Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Mar 6.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry. 2008 Feb 1;63(3):253–255. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.11.014

Impulsivity, Compulsivity, and Habit: The Role of Orbitofrontal Cortex Revisited

Mary M Torregrossa 1, Jennifer J Quinn 1, Jane R Taylor 1
PMCID: PMC2265211  NIHMSID: NIHMS38477  PMID: 18194683

Humans suffering damage to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) are often described as impulsive. The most famous example is Phineas Gage, a railway worker, who in 1848 suffered extreme frontal lobe damage when a long iron rod was projected through his skull after an accidental explosion. Gage survived, but was reported to have an extreme change in personality, including increased inappropriate behavior (impulsiveness) [12]. People with OFC lesions are more impulsive compared to both normal controls and people with non-OFC frontal cortex damage, as measured by self-report and by cognitive/behavioral tasks [3]. But, how is impulsivity defined and how can it be measured?

Impulsivity involves behaviors that are inappropriate for the context, premature, poorly planned and often resulting in adverse consequences. Impulsive behaviors have been described as having three dimensions: 1) an inability to use available information to reflect on the consequences of actions; 2) an inability to forego an immediate small reward in favor of a delayed larger reward; 3) a deficit in suppressing prepotent motor responses [4]. Taken together, these three dimensions of impulsivity reflect an inability to evaluate and subsequently respond flexibly in search of a specific goal or outcome under changing environmental conditions. In this commentary, we reflect on the similarities between impulsive, compulsive and habitual behavior and hypothesize a common neurobiological circuit that depends critically on the function of the OFC.

The ‘toggle’ between flexible, goal-directed actions and reflexive, stimulus-driven habits

In combination with its well-described involvement in inhibitory control [5; 6] the prefrontal cortex (PFC) – including the OFC -- is critical for decision-making and response-selection. The PFC is impaired in disorders of impulsivity and compulsivity such as drug addiction, obsessive compulsive disorder, attention-deficit disorder and Tourette syndrome [5; 712].

Distinct regions of the PFC work in concert with the striatum, forming a distributed network responsible for processing of reward information, reward-related learning, goal-directed actions and the formation of habits [1320]. For example, during the acquisition of actions such as lever-pressing, performance is controlled by an expectation of the future consequences of that action (i.e., a R-O association is acquired). This form of goal-directed instrumental learning is flexible and is argued to be under PFC executive control, as is shown by a reduction in response performance when the outcome is made less attractive (outcome devaluation) [21]. Following repeated practice, however, such control is diminished and performance shifts to depend upon an association between antecedent environmental stimuli and the response (i.e., a S-R association is formed). Accordingly, as training progresses, instrumental performance becomes increasingly insensitive to outcome devaluation [22]. This criterion provides an objective means to discriminate between goal-directed actions and stimulus-driven habits [23], known to be dependent upon corticostriatal circuits [24].

OFC and the modulation of goal-directed behavior

Cognitive control of behavior – the ability to integrate thoughts, emotions and individual motor responses into coordinated, goal-directed behavior – is thought to depend upon the OFC [25]. Several studies have demonstrated that lesions of the OFC in both humans and rodents impair the ability to use outcome expectancies in guiding behavior [2629]. Further, neuroimaging studies reveal changes in OFC activation during action selection following reinforcer devaluation [3031]. Therefore, the OFC may be critically important for guiding behavior on the basis of available information about the consequences of one’s actions. This ability is impaired both in individuals engaged in stimulus-driven habits and in those who have impulse-control disorders, suggesting that these behaviors may have similar neurobiological features. Accordingly, several studies investigating the role of OFC in impulsivity also suggest a role for OFC in habit.

OFC and impulsivity

One dimension of impulsivity is the inability to forego small, immediate rewards for larger, delayed rewards. This deficiency can be quantified in a behavioral task known as delay discounting. In general, the person or animal is given a choice between a response that produces an immediate, small reward, and a response that produces a larger reward after some temporal delay. If the delay is sufficiently short, normal subjects prefer the larger reward; as the delay increases, preference shifts to the small, immediate reward. People with psychiatric disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), drug addiction, and pathological gambling, which are associated with increased impulsivity, will discount a delay more readily than controls (i.e., they prefer the immediate reward) [3233]. Likewise, rats chronically treated with cocaine respond more impulsively than saline treated rats in a delay-discounting procedure for food reward [34]. Interestingly, OFC lesioned rats, rather than behaving more impulsively, favor the larger, delayed reward at delays that make sham-lesioned rats switch their preference to the smaller reward [35]. This result suggests that the role of the OFC may not be to inhibit impulsive behaviors, but may be to assess and update the value of an outcome under changing conditions. The inability to alter behavior despite a decrease in the value of the outcome is reminiscent of habit behavior (see discussion above), and suggests an alternative role for the OFC in guiding behavior.

