Abstract
A review of the literature in both animals and humans reveals that changes in sex hormone have often been associated with changes in behavioral and mental abilities. Previously published research from our laboratory, and others, provides strong evidence that P300 (latency) event-related potential (ERP), a marker of neuronal processing speed, is an accurate predictor of early memory impairment in both males and females across a wide age range. It is our hypothesis, given the vast literature on the subject, that coupling growth hormones (insulin-like growth factor-I, (IGF-I) and insulin-like growth factor binding protein 3 (IGF-BP3)), P300 event-related potential and test of variables of attention (TOVA) are important neuroendocrinological predictors of early cognitive decline in a clinical setting. To support this hypothesis, we utilized structural equation modeling (SEM) parameter estimates to determine the relationship between aging and memory, as mediated by growth hormone (GH) levels (indirectly measured through the insulin-like growth factor system), P300 latency and TOVA, putative neurocognitive predictors tested in this study. An SEM was developed hypothesizing a causal directive path, leading from age to memory, mediated by IGF-1 and IGF-BP3, P300 latency (speed), and TOVA decrements. An increase in age was accompanied by a decrease in IGF-1 and IGF-BP3, an increase in P300 latency, a prolongation in TOVA response time, and a decrease in memory functioning. Moreover, independent of age, decreases in IGF-1 and IGF-BP3, were accompanied by increases in P300 latency, and were accompanied by increases in TOVA response time. Finally, increases in P300 latency were accompanied by decreased memory function, both directly and indirectly through mediation of TOVA response time. In summary, this is the first report utilizing SEM to reveal the finding that aging affects memory function negatively through mediation of decreased IGF-1 and IGF-BP3, and increased P300 latency (delayed attention and processing speed).
Keywords: Structural equation modeling (SEM), P300 latency, TOVA, IGF-1, IGF-BP3, Age and memory
Introduction
Studies demonstrate that all individuals between the ages of 50 and 80 experience some form of mild cognitive impairment and in many cases, numerous cognitive changes. Most (as much as 50%) progress to advanced dementia by age 80. During this time (age 50–80) of progressive cognitive decline few, if any, memory assessments are performed by physicians. As a result, dementia has now become the 8th leading disability in the United States.
Many hormones in the body also decline as we age, with certain exceptions. Those that decline include insulin-like growth factors, testosterone (males), estradiol (females), dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its sulfate, Triiodothyronine, 1,25(OH)2 Vitamin D, inhibin, arginine vasopressin and pregnenolone. Those that increase with age include insulin, vasopressin (basal), cholecystokinin, atrial natriuretic peptide, norepinephrine, epinephrine, FSH, LH (women and some men), parathormone, cortisol, activin (males) and prolactin (males) (Morley and van den Berg 2000).
The question arises, are the hormonal changes antecedent to the cognitive decline; not related to the cognitive decline; or occur as a result of the changes in the brain? Numerous studies support the hypothesis that hormonal decline contributes to the cognitive changes in the brain.
The progression to full blown dementia includes, loss of brain processing speed, decline in memory and attention function, personality and temperament change, IQ changes, and ultimately, the inability to perform daily tasks. It is noteworthy that the impact on declining brain process is influenced by hormone deficiencies including growth hormone (Messier et al. 2004; Kalmijn et al. 2000a,b; Modrego and Ferrandez 2004; Mahajan et al. 2004; Pandian and Nakamoto 2004; Brooke and Monson 2003; Polleri et al. 2002; Aleman et al. 1999; Watanabe et al. 2004; Herrmann et al. 2004; Dumas et al. 2006; Gleason et al. 2005; Pinkerton and Henderson 2005; Honjo et al. 2005; Levine and Battista 2004), estrogen (Burkhardt et al. 2004; Bates et al. 2005; Falter et al. 2006; Hogervorst et al. 2004; Bremner 2004; Okun et al. 2004; Hoskin et al. 2004), testosterone (Moffat et al. 2004; Henderson and Hogervorst 2004; Rosario et al. 2004; Cherrier et al. 2005; Leblhuber et al. 2004; Bicikova et al. 2004; Yai et al. 2003; Weill-Engerer et al. 2002; De Bruin et al. 2002), DHEA (De Bruin et al. 2002; Brown et al. 2003; Ravaglia et al. 2002; Armanini et al. 2003; Rasmuson et al. 2002; Knopman and Henderson 2003; Hoskin et al. 2004; Hoskin et al. 2004; Bowen et al. 2002; Casadesus et al. 2005; Gregory and Bowen 2005; Arwert et al. 2005a,b,c,d), sex binding globulin and other hormones (van Dam 2005; Casadesus et al. 2006; Meethal et al. 2003; Shively and Bethea 2004; Pike 2001; Lim et al. 2003; Wolkowitz et al. 2003).
A review of the literature reveals that sex hormones (Cauley et al. 1989; Kalmijn et al. 2000a,b; Raynaud-Simon et al. 2000; Sherwin 2003; Kawas et al. 1997; Tang et al. 1996; Mulnard et al. 2000; Norbury et al. 2003; Kolsch and Rao 2000; Compton et al. 2001; Behl et al. 1997; Barrett-Conner and Goodman-Gruen 1999; Behl and Manthey 2000; Sano 2000; Cyr et al. 2000; Cholerton et al. 2002; Wang et al. 2000; Breltner and Zandi 2003; Smith and Levin-Allerhand 2003), DHEA (Tan and Pu 2001; Huppert et al. 2000; Leblhuber et al. 2004; Vallee et al. 2001; Murialdo et al. 2001; Pavel et al. 2003; Aleman et al. 1999; Arai et al. 2001; Petruzzi et al. 2002; Kalmijn et al. 2000a,b) and other hormones (i.e. growth hormones) have often been associated with changes in behavioral and mental abilities, memory, brain glucose utilization, neuronal function and dendritic architecture in both animal and human studies (Donahue et al. 2006; Grill et al. 2005; Sonntag et al. 2005a,b; Berton et al. 2006; Lichtenwalner et al. 2001; Ramsey et al. 2004; Shi et al. 2005; Lynch et al. 2001; Thornton et al. 2000; Shin et al. 2005). A series of studies by Sonntag et al. and others strongly suggests a relationship between GH and memory in animals, and provides a framework to evaluate possible relationships in the human as well (Kappeler and Epelbaum 2005; Sonntag et al. 2001; Brunso-Bechtold et al. 2000; Sonntag et al. 2000; Almeida and Barclay 2001). Additionally, the (GH)/IGF–1 axis is known to be involved in aging of physiological functions including, low levels correlating with cognitive decline (Almeida and Barclay 2001) as a function of age. In fact, GH deficiency, an important regulator of IGF-1, is associated with reduced well-being.
A recent Medline search revealed that evaluating IGF-1 can improve well-being in GH deficient adults (Asthana et al. 1999), as well as improve cognitive functioning by increasing brain processing speed (Baum et al. 1998; Rollero et al. 1998; Carro et al. 2002; Van Dam and Aleman 2004; Arai et al. 2001). IGF-1 may also play an important role in the speed of information processing and intelligence (Papadakis et al. 2005). Low IGF-1 levels were directly correlated with lower mini-mental state examination (MMSE) scores in patients with more advanced cognitive (Burman and Deijen 1998) and neuronal function deterioration (Arwert et al. 2005a,b,c,d).
In this regard, a meta analysis revealed that an overall relationship between IGF-1 levels and cognitive functioning in healthy elderly people exist (Arwert et al. 2005a,b,c,d; Binoux 1999). Finally, metabolic studies on insulin-growth factors have been intensively investigated (Baxter and Martin 1986; Arwert et al. 2005a,b,c,d; van Dam et al. 2005). It is noteworthy that a major function of growth hormones in the elderly involves a cellular repair mechanism potentially increasing neuronal connections (Demlin 2005; Leor et al. 2006; Li et al. 2006; Simpson et al. 2006).
In order to evaluate the interrelationships between plasma hormone levels, cognition, and memory, we utilized the P300 (latency) ERP. This electrophysiological marker of brain processing speed has been the subject of many neurocognitive studies (Braverman and Blum 1996, 2003; Braverman et al. 2006). Results suggest that genetic antecedents may control brain P300 functionality (Mulert et al. 2006; Gallinat et al. 2003; Nacher 2000; Comings et al. 1999; Hill et al. 1998; Johnson et al. 1997; Polich and Bloom 1999; Begleiter et al. 1998).
It is our hypothesis that the small percent of individuals seem destined to escape Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), and their relative invulnerability may reflect both genetic and/or environmental factors, which may be related in part to sex hormones and the IGF-1 system (Khachaturian et al. 2004; Morley 2003).
Abnormalities on the TOVA have previously been shown to accurately predict impaired Wechsler memory scale-3rd edition (WMS III) scores and early dementia by our laboratory (Braverman et al. 2006). Thus, we have proposed earlier that the optimal mode for identifying patients at risk for dementia and/or AD would be to couple TOVA with other standard diagnostic techniques, i.e., MMSE, WMS-III, and P300 latency (Braverman and Blum 2003, 1996; Braverman et al. 2006). Screening patients for early dementia requires accurate characterization of attention failure in a clinical setting.
The aim of the present study is to quantify the relationship between plasma growth hormones, P300 ERP and TOVA as putative neuroendocrinological predictors of early cognitive decline in a clinical setting. Thus, structural equation modeling (SEM) parameter estimates were developed hypothesizing a causal directive path leading from age to memory mediated by estrogen, progesterone, testosterone, IGF-1, IGF-BP3, P300 speed, and TOVA.
Methods
In this SEM model, depicted in Fig. 1, age was used as the major independent, or exogenous variable, and memory was used as the major dependent, or endogenous variable. Hormonal levels, P300 and TOVA were hypothesized as intervening variables.
Fig. 1.
SEM of age on memory with mediating variables. The Pearson correlations appear within parentheses; the SEM coefficients, in the form of standardized regression weights, appear without parentheses. All coefficients shown are statistically significant beyond the 0.05 level (one-tail tests)
Participants
A total of 1,545 patients participated in the study. Gender and age were recorded for these patients; 739 (47.6 percent) were male and 806 (52 percent) were female. Data taken from patients 40 years and older, M = 52.73 ± SD = 18.21. The average age for females was 53.74 ± SD = 18.16 and 51.76 ± SD = 18.22 for males. These patients were selected for study from an outpatient private medical/neuropsychiatric clinical practice and research foundation in New York City. Other than age due to missing data, only data for 1,372 of the original sample were analyzed utilizing the SEM.
All subjects signed an approved IRB consent form based on an approval from the PATH Foundation IRB committee (registration #IRB00002334) and ethics board approval from the PATH Foundation. The criterion for study inclusion was at least one P300 test for each patient. Trained EEG medical and psychometric technicians conducted the tests. All test interpreters were blinded to other patient results. All subjects were part of a catchment study involving brain electrical activity mapping and aging research.
Analysis of hormones
IGF-1 and IGF-BP3 were used as the hormone measures, shown in the model appearing in Fig. 1. This hypothesis was tested in the statistical analysis of the data described below.
Venipuncture was done in nonfasting subjects between 8:30 AM and 7:30 PM at baseline examination of the Path Medical Clinic Program. Blood samples were collected in 5-ml tubes containing a 0.5 ml sodium citrate solution. All tubes were stored on ice before and after blood sampling. Platelet-free plasma was obtained by –2-stage centrifugation (10 min at 1600 g at 4°C and 30 min at 7000 g at 4°C). Platelet-free samples were immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at −80°C. Assays were performed blinded to information on the subject. Plasma levels of estradiol and sex hormone-binding globulin were estimated with double anti-body radioimmunoassays (Bioreference Lab, New York, NY). As measures of the levels of bioavailable and free estradiol, testosterone, and nonprotein-bound estradiol, respectively, were calculated in the basis of hormone and binding protein levels, for the analysis of GH and IGF-BP3, the laboratory performed standardized procedures (Sodergard et al. 1982; van den Beid et al. 2000; Brondu et al. 1996).
P300 latency
All patients in this study were analyzed by the BEAM. A 24-channel EEG recorder was used according to previous studies in our laboratory (Braverman and Blum 1996, 2003; Braverman et al. 2006). The standard international 10/20 system of electrode placement was used. In addition, there were two electrodes on the earlobes, two EKG electrodes connected to the cervical spine, two supraorbital electrodes (EOG) and an electrocap. During BEAM data collection, digital EEG was recorded in a monopolar (LR linked ears left over right) and bipolar (LR 3,4 linked ears left over right) montage. Waveforms were averaged off-line, such that trials on which the EEG or EOG exceeded ±100 microvolts were rejected. Single-trial data also was subjected to an EOG correction procedure to remove any remaining artifact.
The computer averaged evoked potentials. The system mathematically adjusted the baseline to the average value of stimulus signal per channel. P300 results were read at the maximum voltage (dV differential voltage; amplitude is relative to the mean voltage of the entire waveform) electrode (i.e. usually at PZ). Neuropsychiatric patients frequently have peaks that occur at FZ, CZ, OZ, P3, P4, O1, and O2. Review by a neurologist showed our computerized QEEG-P300 test to be 100% inter- and intra-reliable.
TOVA
TOVA was developed in the 1960s and the current version number 7 was released in 1997 (Manor et al. 2004). The visual TOVA is 21.6 minutes in duration and its lengthy run allows it to effectively measure attention deficit as a continuous performance test (CPT). In the visual form, a square flashes on the screen for 1/10th of a second in two-second intervals. As a person sits in front of the screen, a small box appears either on the top of the square or the bottom of the square. If the small box appears on the top, it is labeled as the target, and if at the bottom, it is labeled as the non-target. Each time the target box appears, the person is instructed to press a small, accurate microswitch. Every time the non-target box appears, the person is instructed to refrain from pressing the microswitch. Each patient begins with a 1-minute practice test and is instructed to take the test as accurately, quickly, and consistently as possible without mistake. An omission error signifies the times the patient failed to click the microswitch at the correct time. A commission error signifies the number of times the patient clicked the microswitch at the wrong time. Response time result is the amount of time that the patient took to answer. Variability result is a measure of the consistency of the patient’s responses. In order for a score to be significantly deviant (SD) on the TOVA, it has to be less than –1.33. The TOVA score has to be less than –1.0 in order to be considered significantly deviant or borderline (SD-BL). TOVA results are age adjusted.
Memory
WMS-III is the standardized measure we used to assess learning and memory abilities in this study. Results are organized into summary index scores reflecting verbal, visual, immediate and working memory.
Statistical analysis
A path analysis or SEM was conducted to ascertain whether age influences memory through mediation of hormonal level, P300, and TOVA. SEM is a procedure used to statistically control for mediating effects when intervening variables occur in the research design (Bollen 1989; Maruyama 1997; Loehlin 1992; Long 1983). Once a zero-order correlation is found between two adjacent variables (X and Y) in such a model, SEM decomposes the coefficient into direct, indirect, total, and spurious effects. Often, what originates as a high correlation between X and Y proves to be null once the mediating effects of I are statistically controlled. In such cases, it is said that the effect of X on Y is indirect, mediated by I.
When all variables in the model are linked with one-another, the model is said to be full. When linkages are omitted, the model is said to be restricted. A key feature of SEM is its test of the statistical accuracy in omitting any set of linkages. The issue is addressed by multiple goodness of fit indices measuring the amount of variance explained in the dependent variables with and without the linkages included. Moreover, contemporary fit indices are least susceptible to the bias associated with confirmatory factor analysis involving a large number of measured variables. With this in mind, the most widely used statistical index for this test is the comparative fit index (CFI) (Bentler 1990). A CFI of 0.95 or higher indicates that the amounts of variance would be the same, and that therefore, in the interest of economy, the linkages in question may be omitted from the analysis. Three indices of fit were given: the CFI, the relative fit index (RFI) (Bentler 1990) and the normed fit index (NFI) of Bentler and Bonett (1980). Once a final model is identified, these fit indices were provided to show the model was an adequate fit for the data, if not, they were not included in the model.
The present SEM procedure involved a hybrid structure including both a measurement model for memory (a hypothesized factor composed of auditory, visual, immediate and working memory measures), and the structural model shown in Fig. 1.
On the basis of the above review of the literature, this model hypothesized negative effects of increasing age on IGF-1, IGF-BP3, P300, TOVA, and memory. The model also hypothesized negative effects of prolonged latency of P300 and TOVA decrements on memory. It is noteworthy that when there is a prolongation of P300 latency there is also TOVA decrements independent of age, as neuronal processing with prolonged P300 latency attention processing slows down. Both have a negative effect on memory. Antecedent to this decline of neuronal and attentional processing appears to be hormonal growth hormone factor loss. Since these hypotheses were unidirectional in form, the model was tested using one-tail tests of statistical significance.
Results
Although age exerted an influence on both testosterone (β = −0.11, df=1, p < 0.05) and estrogen (β = −0.24, df = 1, p < 0.05), no effects of testosterone or estrogen on P300 speed, TOVA, or memory were found. For testosterone these effects were β = −0.04, df = 1, p > 0.05, β = −0.03, df = 1, p >0.05, and β = −0.01, df =1, p >0.05, respectively. For estrogen, they were β = −0.06, df = 1, p < 0.05, β = −0.05, df = 1, p > 0.05, and β = −0.08, df = 1, p >0.05, respectively. These tests were asymptotic Z tests (with no degrees of freedom). Further, the GFI, CFI, and NFI scores forcing the non-significant testosterone and estrogen were 0.85, 0.78, and 0.65, respectively, indicating a decrease in the goodness of fit when estrogen and testosterone are included in the model. Given these findings, testosterone and estrogen were not considered further in the analyses of the data.
The correlation matrix of the variables in the study appears in Table 1. The means and standard deviations appear in Table 2, and the SEM outcomes appear in Fig. 1.
Table 1.
Correlation matrix
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Age | 1.00 | ||||||||
2. IGF1 | −0.225 | 1.00 | |||||||
3. IGF3 | −0.246 | 0.055 | 1.00 | ||||||
4. P300SPEE | 0.377 | −0.146 | −0.215 | 1.00 | |||||
5. TOVA | 0.040 | −0.015 | −0.023 | 0.106 | 1.00 | ||||
6. Immediate | −0.065 | 0.025 | 0.037 | −0.171 | −0.120 | 1.00 | |||
7. Working | −0.028 | 0.011 | 0.016 | −0.075 | −0.052 | 0.459 | 1.00 | ||
8. Visual | −0.050 | 0.019 | 0.028 | −0.132 | −0.092 | 0.810 | 0.353 | 1.00 | |
9. Auditory | −0.052 | 0.020 | 0.030 | −0.138 | −0.097 | 0.847 | 0.369 | 0.651 | 1.00 |
Table 2.
Summary statistics
Age | IGF1 | IGF3 | P300 | TOVA | Auditor memory | Visual memory | Immediate memory | Working memory | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | 52.73 | 182.3319 | 3.38402 | 340.3854 | .54 | 99.90 | 95.25 | 97.17 | 94.02 |
Std dev | 18.215 | 100.13261 | 1.149586 | 35.95276 | 1.150 | 19.482 | 20.033 | 21.833 | 16.810 |
Std error | .47 | 4.20 | .05 | .91 | .04 | .71 | .73 | .79 | .61 |
In Fig. 1, the Pearson correlation coefficients appear within parentheses; and the SEM coefficients, in the form of standardized regression weights (β), appear without parentheses. Only SEM coefficients statistically significant beyond the 0.05 level are displayed in Fig. 1. The model displayed a good fit of the data (CFI = 0.96, NFI = 0.95, RFI = 0.90) showing the justification in leaving out the parameter estimates where IGF-BP3 predicts memory linkage.
The factor loadings of auditory, visual, immediate and working memory measures of the memory factor were 0.83, p < 0.05, 0.79, p <0.05, 1.0, p<0.05, and 0.45, p < 0.05. These loadings were derived from the correlations of the four components of memory. The SEM hypothesized are identifying the construct of memory. These factor loadings mean that each measured component of memory had a strong (r>0.45) correlation with the single construct of memory.
The model explained 5% of the variance in memory, which may not be clinically relevant to which the contribution by age was β = −0.11, p < 0.05; that by P300 was β = −0.11, p = 0.05; and that by TOVA was β = −0.10, p < 0.05. The model also explained 2% of the variance in TOVA, which may not be clinically relevant to which the contribution by age was β = −0.09, p < 0.05; and that by P300 was β = 0.14, p < 0.05. The model explained 16% of the variance in P300, to which the contribution by age was β = 0.33, p < .05; that by IGF-BP3 was β = −0.13, p < 0.05; and that by IGF-1 was β = −0.07, p < 0.05.
As evidenced by the above findings, an increase in age was accompanied by decreases in IGF-BP3 and IGF-1; an increase in P300 latency; a decrease in TOVA response time; and a decrease in memory. Moreover, decreases in IGF-BP3 and IGF-1 were accompanied by an increase in P300 latency, and an increase in the latter was accompanied by an increase in TOVA response time. Finally, an increase in TOVA response time was accompanied by a decrease in memory. P300 influenced memory both directly and indirectly through mediation of TOVA.
Discussion
With regard to the present study it is noteworthy through a literature review, that binding sites for GH and, in particular, IGF-1 contribute to the function of certain brain areas (Sodergard et al. 1982). For example, several studies found that GH and IGF-1 contribute to the function of the hippocampus, a brain structure important for the maintenance of cognitive functions such as learning and memory (Pavel et al. 2003). Evidence for cognitive deficits in GH-deficient individuals has been found in several studies, some of which have shown that these deficits can be reversed by GH replacement therapy.
As previously noted, based on available data, one might hypothesize that relative GH or IGF-1 deficiency could contribute to the deterioration of cognitive function observed in the middle aged and elderly. Evidently, it appears that free testosterone levels negatively correlate with prolonged P300 latency especially in older men. These findings could have very significant importance in targeting both prevention and treatment of cognitive dysfunction in both males and females. Moreover, in other unpublished studies from our laboratory, we found a statistically significant relationship between age, memory, P300 and TOVA, with DHEA using SEM.
The present study does not reject the possibility that exogenous estrogens (Braverman et al. 2005; Dumas et al. 2006), testosterone, DHEA, and GH replacement may be beneficial in protecting against cognitive decline and AD, as a protectant against cell loss, as an antioxidant and as a protectant against other negative hormonal effects on brain function, including depression which may contribute to memory loss (Lobie et al. 2000; Heijer den et al. 2003; Burkhardt et al. 2006; LeBlance et al. 2001; Ellwart et al. 2003).
One important caveat in this study involves the percentage of clinical relevance using this SEM model whereby only 5% of the variability in memory is explained by these significant and important factors. Because the goodness of fit indices are greater than 0.90, it appears that much of the remaining 95% of variability in memory is either random error or other important factors such as genetics (other factors have not been adequately addressed in this proposed model). This is not to say that better models will not be found in the future and warrants intensive investigation. Moreover, the goodness of fit indices for the figure are all above 0.90 indicating a better fit for the model without estrogen and progesterone. The coefficients are not significant for estrogen and progesterone and the goodness of fit indices are reduced when these coefficients are included in the model. For these reasons, we leave estrogen and progesterone out of the model.
We believe that the primary objective of diagnostic evaluations is to determine the most appropriate targets for and efficacy of therapeutic interventions. Our objective here is to add multifactorial physical and statistical diagnostic correlatives to confirm, question, refute and/or clarify the diagnostic conclusions derived from conventional procedures and assessments, thereby improving the focus of targeted therapeutic interventions, especially as it relates to age related neurocognitive decline. In this regard better methodology is required in a clinical setting to predict potential AD.
Summary
It has been suggested that utilization of P300 latency, a potential inheritable factor (Mulert et al. 2006; Gallinat et al. 2003; Nacher 2000; Comings et al. 1999; Hill et al. 1998; Johnson et al. 1997; Polich and Bloom 1999; Begleiter et al. 1998; Berton et al. 2006), is an important marker for detecting cognitive dysfunction in GH deficient patients with Sheehan’s syndrome. It was proposed that these results must be performed on healthy patients to assess the clinical use of this electrophysiological method in the diagnosis of cognitive dysfunction due to GH deficiency.
In keeping with this suggestion, to our knowledge this is the first study to support the interactive role of growth hormone and other hormones and brain processing speed in humans attending a primary care facility. This takes on increased importance, when it is coupled with the recent finding that higher midlife free IGF-1 may be associated with better late-life cognition (Okereke et al. 2006)
We propose, based on this SEM, that primary care medicine directed at both the prevention and early diagnosis of dementia should be changed to emphasize the importance of coupling plasma hormone levels, with an electrophysiological marker such as P300 latency and TOVA decrements in response time as diagnostic tools.
This is the first study using SEM to reveal that age affects memory function negatively through mediation of decreased IGF-1 and IGF-BP3, increased P300 latency and prolonged TOVA response time.
Essentially, brain processing speed is antecedent to declines in memory and attention. It also appears that insulin growth factors, IGF-1 and IGF-BP3 (the precursor of GH), may be antecedent or concomitantly related to declines in attention and brain processing speed. This means that individuals who are approaching this wide spread epidemic of cognitive decline between the ages of 50 and 80, may need monitoring with psychometric testing and neuropsychological testing, as well as hormonal therapy.
Finally, our data support the contention that insulin-like growth factors, P300 ERP and TOVA, are important neuroendocrinological predictors of early cognitive decline in a clinical setting but interpretation of these interesting results must await further intensive investigation.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Salugen, Inc., San Diego, California, PATH Research Foundation, New York, NY, and Synaptamine, Inc., San Antonio, TX for their financial support. William Sonntag is the recipient of grant # P01 AG11370. We want to thank both Arpana Rayannavar and Neeta Makhija for their contributions to the manuscript and the entire PATH staff. A special acknowledgement is provided for the generosity of Rein Narma.
Conflict of interest statement We declare that we have no conflict of interest. Eric R. Braveman MD, is the director of PATH Clinics where he utilizes both the P300 and TOVA as diagnostics, and Kenneth Blum, PhD is the scientific director of the PATH Research Foundation and is a paid consultant.
References
- Aleman A, Verhaar HJ, De Haan EH, De Vries WR, Samson MM, Drent ML, Van Der Veen EA, Koppeschaar HP (1999) Insulin-like growth factor-I and cognitive function in healthy older men. Clin Endocrinol Metab 84(2):471–475 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Aleman A, de Vries WR, de Haan EG, Verhaar HJ, Samson MM, Koppeschaar HP (2000) Age-sensitive cognitive function, growth hormone and insulin growth factor 1 plasma levels in healthy older men. Neuropsychobiology 41(2):73–78 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Allen KV, Frier BM, Strachan MW (2004) The relationship between type 2 diabetes and cognitive dysfunction: longitudinal studies and their methodological limitations. Eur J Pharmacol 490(1–3):169–175 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Almeida OP, Barclay L (2001) Sex hormones and their impact on dementia and depression: a clinical perspective. Expert Opin Pharmacother 2(4):527-535 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Arai Y, Hirose N, Yamamura K, Shimizu K, Takayama M, Ebihara Y, Osono Y (2001) Serum insulin–like growth factor–1 in centenarians: implications of IGF-1 as a rapid turnover protein. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci 56(2):M79–M82 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Armanini D, Vecchio F, Basso A, Milone FF, Simoncini M, Fiore C, Matterello MJ, Sartorato P, Karbowiak I (2003) Alzheimer’s disease: pathophysiological implications of measurement of plasma cortisol, plasma dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate, and lymphocytic corticosteroid receptors. Endocrine 22(2):113–118 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Arwert LI, Deijen JB, Drent ML (2005a) The relation between insulin-like growth factor 1 levels and cognition in healthy elderly: A meta-analysis. Growth Horm IGF Res 15:416–422 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Arwert LI, Deijen JB, Muller M, Drent ML (2005b) Long-term growth hormone treatment preserves GH-induced memory and mood improvement: a 10-year follow-up study in GH-deficient adult men. Horm Behav 47:343–349 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Arwert LI, Deijen JB, Witlox J, Drent ML (2005c) The influence of growth hormone (GH) substitution on patient-reported outcomes and cognitive functions in GH-deficient patients: A meta analysis. Growth Horm IGF Res 15:47–54 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Arwert LI, Veltman DJ, Deijen JB, van Dam PS, Delemarre-van deWaal HA, Drent ML (2005d) Growth hormone deficiency and memory functioning in adults visualized by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroendocrinology 82(1):32–40 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Asthana S, Craft S, Baker LD, Raskind MA, Birnbaum RS, Lofgreen CP, Veith TC, Plymate SR (1999) Cognitive and neuroendocrine response to transdermal estrogen in postmenopausal women with Alzheimer’s disease: results of a placebo-controlled, double blind, pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 24(6):657–677 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Barrett-Conner E, Goodman-Gruen D (1999) Cognitive function and endogenous sex hormones in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc 47:1289–1293 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Bates KA, Harvey AR, Carruthers M, Martins RN (2005) Androgens, andropause and neurodegeneration: exploring the link between steroidogenesis, androgens and Alzheimer’s disease. Cell Mol Life Sci 62(3):281–292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Baum HB, Katznelson L, Sferman JC, Biller BM, Hayden DL, Schoenfeld DA, Cannistraro KE, Klibanski A (1998) Effects of physiological growth hormone (GH) therapy on cognition and quality of life in patients with adult-onset GH deficiency. J Clin Endocrin Metab 83(9):3184–3189 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Baxter RC, Martin JL (1986) Radioimmunoassay of growth hormone-dependent insulin-like growth factor binding protein in human plasma. J Clin Investigation 78:1504–1512 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Begleiter H, Porjesz B, Reich T, Edenberg JH, Goate A, Blangero J, Almasy L, Foriud T, Van Eerdewegh P, Polich J, Rohrbaugh J, Kuperman S, Bauer LO, O’Connor SJ, Chorlian DB, Li TK, Connelly PM, Hesselbrock V, Rice JP, Schuckit MA, Cloninger R, Nurnberger J Jr, Crowe R, Bloom FE (1998) Quantitative trait loci analysis of human event-related brain potentials: P3 voltage. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol 108:244–250 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Behl C, Manthey D (2000) Neuroprotective activities of estrogen: an update. J Neurocytology 29(5–6):351–358 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Behl C, Skutella T, Lezoualc’h F et al. (1997) Neuroprotection against oxidative stress by estrogens: structure–activity relationship. Mol Pharmacol 51:535–541 [PubMed]
- Bentler PM (1990) Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull 238–246 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Bentler PM, Bonett DG (1980) Significant tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol Bull 88:588–600
- Berton O, McClung CA, Dileone RJ, Krishnan V, Renthal W, Ruso SJ, Graham D, Tsankova NM, Belanos CA, Rios M, Monteggia LM, Self DW, Nestler EJ (2006) Essential role of BNDF in the mesolimbic dopamine PATHway in social defeat stress. Science 311:864–868 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Bicikova M, Ripova D, Hill M, Jirak R, Havlikova H, Tallova J, Hampl R (2004) Plasma levels of 7-hydroxylated dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) metabolites and selected amino-thiols as discriminatory tools of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Clin Chem Lab Med 42(5):518–524 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Binoux M (1999) GH, IGFs, IGF-binding protein-3 and acid-labile subunit: What is the pecking order? Eur J Endocrinol 137:805–809 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Bollen KA (1989) Statistical equations with latent variables. Wiley, New York
- Bowen RL, Smith MA, Harris PL, Kubat Z, Martins RN, Castellani RJ, Perry G, Atwood CS (2002) Elevated luteinizing hormone expression colocalized with neuron vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease pathology. J Neurosci Res 70(3):514–518 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Braverman ER, Blum K (1996) Substance use disorder exacerbates brain electrophysiological abnormalities in a psychiatrically-ill population. Clin Electroencephalogr 27(4):25–27 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Braverman ER, Blum K (2003) P300 (latency) event-related potential: an accurate predictor of memory impairment. Clin Electroencephalogr 34:124–139 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Braverman ER, Chen TJH, Schoolfield J, Mengucci JF, Blum SH, Downs BW, Meshkin B, Blum K (2005) Low plasma levels of sex hormones and human growth factor(s) correlate to cognitive decline as a function of gender. International Journal of Anti-Aging Medicine Annual Meeting, Las Vegas, NV, USA
- Braverman ER, Chen TJH, Schoolfield J, Martinez-Pons M, Gordon CA, Mengucci JF, Blum SH, Meshkin B, Downs BW, Blum K (2006) Delayed P300 latency correlates with abnormal test of variables of attention (TOVA) in adults and predicts early cognitive decline in a clinical setting. Adv ther 23:582–600 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Breltner JC, Zandi PP (2003) Effects of estrogen plus progestin on risk of dementia. JAMA 28:2651–2662 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Bremner WJ (2004) Testosterone tied to mental function. But questions about the benefits and risks of testosterone therapy persist. Health News 10(4):14–15 [PubMed]
- Brondu NE, Drake BL, Moser DR, Lin M, Boses M, Bar RS (1996) Regulation of endothelial IGF-BP3 synthesis and secretion by IGF-1 and TGF-beta. Growth Regul 6:1–9 [PubMed]
- Brooke AM, Monson JP (2003) Adult growth hormone deficiency. Clin Med 3:15–19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Brown RC, Han Z, Cascio C, Papadopoulos V (2003) Oxidative stress-mediated DHEA formation in Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Neurobiol Aging 24(1):57–65 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Brunso-Bechtold JK, Linville MC, Sonntag WE (2000) Age-related synaptic changes in sensorimotor cortex of the Brown Norway X fischer 344 rat. Brain Res 872:125–133 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Burkhardt MS, Foster JK, Laws SM, Baker LD, Craft S, Grandy SE, Stucky BG, Clarnette R, Nolan D, Hewson-Bower B, Martins RN (2004) Estrogen replacement therapy may improve memory functioning in the absence of APOE epsilon4. J Alzheimers Dis 6(3):221–228 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Burkhardt MS, Foster JK, Clarnette RM, Chubb SAP, Bruce DG, Drummond PD, Matins RN, Yeap BB (2006) Interaction between testosterone and apolipoprotein E4 status on cognition in healthy older men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 91:1168–1172 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Burman P, Deijen JB (1998) Quality of life and cognitive function in patients with pituitary insufficiency. Psychother Pyschosom 67:154–167 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Carro E, Trejo JL, Gomez-Isla T, LeRoith D, Torres-Aleman I (2002) Serum insulin-like growth factor 1 regulates brain amyloid-beta levels. Nat Med 12:1390–1397 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Casadesus G, Atwood CS, Zhu X, Hartzler AW, Webber KM, Perry G, Atwood CS, Zhu X, Hartzler AW, Webber KM, Perry G, Bowen RL, Smith MA (2005) Evidence for the role of gonadotropin hormones in the development of Alzheimer’s disease. Cell Mol Life Sci 62(3):293–298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Casadesus G, Webber KM, Atwood CS, Pappolla MA, Perry G, Bowen RL, Smith MA (2006) Luteinizing hormone modulates cognition and amyloid-beta deposition in Alzheimer APP transgenic mice. Biochim Biophysica Acta 1762(4):447-52 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cauley JA, Gutal JP, Kuller LH, LeDonne D, Powell JG (1989) The epidemiology of serum sex hormones on postmenopausal women. Am J Epidemiol 129:1120–1131 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cherrier MM et al. (2005) Testosterone improves spatial memory in men with Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 64(12):2063–2068 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cholerton B, Gleason CE, Baker LD, Asthana SW (2002) Estrogen and Alzheimer’s disease: The story so far. Drugs and Aging 19(6):405–427 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Comings DE, Dietz G, Johnson JP, MacMurray JP (1999) Association of the enkephalinase gene with low amplitude P300 waves. Neuroreport 10(11):2283–2285 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Compton J, van Amelsvoort T, Murphy D (2001) HRT and its effect on normal ageing of the brain and dementia. British J Clin Pharmacol 52(6):647–653 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Cyr M, Calon F, Morissette M, Grandbois M, Di Paolo T, Callier S (2000) Drugs with estrogen-like potency and brain activity: potential therapeutic application for the CNS. Cur Pharma Des 6(12):1287–1312 [DOI] [PubMed]
- De Bruin VM, Vieira MC, Rocha MN, Viana GS (2002) Cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate plasma levels and their relationship to aging, cognitive function, and dementia. Brain Cogn 50(2):316–323 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Demlin RH (2005) The role of anabolic hormones for wound healing in catabolic states. J Burns Wounds 4:2 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Donahue AN, Aschner M, Lash LH, Syversen T, Sonntag WE (2006) Growth hormone administration to aged animals reduces disulfide glutathione levels in hippocampus. Mech Aging Dev 127:57–63 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Dumas J, Hancur-Bucci C, Naylor M, Sites C, Newhouse P (2006) Estrogen treatment effects on anticholinergic-induced cognitive dysfunction in normal postmenopausal women. Neuropsychopharmacology [Epub ahead of Print] [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ellwart T, Rinck M, Becker ES (2003) Selective memory and memory deficits in depressed inpatients. Depress Anxiety 17:197–206 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Falter CM, Arroyo M, Davis GJ (2006) Testosterone: Activation or organization of spatial cognition? Biol Psych 73(2):132-140 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Gallinat J, Bajbouj M, Sander T, Schlattmann P, Xu K, Ferro EF, Goldman D, Winterer G (2003) Association of the G1947A COMT (Val(108/158)Met) gene polymorphism with prefrontal P300 during information processing. Biol Psych 54(1):40–48 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Gleason CE, Cholerton B, Carlsson CM, Johnson SC, Asthana S (2005) Neuroprotective effects of female sex steroids in humans: current controversies and future directions. Cell Mol Sci 62(3):299–312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Godbolt AK et al. (2004) The natural history of Alzheimer disease: A longitudinal presymptomatic and symptomatic study of a familial cohort. Arch Neurol 61:1743–1748 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Gregory CW, Bowen RL (2005) Novel therapeutic strategies for Alzheimer’s disease based on the forgotten reproductive hormones. Cell Mol Life Sci 62(3):313–319 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Grill JD, Sonntag WE, Riddle DR (2005) Dendritic stability in a model of adult-onset IGF-1 deficiency. Growth Horm IGF Res 15:337–348 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Heijer den T, Geerkings MI, Hofman A, de Jong FH, Launer LJ, Huibert AP, Pols MD, Breteler MM (2003) Higher estrogen levels are not associated with larger hippocampi and better memory performance. Arch Neurology 60:213–220 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Henderson VW, Hogervorst E (2004) Testosterone and Alzheimer’s disease: is it men’s turn now? Neurology 62(2):170–171 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Herrmann N, Lanctot KL, Eryavec G, Van Reekum R, Khan LR (2004) Growth hormone response to clonidine predicts aggression in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29(9):1192–1197 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hill SY, Locke J, Zezza N, Kaplan B, Neiswanger K, Steinhauer SR, Wipprecht G, Xu J (1998) Genetic association between reduced P300 amplitude and the DRD2 dopamine receptor A1 allele in children at high risk for alcoholism. Biol Psych 43(1):40–51 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hogervorst E, Bandelow S, Combrink M, Smith AD (2004) Low free testosterone is an independent risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. Exp Gerontol 39(11–12):1633–1639 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Honjo H, Iwasa K, Kawata M, Fushiki S, Hosoda T, Tatsumi H, Oida N, Mihara M, Hirasugi Y, Yamamoto H, Kikuchi N, Kitawaki J (2005) Progestins and estrogens and Alzheimer’s disease. Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 93(2–5):305–308 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hoskin EK, Tang MX, Manly JJ, Mayeux R (2004) Elevated sex-hormone binding globulin in elderly women with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 25(2):141–147 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Huppert Fa, Van Niekerk JK, Herbert J (2000) Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) supplementation for cognition and well-being. Cochrane Database of Systemic Review 2:CD000304 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Johnson JP, Muhleman D, MacMurray J, Gade R, Verde R, Ask M, Kelley J, Comings DE (1997) Association between the cannabinoid receptor gene (CNR1) and the P300 event-related potential. Mol Psych 169–171 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kalmijn S, Janssen JA, Pols HA, Lamberts SW, Breteler MM (2000a) A prospective study on circulating insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1), IGF-binding proteins, and cognitive function in the elderly. J Clin Endocrin Metab 85(12):4551–4555 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kalmijn S, Mehta KM, Pols HA, Horman A, Drexhage HA, Breteler MM (2000b) Subclinical hyperthyroidism and the risk of dementia. The Rotterdam study. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 53(6):733–737 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kappeler L, Epelbaum J (2005) Biological aspects of longevity and ageing. Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique 53:235–241 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kawas C, Resnick S, Morrison A et al. (1997) A prospective study of estrogen replacement therapy and the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neurology 48:1517–1521 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kawas CH, Corrada MM, Brookmeyer R, Morrison A, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB, Arenberg D (2003) Visual memory predicts Alzheimer’s disease more than a decade before diagnosis. Neurology 60(7):1089–1093 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kessing LV, Andersen PK (2004) Does the risk of developing dementia increase with the number of episodes in patients with depressive disorder and in patients with bipolar disorder? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75(12):1662–1666 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Khachaturian AS, Corcoran CD, Mayer LS, Zandi PP, Breitner JCS (2004) Apolipoprotein E 4 count affects age at onset of Alzheimer disease, but not lifetime susceptibility. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61:518–524 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Knopman D, Henderson VW (2003) DHEA for Alzheimer’s disease: a modest showing by a superhormone. Neurology 60(7):1060–1061 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kolsch H, Rao ML (2000) Neuroprotective effects of estradiol–17 beta: implications for psychiatric disorders. Arch Women’s Mental Health 5(3):105–110 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Langa KM, Norman L, Foster NL, Larson EB (2004) Mixed dementia: emerging concepts and therapeutic implications. JAMA 292(23):2901–2908 [DOI] [PubMed]
- LeBlance E, Janowsky J, Chan BKS, Nelson HD (2001) Hormone replacement therapy and cognition: systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA 285:1489–1499 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Leblhuber F, Haller H, Steiner K, Fuchs D (2004) DHEA treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: A randomized, double-blind, placebo controlled trial. Neurology 62(6):1030 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Leor J, Rozen L, Zuloff-Shani A, Feinberg MS, Amsalem Y, Barbash IM, Kachel E, Holbova R, Mardor Y, Daniels D, Ocherashvilli A, Orenstein A, Danon D (2006) Ex vivo activated human macrophages improve healing, remodeling, and function of the infarcted heart. Circulation 114(1Supple):94–100 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Levine AJ, Battista M (2004) Estrogen replacement therapy: effects on the cognitive functioning and clinical course of women with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 19(6):769–778 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Li JX, Liu XS, Tang H, Zhou X, Huang YS (2006) Influence of some topical antibiotics and FGF2, EGF and rhGH on the biological characteristics of fibroblasts in vitro. Zhonghua Shao Shang Za Zhi 1:33–37 [PubMed]
- Lichtenwalner RJ, