B.L., Regehr W.G. Timing of synaptic transmission. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 1999;61:521–542. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.61.1.521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley E.F. Single calcium channels and acetylcholine release at a presynaptic nerve terminal. Neuron. 1993;11:1007–1011. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90214-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stea A., Tomlinson W.J., Soong T.W., Bourinet E., Dubel S.J., Vincent S.R., Snutch T.P. Localization and functional properties of a rat brain alpha 1A calcium channel reflect similarities to neuronal Q- and P-type channels. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1994;91:10576–10580. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.22.10576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi T., Momiyama A. Different types of calcium channels mediate central synaptic transmission. Nature. 1993;366:156–158. doi: 10.1038/366156a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomlinson W.J., Stea A., Bourinet E., Charnet P., Nargeot J., Snutch T.P. Functional properties of a neuronal class C L-type calcium channel. Neuropharmacology. 1993;32:1117–1126. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(93)90006-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsien R.W., Tsien R.Y. Calcium channels, stores, and oscillations. Annu. Rev. Cell Biol. 1990;6:715–760. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cb.06.110190.003435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wanke E., Ferroni A., Malgaroli A., Ambrosini A., Pozzan T., Meldolesi J. Activation of a muscarinic receptor selectively inhibits a rapidly inactivated Ca2+ current in rat sympathetic neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1987;84:4313–4317. doi: 10.1073/pnas.84.12.4313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westenbroek R.E., Hell J.W., Warner C., Dubel S.J., Snutch T.P., Catterall W.A. Biochemical properties and subcellular distribution of an N-type calcium channel alpha 1 subunit. Neuron. 1992;9:1099–1115. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90069-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westenbroek R.E., Hoskins L., Catterall W.A. Localization of Ca2+ channel subtypes on rat spinal motor neurons, interneurons, and nerve terminals. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:6319–6330. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-16-06319.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheeler D.B., Randall A., Tsien R.W. Roles of N-type and Q-type Ca2+ channels in supporting hippocampal synaptic transmission. Science. 1994;264:107–111. doi: 10.1126/science.7832825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams M.E., Brust P.F., Feldman D.H., Patthi S., Simerson S., Maroufi A., McCue A.F., Velicelebi G., Ellis S.B., Harpold M.M. Structure and functional expression of an omega-conotoxin-sensitive human N-type calcium channel. Science. 1992;257:389–395. doi: 10.1126/science.1321501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu L.G., Saggau P. Adenosine inhibits evoked synaptic transmission primarily by reducing presynaptic calcium influx in area CA1 of hippocampus. Neuron. 1994;12:1139–1148. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90321-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu L.G., Saggau P. Presynaptic inhibition of elicited neurotransmitter release. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:204–212. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)01015-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zamponi G.W., Bourinet E., Nelson D., Nargeot J., Snutch T.P. Crosstalk between G proteins and protein kinase C mediated by the calcium channel alpha1 subunit. Nature. 1997;385:442–446. doi: 10.1038/385442a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zamponi G.W., Snutch T.P. Decay of prepulse facilitation of N type calcium channels during G protein inhibition is consistent with binding of a single G beta subunit. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1998;95:4035–4039. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.7.4035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang J.-F., Ellinor P.T., Aldrich R.W., Tsien R.W. Multiple structural elements in voltage-dependent Ca2+ channels support their inhibition by G proteins. Neuron. 1996;17:991–1003. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80229-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu Y., Ikeda S.R. VIP inhibits N-type Ca2+ channels of sympathetic neurons via a pertussis toxin–insensitive but cholera toxin–sensitive pathway. Neuron. 1994;13:657–669. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90033-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]