In addition, impulsivity involves an inability to inhibit prepotent (well-established) motor responses. Differences in response inhibition can be measured using the 5-choice serial reaction time (5-CSRTT), go/no-go, or stop-signal tasks. Humans with impulsive disorders such as ADHD, trichotillomania (repetitive hair pulling), and drug addiction have been shown to have deficits in motor response inhibition in the go/no-go and stop-signal reaction time tasks [4]. The 5-CSRTT allows for measurements of response accuracy, response omissions, premature responses or impulsivity, and perseverative responses. OFC lesioned rats have increased omissions, premature, and perseverative responses, suggesting that the OFC dysfunction plays a role in impulsivity, but also in response flexibility [36]. OFC lesions in the rat also produce deficits in the stop-signal reaction time task, again implicating the OFC in the inhibition of prepotent motor responses [37].

The most common finding in animals and humans with OFC lesions is a reversal-learning deficit. OFC lesions cause reversals to be acquired more slowly, and involve continued performance of the response that is no longer rewarded [38; 39]. This result has often been interpreted as a failure to inhibit prepotent responses, but several lines of evidence suggest that the OFC may actually be important for encoding the outcome of the response (i.e., the presence or absence of the reward) [39]. Thus, the reversal-learning deficit observed with OFC dysfunction could be considered a failure to devalue the reinforcer (that is no longer presented), which could be interpreted as an increase in habitual responding.

Implications

Habits and impulsive behavior may intuitively appear to be on opposite ends of the behavioral spectrum, but the behaviors that define impulsivity and habit have some commonalities. Clinically speaking, the comparison between impulsivity and habit may be much like comparing impulsivity and compulsivity. Compulsivity is epitomized by obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and Tourettes syndrome, where a person feels compelled to perform a behavior in order to relieve anxiety or stress, even if the behavior is inappropriate or counterproductive. These ritualistic behaviors are often described as habitual, and involve dysfunction in OFC [40]. However, the inappropriateness of the behavior, and the inability to inhibit a prepotent motor response also defines impulsivity. Impulsive disorders are often described as having compulsive features. For example, kleptomania and pathological gambling are considered impulsive disorders, yet those afflicted often describe obsessing about stealing or gambling and feeling compelled to do so [41].

Drug addiction also is a disorder that is certainly described as involving an impulsiveness to take drugs (especially in the initial phases of drug-taking), a compulsion to take drugs after chronic use, and the eventual development of a drug habit, characterized by automated responses to take the drug despite its adverse consequences [see review by Schoenbaum and Shaham in this issue 42; 43]. In addition, Diergaarde and colleagues [44] report in this issue that rats with increased impulsivity on the 5-CSRTT and delayed reward task will subsequently show increased motivation to self-administer nicotine and a resistance to stop responding for nicotine in extinction, suggesting that impulsiveness may yield vulnerability toward compulsion and habit ultimately leading to substance abuse disorders. The neural systems regulating impulsive, compulsive, and habitual behaviors likely have some differences; however, there may be overlapping neurobiology (e.g., activation of the OFC) that may explain why several psychiatric disorders have co-morbid impulsive and compulsive features.

Future studies aimed at disentangling the psychological constructs of impulsivity, compulsivity, and habit, and defining their neurobiological underpinnings within corticostriatal circuits will likely reveal a critical role for the OFC in adaptive behavioral regulation, as well as new insights into multiple psychiatric disorders characterized by maladaptive, inflexible, decision-making and response-selection processes. Such a focus has already become an area of intensive research within multiple disciplines relevant to Biological Psychiatry.

Acknowledgments

The preparation of this commentary was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (DA011717, DA015222) and the Tourette Syndrome Association.

Drs. Torregrossa, Quinn and Taylor have not received lecture fees or research funding from any commercial interests and they do not serve as paid consultants for any pharmaceutical or other companies, and consequently have no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

From the Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, Abraham Ribicoff Research Facilities, Connecticut Mental Health Center, New Haven, Connecticut.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Haas LF. Phineas Gage and the science of brain localization. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;71:761. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.6.761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Murray EA, O’Doherty JP, Schoenbaum G. What we know and do not know about the functions of the orbitofrontal cortex after 20 years of cross-species studies. J Neurosci. 2007;27:8166–8169. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1556-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Berlin HA, Rolls ET, Kischka U. Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2004;127:1108–1126. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chamberlain SR, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Curr OpinPsychiatry. 2007;20:255–261. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ba4989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacol. 1999;146:373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Roberts AC, Wallis JD. Inhibitory control and affective processing in the prefrontal cortex: neuropsychological studies in the common marmoset. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:252–262. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hildebrandt H, Brokate B, Hoffmann E, Kroger B, Eling P. Conditional responding is impaired in chronic alcoholics. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28:631–645. doi: 10.1080/13803390590949520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bechara A. Risky business: emotion, decision-making, and addiction. J Gambl Stud. 2003;19:23–51. doi: 10.1023/a:1021223113233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Schoenbaum G, Roesch MR, Stalnaker Stalnaker TA. Orbitofrontal cortex, decision-making and drug addiction. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29:116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R, Wynne K, Baker NB, Hunter J, Carthy T, Booker E, London M, Deakin JF, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacol. 1999;20:322–339. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00091-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sheppard DM, Bradshaw JL, Purcell R, Pantelis C. Tourette’s and comorbid syndromes: obsessive compulsive and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. A common etiology. Clin Psychol Rev. 1999;19:531–552. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00059-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Drug addiction: bad habits add up. Nature. 1999;398:567–570. doi: 10.1038/19208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gerdeman GL, Partridge JG, Lupica CR, Lovinger DM. It could be habit forming: drugs of abuse and striatal synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:184–192. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00065-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vanderschuren LJ, Di Ciano P, Everitt BJ. Involvement of the dorsal striatum in cue-controlled cocaine seeking. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8665–8670. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0925-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, Jayne M, Ma Y, Wong C. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6583–6588. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19:181–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Blockade of NMDA receptors in the dorsomedial striatum prevents action-outcome learning in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:505–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04219.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Inactivation of dorsolateral striatum enhances sensitivity to changes in the action-outcome contingency in instrumental conditioning. Behav Brain Res. 2006;166:189–196. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:513–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04218.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Colwill RM, Rescorla RA. Effect of reinforcer devaluation on discriminative control of instrumental behavior. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1990;16:40–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dickinson A. Actions and habits: The development of behavioural autonomy. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 1985;308:67–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Balleine BW, Dickinson A. Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37:407–419. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00033-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:464–476. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Holland PC, Gallagher M. Amygdala-frontal interactions and reward expectancy. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:148–155. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50:7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Setlow B, Gallagher M, Holland PC, Schoenbaum G. Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task. J Neurosci. 2003;23:11078–11084. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11078.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M, Holland PC. Orbitofrontal lesions impair use of cue-outcome associations in a devaluation task. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:317–322. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome encoding in Pavlovian but not instrumental conditioning. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4819–4825. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5443-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Valentin VV, Dickinson A, O’Doherty JP. Determining the neural substrates of goal-directed learning in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4019–4046. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0564-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gottfried JA, O’Doherty J, Dolan RJ. Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science. 2003;301:1104–1107. doi: 10.1126/science.1087919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Reynolds B. A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to drug use and gambling. Behav Pharmacology. 2006;17:651–667. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3280115f99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Winstanley CA, Eagle DM, Robbins TW. Behavioral models of impulsivity inrelation to ADHD: translation between clinical and preclinical studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:379–395. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Simon NW, Mendex IA, Setlow B. Cocaine exposure causes long-term increases in impulsive choice. Behav Neurosci. 2007;121:543–549. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.3.543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4718–4722. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5606-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Chudasama Y, Passetti F, Rhodes SE, Lopian D, Desai A, Robbins TW. Dissociable aspects of performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following lesions of the dorsal anterior cingulate, infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat: differential effects on selectivity, impulsivity, and compulsivity. Behav Brain Res. 2003;146:105–119. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Eagle DM, Baunez C, Hutcheson DM, Lehmann O, Shah AP, Robbins TW. Stop-Signal Reaction-Time Task Performance: Role of Prefrontal Cortex and Subthalamic Nucleus. Cereb Cortex. 2007 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23:8771–8780. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08771.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Schoenbaum G, Saddoris MP, Stalnaker TA. Reconciling the roles of orbitofrontal cortex in reversal learning and the encoding of outcome expectancies. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2007 doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Evans DW, Lewis MD, Iobst E. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in normally developing compulsive-like behaviors and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain and Cognition. 2004;55:220–234. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00274-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Grant JE, Potenza MN. Compulsive aspects of impulse-control disorders. Psychiatry Clin North Am. 2006;29:539–551. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2006.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Schoenbaum G, Shaham Y. The role of orbitofrontal cortex in drug addiction: a review of preclinical studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2007 doi: 10.1016/j.biopscyh.2007.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Diergaarde L, Pattij T, Poortvliet I, Hogenboom F, de Vries W, Schoffelmeer ANM, De Vries TJ. Impulsive choice and impulsive action predict vulnerability to distinct stages of nicotine seeking in rats. Biol Psychiatry. 2007 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES