Abstract
The paper sets forth a set of evidence-based recommendations for interventions to combat unintentional bias among health care providers, drawing upon theory and research in social cognitive psychology. Our primary aim is to provide a framework that outlines strategies and skills, which can be taught to medical trainees and practicing physicians, to prevent unconscious racial attitudes and stereotypes from negatively influencing the course and outcomes of clinical encounters. These strategies and skills are designed to: l) enhance internal motivation to reduce bias, while avoiding external pressure; 2) increase understanding about the psychological basis of bias; 3) enhance providers’ confidence in their ability to successfully interact with socially dissimilar patients; 4) enhance emotional regulation skills; and 5) improve the ability to build partnerships with patients. We emphasize the need for programs to provide a nonthreatening environment in which to practice new skills and the need to avoid making providers ashamed of having racial, ethnic, or cultural stereotypes. These recommendations are also intended to provide a springboard for research on interventions to reduce unintentional racial bias in health care.
KEY WORDS: provider behavior, disparities, race, ethnicity, social cognition
Despite ample evidence of racial and ethnic disparities in health care1,2 progress in correcting these inequities remains elusive.1–3 Provider behavior has been identified as 1 important contributor to disparities in health care. There is significant evidence that (1) health care providers hold stereotypes—based on patient race, class, sex, and other characteristics4–17—that influence their interpretation of behaviors and symptoms, and their clinical decisions17–25; (2) application of such stereotypes frequently occurs outside conscious awareness26–31; and (3) providers interact less effectively with minority than with white patients.32–37 In response to this evidence, significant resources have been devoted to programs to prepare providers to better care for patients from diverse backgrounds. These programs, however, typically focus on improving providers’ cross-cultural communication skills and, as such, are likely to have only limited effects on the unconscious cognitive processes that result in stereotype activation and application. In fact, there has been relatively little discussion to date of how to mitigate the negative impact of unconscious racial stereotyping among health care providers, despite the acknowledged need to do so.32–35 This paper is intended to address this gap by drawing from a highly developed body of research from social cognitive psychology to recommend a set of evidence-based intervention strategies.
First, it is important to note that overt expressions of prejudice and negative racial stereotypes have declined substantially over time, as norms condemning bias and endorsing racial equality have become stronger and more widespread across the population.38,39 However, even consciously egalitarian people may hold negative ethnic and racial stereotypes and attitudes,26–31 of which they may not be fully conscious. For example, Whites tend to have unconscious stereotypes of Whites as intelligent, successful, and educated, and of Blacks as aggressive, impulsive, and lazy,40 and Whites spontaneously associate different health conditions with Whites and Blacks. 41 The consequences of this unintentional bias ultimately may be as adverse as more overt biases.42–44 Furthermore, as unconscious biases are “habits of mind”,31,45 learned over time through repeated personal experiences and cultural socialization, they are highly resistant to change.46 Given the realities of medical training and continuing education, our recommended strategies are not intended to change these associations directly,47 but rather to give providers strategies and skills to prevent unconscious attitudes and stereotypes from influencing the course and outcomes of clinical encounters in negative ways. It is important to note that these proposed strategies have not yet been tested in the realm of health care and should undergo rigorous evaluation before widespread adoption.
COUNTERACTING UNCONSCIOUS PREJUDICE AND STEREOTYPES THROUGH INDIVIDUATION
Promising evidence in social cognitive psychology indicates that with sufficient motivation, cognitive resources, and effort, people are able to focus on the unique qualities of individuals, rather than on the groups they belong to, in forming impressions and behaving toward others.48,49 Even automatically activated prejudice and stereotypes can be inhibited when people are perceived more in terms of their particular qualities than primarily as members of social categories.50 Interventions to reduce bias in provider judgment, behavior, and decision making should therefore promote the cognitive strategy of individuation, in which the provider focuses on the individual attributes of a particular patient, as opposed to categorization, in which the provider perceives the patient through the filter of his or her group membership (e.g., race). Based on this premise, we outline the elements of a training program that builds upon psychological research on unconscious prejudice and stereotypes. Our recommendations are rooted in an evidence-based conceptual model, depicted in Figure 1. It recognizes the importance of motivation, information, and skills, which have been identified as key ingredients for successful interventions in other domains,51 but also considers the importance of emotion in interracial interaction.
Enhance Internal Motivation and Avoid External Pressure to Reduce Bias
As we have discussed earlier, much of contemporary racism occurs unintentionally, fueled by unconscious prejudice and stereotypes. To the extent that many people are unaware of their biases, there is little motivation for change. However, for people who believe that they are unbiased and aspire to be nonprejudiced, it may be possible to capitalize on their good intentions to motivate efforts to reduce their unconscious biases once they become aware of them. Techniques that lead people to recognize their unconscious biases include exercises where they are prompted to compare what they would do and what they should do in a variety of intergroup circumstances,45 and recently developed procedures (such as the Implicit Association Test52,53) that can reveal unconscious prejudice and stereotypes. These procedures can engender negative emotional states54,55 that motivate people to become more sensitive to and attempt to counteract the effects of unconscious prejudice and stereotypes. Promoting awareness of the social and historical context of race, the evidence documenting racial disparities in the quality of health care,2 and the evidence that provider bias may contribute to those disparities15 may also help in nurturing internal motivation to reduce bias, based on a sense of fairness.
A critical element of this process is self-discovery within a nonthreatening, private context. Anticipated public censure can have paradoxical effects.57,58 Efforts to reduce racial/ethnic disparities should avoid imposing a “politically correct” agenda, but instead appeal to providers’ desire to provide the best possible care to all patients.
Enhance Understanding of the Psychological Basis of Bias
In contrast to the traditional psychological perspective that “pathologized” prejudice and stereotyping, current understanding posits that the cognitive strategy of categorization that gives rise to stereotyping and racial prejudice is a normal aspect of human cognition. Helping providers understand this may allow them to approach their own potential biases in a more informed and open way. This can be accomplished with selected readings, demonstrations of unconscious stereotyping using web-based tools, and guided discussion.59,60
One direct consequence of this approach of openly acknowledging stereotypes is that it can facilitate sensitivity to the negative impact of unconscious prejudice and stereotypes, whereas denial of their existence and direct attempts to suppress their activation can have further negative consequences. Stereotype suppression (e.g., trying to push away any stereotypic thoughts that arise while in the presence of an African-American patient) can lead to a number of negative, unintended consequences. Experiments testing the impact of instructions to avoid stereotyping have shown very short-term benefit with a “rebound effect,” in which the stereotype later recurred at a higher rate than among the control groups who were not instructed to suppress.61–64 Stereotype suppression also can result in increased social distancing, thus potentially undermining the interpersonal quality of the encounter.61 In addition, suppressing stereotypes requires effort,65 which can deplete cognitive resources and adversely affect the ability to solve problems and make correct decisions.66 It is therefore important to reinforce that stereotypes—even negative racial stereotypes—are a natural phenomenon in our society, and that it is better to recognize and use strategies (discussed below) to counteract them, rather than to try to actively suppress them.
Enhance Providers’ Confidence in their Ability to Successfully Interact with Socially Dissimilar Patients
Psychological research has shown that Whites often feel anxious when interacting with Blacks, because of a lack of positive experiences with interracial encounters, leading them to avoid such interactions.67 In the context of the clinical encounter, this may translate into White providers’ engaging in avoidance behaviors and spending less time with non-White patients, leading to poorer patient–provider relationships. Non-White patients, who may be particularly vigilant for signs of prejudice or rejection,68 may interpret signs of anxiety displayed by White providers as reflecting negative attitudes.69 Nonverbal behaviors associated with anxiousness overlap considerably with cues of dislike.70
The most successful way to alleviate intergroup anxiety and increase provider confidence is through direct “contact” with members of other groups.71,72 Thus interactive, facilitated discussions, particularly in which people interact in individualized ways, among colleagues of different race and ethnicity may be one method to enhance providers’ confidence in interracial interactions. Programs designed to enhance providers’ overall communication skills73 may also improve interracial clinical encounters, although this hypothesis has not been tested.
Enhance Emotional Regulation Skills Specific to Promoting Positive Emotions
Recent research suggests that providers who experience higher levels of positive emotion during clinical encounters may be less likely to categorize patients in terms of their racial, ethnic, or cultural group and more likely to view patients in terms of their individual attributes (i.e., individuation). For instance, in several studies, White subjects in whom positive emotions were induced more accurately recognized Black faces, suggesting that positive emotional states can lead to lower levels of racial categorization.74 Positive emotion has also been shown to lead to the use of more inclusive social categories, so that people are more likely to view themselves as being part of a larger group,28,75 which can facilitate empathy and increase the capacity to see others as members of a common “ingroup,” as opposed to “outgroup.”43 More generally, positive emotional states have been shown to broaden the scope of attention and the tendency to attend to new information,76 a mental state that is consistent with greater levels of individuation and lower levels of categorization.
Clearly, an individual’s emotional states vary in ways that are not always easy to regulate. However, simply being aware that stress and negative emotions may increase stereotyping could make providers more vigilant about when their interactions with patients are likely to be biased. Additionally, when time and circumstances allow, the use of stress-reducing techniques to enhance emotional well-being before patient encounters may help reduce racial bias. Teaching these methods—e.g., mindfulness techniques, meditation, Balint groups—may be an appropriate aspect of training programs that aim to reduce provider bias.
Increase Perspective Taking and Affective Empathy
Enhancing provider empathy is a particularly promising strategy for increasing the effectiveness of patient–provider interactions generally, and especially for interracial interactions. Although the development of empathic skill is a general aspect of medical training, providers may not recognize inherent obstacles to empathizing with patients from a different racial or ethnic group. People tend to have less empathy for others perceived as dissimilar,77 including members of other social categories,67 which typically result in low rapport.29,35,37,69 For instance, one study found that non-Hispanic White psychologists viewing videotapes of an actress playing a patient reported less empathy when the patient was described as Hispanic rather than White.78
Empathy has both cognitive and affective components.79,80 The cognitive component is perspective taking, in which a person is able to consider a situation from the position of another, e.g., imagining themselves in the other’s shoes. Controlled laboratory studies instructing subjects to adopt a perspective associated with another person and his or her situation have been shown to reduce bias toward a range of stigmatized groups,81 including Blacks,82,83 and to inhibit the activation of unconscious stereotypes and prejudices.84 Similarly, one study found that physicians rated as “empathetic/compassionate” by their peers had less stereotypic attitudes toward patients than physicians classified as low on empathy.85
The affective component involves empathic emotions. In practice, perspective taking and affective empathy are closely related: perspective taking arouses affective empathy, and affective empathy facilitates adopting the other’s perspective.79 Both ultimately produce more positive orientations toward the other person and a greater interest in the other’s welfare.86 Many studies have documented the positive effect of provider empathy on patients, which include increased patient satisfaction, adherence, self-efficacy, perceptions of control, less emotional distress, and better outcomes.87–90 Unfortunately, empathy has been shown to decline over the course of medical training91–94 and the course of physicians’ medical careers,85 suggesting that interventions to increase empathy might need to be repeated over time to be effective.
Programs might promote routine use of the types of imagery strategies successfully used in experimental studies, such that providers spend a moment in each encounter “imagining [themselves] living in the shoes of the patient...feeling what he [/she] feels.” As with any behavior, repeated rehearsal can make the behavior routine. There is also some evidence that having providers take on the role of patients in role-play exercises can increase empathy among health care providers. This strategy might be useful in team discussions of difficult patients.95–97
Improve Ability to Build Partnerships with Patients
Partnership building can improve the effectiveness of patient–provider interactions by reframing the interaction as one between collaborating equals, rather than between one high-status person, the provider, and one low-status person, the patient.98 Whether people perceive another as a partner on the “same team” (i.e., as part of the same group, the “ingroup”) or as a member of a different groups (“outgroup”) has profound implications on their reactions to the other.42,98–101 These reactions are so ingrained and fundamental that they occur automatically, without awareness or intention. In the United States, race, ethnicity, and sex represent automatically activated categories that form the basis for ingroup–outgroup differentiation. In general, people retain information in a more detailed fashion, remember more positive information, and are more forgiving of behaviors for ingroup compared to outgroup members.102–104
Perceptions of common ingroup membership also increase psychological bond and feelings of “oneness” that facilitate perspective taking82 and the arousal of empathy in response to their needs or problems.105–107 Creating a partnership, which produces a common group identity, reduces conscious and unconscious racial biases among Whites in interracial interactions.98,108 Partnership-building between provider and patient would not only be expected to produce a more positive and responsive orientation of the provider to the patient, but also a more open and cooperative orientation of the patient to the provider. Developing a common identity through partnership building is conceptually similar to the “finding common ground” component of the patient-centered approach to care.109 Finding common ground requires that two parties reach agreement with regard to the nature of the medical problem and other priorities, the goals of treatment, and the roles of the doctor and patient. Studies of patient-centered care suggest significant benefits in terms of patient satisfaction and medical outcomes.110–114
CONCLUSION
Using lessons from research in social-cognitive psychology, we have made recommendations that we hope will inform and accelerate efforts to reduce unintentional bias among health care providers. We have proposed specific objectives, and have discussed the scientific rationale behind them, as a way of establishing a foundation on which current and future interventions to reduce provider racial bias might build. Those who choose to build on this foundation should be aware of several important caveats. First, although our recommendations are based on empirical work, most of that work has not been conducted in health care providers and settings. It will therefore be important to rigorously evaluate future interventions in terms of their success in reducing racial bias in patient–provider encounters, as well as to understand any unintended consequences. Second, it is important to recognize that the initial stages of changing old habits and developing new skills are likely to temporarily result in greater cognitive burden for providers during clinical encounters. Thus, it will take time and practice before providers are able to execute the strategies we have discussed in an automatic way, and to therefore fully realize their purported benefit. Third, not all of our recommendations should necessarily be construed as elements of single or stand-alone programs. Efforts such as enhancing empathy and partnership building are relevant not only to reducing provider bias but to improving patient–provider relationships in general. These efforts may already be incorporated into training programs and curricula that are not explicitly focused on reducing racial disparities. In such cases, our framework might serve as a guide to help educators and administrators determine to what extent their training environment includes, or does not include, components that are likely to contribute to reducing provider racial bias. Finally, our discussion is not meant to be a comprehensive review of all potentially important principles and strategies for reducing provider bias, but instead is intended to provide a starting point for the important task of reducing racial bias in health care.
Acknowledgments
Dr. Burgess is supported by a Merit Review Entry Program Award from VA HSR&D. Dr. Saha is supported by awards from the VA HSR&D Advanced Career Development Program and the Robert Wood Johnson Generalist Physician Faculty Scholars Program. The authors would like to thank Dr. Hanna Bloomfield for her helpful comments.
Conflicts of Interest None disclosed.
References
- 1.Moy E, Dayton E, Clancy CM. Compiling the evidence: The national health care disparities reports. Health Aff (Millwood). 2005;24(2):376–87. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2.Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, eds. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Healthcare. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press; 2002. [PubMed]
- 3.Kelley E, Moy E, Stryer D, Burstin H, Clancy C. The national healthcare quality and disparities reports: an overview. Med Care. 2005;43(suppl 3):I3–8, Mar. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 4.van Ryn M, Burke J. The effect of patient race and socio-economic status on physicians’ perceptions of patients. Soc Sci Med. 2000;50(6):813–28. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 5.van Ryn M. Research on the provider contribution to race/ethnicity disparities in medical care. Med Care. 2002;40(suppl):I 140–51. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 6.Burgess DJ, Fu SS, van Ryn M. Why do providers contribute to disparities and what can be done about it? J Gen Intern Med. 2004;19(11):1154–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 7.Kelly CE. Bringing homophobia out of the closet: antigay bias within the patient–physician relationship. Pharos Alpha Omega Alpha Honor Med Soc. 1992;55(1):2–8. [PubMed]
- 8.Kelly JA, St Lawrence JS, Smith S, Hood HV, Cook DJ. Medical students’ attitudes toward AIDS and homosexual patients. J Med Educ. 1987;62(7):549–56. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 9.Kelly JA, St Lawrence JS, Smith S, Jr., Hood HV, Cook DJ. Stigmatization of AIDS patients by physicians. Am J Public Health. 1987;77(7):789–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 10.Lewis G, Croft-Jeffreys C, David A. Are British psychiatrists racist? Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157:410–15. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 11.Like R, Zyzanski SJ. Patient satisfaction with the clinical encounter: social psychological determinants. Soc Sci Med. 1987;24(4):351–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 12.Porter JR, Beuf AH. The effect of a racially consonant medical context on adjustment of African-American patients to physical disability. Med Anthropol. 1994;16(1):1–16. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 13.Rosenthal MP, Diamond JJ, Rabinowitz HK, et al. Influence of income, hours worked, and loan repayment on medical students’ decision to pursue a primary care career. JAMA. 1994;271(12):914–7. [PubMed]
- 14.Roter DL, Hall JA. Strategies for enhancing patient adherence to medical recommendations. JAMA. 1994;271(1):80. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 15.Schulman KA, Berlin JA, Harless W, et al. The effect of race and sex on physicians’ recommendations for cardiac catheterization. N Engl J Med. 1999;340(8):618–26. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 16.Stern M, Arenson E. Childhood cancer stereotype: impact on adult perceptions of children. J Pediatr Psychol. 1989;14(4):593–605. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 17.Tobin JN, Wasserheil-Smoller S, Wexler JP, et al. Sex bias in considering coronary bypass surgery. Ann Intern Med. 1987;107:19–25. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 18.Darley JG. A hypothesis confirming bias in labeling effects. In: Stangor C, ed. Sterotypes and Prejudice. Ann Arbor, MI: Taylor and Francis, Psychology Press; 2000.
- 19.Duncan B. Differential social perception and attribution. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1976;34:22–37. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 20.Kunda Z. Social Cognition: Making Sense of People. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press; 1999.
- 21.Kunda Z, Sherman-Williams B. Stereotypes and the construal of individuating information. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1993;19:90–9.
- 22.Lepore L, Brown R. Category and stereotype activation: is prejudice inevitable? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;72:275–87.
- 23.Locksley A, Hepburn C, Ortiz V. Social stereotypes and judgements of individuals: an instance of the base-rate fallacy. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1982;18:23–42.
- 24.Sagar H, Schofield J. Racial and behavioral cues in black and white children’s perceptions of ambiguously aggressive acts. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39:590–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 25.Stangor C, ed. Stereotypes and prejudice. Key readings in social psychology. Philadelphia: Psychology Press; 2001.
- 26.Banaji M, Greenwald AG. Implicit stereotyping and prejudice. In: Zanna MP, Olson JM, eds. The Psychology of Prejudice, The Ontario Symposium, vol. 7: 55–76.
- 27.Devine PG, Monteith MJ. Automaticity and control in stereotyping. In: Chaiken S, Trope Y, eds. Dual-process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guilford Press; 1999:339–60
- 28.Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL. On the nature of contemporary prejudice: The causes, consequences, and challenges of aversive racism. In: Eberhardt JL, Fiske, ST eds. Racism: The problem and the Response. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1998.
- 29.Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;82(1):62–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 30.Fazio RH, Jackson JR, Dunton BC, Williams CJ. Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(6):1013–27. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 31.Wilson TD, Lindsey S, Schooler TY. A model of dual attitudes. Psychol Rev. 2000;107(1):101–26. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 32.Cooper LA, Roter DL, Johnson RL, Ford DE, Steinwachs DM, Powe NR. Patient-centered communication, ratings of care, and concordance of patient and physician race. Ann Intern Med. 2003;139(11):907–15. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 33.Cooper-Patrick L, Powe NR, Jenckes MW, Gonzales JJ, Levine DM, Ford DE. Identification of patient attitudes and preferences regarding treatment of depression. J Gen Intern Med. 1997;12(7):431–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 34.Hooper EM, Comstock LM, Goodwin JM, Goodwin JS. Patient characteristics that influence physician behavior. Med Care. 1982;20(6):630–8 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35.Johnson RL, Roter D, Powe NR, Cooper LA. Patient race/ethnicity and quality of patient–physician communication during medical visits. Am J Public Health. 2004;94(12):2084–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 36.Cooper LA, Beach MC, Johnson RL, Inui TS. Delving below the surface. Understanding how race and ethnicity influence relationships in health care. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21(suppl 1):S21–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 37.Cooper LA, Roter D. Patient–provider communication: the effect of race and ethnicity on process and outcomes of healthcare. In: Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, eds. Unequal treatment: confronting racial and ethnic disparities in healthcare. Washington, D.C.: The National Academies Press; 2003:552–93. [PubMed]
- 38.Schuman H, Steeh C, Bobo L, Krysan M. Racial attitudes in America: Trends and interpretations, 2nd ed. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1997.
- 39.Bobo L. Racial attitudes and relations at the close of the twentieth century. In: Smelser NJ, Wilson WJ, Mitchell F, eds. Racial trends and their consequences. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2001:264–301.
- 40.Wittenbrink B, Judd CM, Park B. Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;72(2):262–74. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 41.Hunt JS, Rothmann TL, Rothman AJ, Iyer SN, McGorty EK. Implicit and explicit associations between health problems and social groups. Psychol Health. In press.
- 42.Brewer MB, Brown RJ. Intergroup relations. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, eds. The Handbook of Social Psychology (4th ed). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill; 1998.
- 43.Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL. Aversive racism. In: Zanna MP, ed. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol 36. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2004:1–51.
- 44.Tajfel H, ed. Differentiation between Social Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Oxford, England: Academic Press; 1978.
- 45.Devine PG, Monteith MJ. The role of discrepancy-associated affect in prejudice reduction. In: Mackie DM, Hamilton DL, eds. Affect, Cognition and Stereotyping: Interactive Processes In Intergroup Perception. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1993:317–44.
- 46.Kawakami K, Dovidio JF, Moll J, Hermsen S, Russin A. Just say no (to stereotyping): effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(5):871–88. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 47.Bargh J. The cognitive monster: The case against controllability of automatic stereotype effects. In: Chaiken S, Trope Y, eds. Dual Process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guilford Press; 1999:361–82.
- 48.Brewer MB. A dual process model of impression formation. In: Wyer TSSRS, ed. Advances in Social Cognition. Vol I. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988:1–36.
- 49.Fiske ST, Lin M, Neuberg SL. The continuum model: ten years later. In: Trope SCY, ed. Dual Process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guilford; 1999:211–54.
- 50.Blair IV. The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2002;6:242–61.
- 51.Fisher JD, Fisher WA, Amico KR. An information-motivation-behavioral skills model of adherence to antiretroviral therapy. Health Psychol. 2006;25:462–73. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 52.Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JLK. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(6):1464–80. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 53.Nosek BA. Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2005;134:565–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 54.Leippe MR, Eistenstadt D. Generalization of dissonance reduction: Decreasing prejudice through induced compliance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:395–413.
- 55.Rokeach M. The Nature of Human Values. New York: Free Press; 1973.
- 56.Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Reducing contemporary prejudice: combating explicit and implicit bias at the individual and intergroup level. In: Oskamp S, ed. Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2000:137–63.
- 57.Devine PG, Plant E, Amodio DM, Harmon-Jones E, Vance SL. The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: the role of motivations to respond without prejudice. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;82(5):835–48. [PubMed]
- 58.Plant EAD, Patricia G. Responses to other-imposed pro-Black pressure: acceptance or backlash? J Exp Soc Psychol. 2001;37:486–501.
- 59.Betancourt JR, Green AR, Carrillo JE, Ananeh-Firempong O, 2nd. Defining cultural competence: a practical framework for addressing racial/ethnic disparities in health and health care. Public Health Rep. 2003;118(4):293–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 60.Betancourt JR, Green AR, Carrillo JE, Park ER. Cultural competence and health care disparities: key perspectives and trends. Health Aff (Millwood). 2005;24(2):499–505. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 61.Macrae C, Bodenhausen GV, Milne AB, Jetten J. Out of mind but back in sight: stereotypes on the rebound. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67(5):808–17.
- 62.Macrae CN, Bodenhausen GV, Milne AB. Saying no to unwanted thoughts: self-focus and the regulation of mental life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(3):578–89. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 63.Wenzlaff RMW, Daniel M. Thought suppression. Annu Rev Psychol. 2000;51:59–91. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 64.Wyer NA, Sherman JW, Stroessner SJ. The roles of motivation and ability in controlling the consequences of stereotype suppression. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2000;26(1):13–25.
- 65.Shelton JN, Richeson JA, Salvatore J, Trawalter S. Ironic effects of racial bias during interracial interactions. Psychol Sci. 2005;16:397–402. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 66.Richeson JA, Shelton JN. When prejudice does not pay: effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychol Sci. 2003 14:287–90. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 67.Plant E, Devine PG. The antecedents and implications of interracial anxiety. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29(6):790–801. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 68.Vorauer JD, Kumhyr SM. Is this about you or me? Self- versus other-directed judgments and feelings in response to intergroup interaction. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2001;27:706–19.
- 69.Dovidio JF, Gaertner SE, Kawakami K, Hodson G. Why can’t we just get along? Interpersonal biases and interracial distrust. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2002;8(2):88–102. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 70.Dovidio JF, Hebl M, Richeson J, Shelton JN. Nonverbal communication, race, and intergroup interaction. In: Manusov V, Patterson ML, eds. Handbook of Nonverbal Communication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2006.
- 71.Pettigrew TF, Tropp LR. A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;90(5):751–83. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 72.Stephan WG, Stephan CW. Improving Intergroup Relations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2001.
- 73.Stein T, Frankel RM, Krupat E. Enhancing clinician communication skills in a large healthcare organization: a longitudinal case study. Patient Educ Couns. 2005;58(1):4–12. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 74.Johnson KJF, Barbara L. “We All Look the Same to Me”: positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face recognition. Psychol Sci. 2005;16:875–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 75.Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL, Isen AM, Lowrance R. Group representations and intergroup bias: positive affect, similarity, and group size. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1995;21(8):856–65.
- 76.Sexton K, Adgate JL, Church TR, et al. Children’s exposure to environmental tobacco smoke: using diverse exposure metrics to document ethnic/racial differences. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112(3):392–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 77.Krebs D. Empathy and altruism. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1975;32:1134–46. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 78.Arroyo JA. Psychotherapist bias with Hispanics: an analog study. Hisp J Behav Sci. 1996;18(21–8).
- 79.Batson CD. Altruism and prosocial behavior. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, eds. The Handbook of Social Psychology, No. 2. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 1998:282–316.
- 80.Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL, Stewart TL, Esses VM,ten Vergert M. From intervention to outcomes: processes in the reduction of bias. In: Stephan WG, Vogt P, eds. Intergroup Relations Programs: Practice, Research, and Theory. New York Teachers College Press; 2004:243–65.
- 81.Batson CD, Polycarpou MP, Harmon-Jones E, et al. Empathy and attitudes: can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;72(1):105–18. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 82.Dovidio JF, ten Vergert M, Stewart TL, et al. Perspective and prejudice: antecedents and mediating mechanisms. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004;30(12):1537–49. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 83.Finlay KA, Stephan WG. Improving intergroup relations: the effects of empathy on racial attitudes. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2000;30(8):1720–37.
- 84.Galinsky AD, Moskowitz GB. Perspective-taking: decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(4):708–24. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 85.Carmel S, Glick SM. Compassionate-empathic physicians: personality traits and socio-organizational factors that enhance or inhibit this behavior pattern. Soc Sci Med. 1996;43(8):1253–61. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 86.Batson CD, Kobrynowicz D, Dinnerstein JL, Kampf HC, Wilson AD. In a very different voice: unmasking moral hypocrisy. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;72(6):1335–48. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 87.Beck RS, Daughtridge R, Sloane PD. Physician–patient communication in the primary care office: a systematic review. J Am Board Fam Pract. 2002;15(1):25–38. [PubMed]
- 88.Ryan S, Hassell A, Dawes P, Kendall S. Control perceptions in patients with rheumatoid arthritis: the impact of the medical consultation. Rheumatology. 2003;42(1):135–40. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 89.Kim SS, Kaplowitz S, Johnston MV. The effects of physician empathy on patient satisfaction and compliance. Eval Health Prof. 2004;27(3):237–51. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 90.Zachariae R, Pedersen CG, Jensen AB, et al. Association of perceived physician communication style with patient satisfaction, distress, cancer-related self-efficacy, and perceived cntrol over the disease. Br J Cancer. 2004;88(5):658–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 91.Bellini LM, Baime M, Shea J. Variation of mood and empathy during internship. JAMA. 2002;287:3143–46. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 92.Bellini LM, Shea JA. Mood change and empathy decline persist during three years of internal medicine training. Acad Med. 2005;80(2):164–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 93.Diseker RA, Michielutte R. An analysis of empathy in medical students before and following clinical experience. J Med Educ. 1981;56(12):1004–10. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 94.Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, et al. An empirical study of decline in empathy in medical school. Med Educ. 2004;38(9):934–41. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 95.Cosgray RE, Davidhizar RE, Grostefon JD, Powell M, Wringer PH. A day in the life of an inpatient: an experiential game to promote empathy for individuals in a psychiatric hospital. Arch Psychiatr Nurs. 1990;4(6):354–9. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 96.Henderson P, Johnson MH. Assisting medical students to conduct empathic conversations with patients from a sexual medicine clinic. Sex Transm Infect. 2002;78(4):246–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 97.Seaberg DC, Godwin SA, Perry SJ. Teaching patient empathy: the ED visit program. Acad Emerg Med. 2000;7(12):1433–6. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 98.Gaertner SL, Dovidio JF. Reducing Intergroup Bias: The Common Ingroup Identity Model. Philadelphia, PA, US: Psychology Press. 2000;212.
- 99.Fiske ST. Stereotyping, Prejudice, and Discrimination. Vol 2., 4th ed. Boston: McGraw-Hill; 1998.
- 100.Fiske ST. What we know about bias and intergroup conflict, the problem of the century. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2002;11(4):123–8.
- 101.Hewstone M, Rubin M, Willis H. Intergroup bias. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:575–604. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 102.Hewstone M. The ‘ultimate attribution error’? A review of the literature on intergroup causal attribution. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1990;20:311–35.
- 103.Howard JW, Rothbart M. Social categorization and memory for in-group and out-group behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;38(2):301–10.
- 104.Park B, Rothbart M. Perception of out-group homogeneity and levels of social categorization: memory for the subordinate attributes of in-group and out-group members. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1982;42(6):1051–68.
- 105.Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL, Validzic A, Matoka K, Johnson B, Frazier S. Extending the benefits of recategorization: evaluations, self-disclosure, and helping. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1997;33(4):401–20. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 106.Hornstein HA. Cruelty and Kindness: A new Look at Aggression and Altruism. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1976.
- 107.Piliavin JAD, JF, Gaertner SL, Clark RD. Emergency Intervention. New York: Academic Press; 1981.
- 108.Nier JA, Gaertner SL, Dovidio JF, et al. Changing interracial evaluations and behavior: the effects of a common group identity. Group Proces Intergroup Relat. 2001;4:299–316.
- 109.Stewart M, Brown JB, Weston WW, McWhinney IR, McWilliam CL, Freeman TR. Patient-centered Medicine: Transforming the Clinical Method. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1995.
- 110.Kinnersley P, Stott N, Peters TJ, Harvey I. The patient-centredness of consultations and outcome in primary care. Br J Gen Pract. 1999;49:711–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 111.Little P, et al. Preferences of patients for patient centred approach to consultation in primary care: observational study. Br Med J. 2001;322:468–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 112.Mead N, Bower P, Hann M. The impact of general practitioners’ patient-centredness on patients’ post-consultation satisfaction and enablement. Soc Sci Med. 2002;55(2):283–99. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 113.Roter DL, Stewart M, Putnam SM, Lipkin M, Jr., Stiles W, Inui TS. Communication patterns of primary care physicians. JAMA. 1997;277(4):350–6. [PubMed]
- 114.Stewart M, Brown JB, Donner A, et al. The impact of patient-centered care on outcomes. J Fam Pract. 2000;49(9):796–804. [PubMed]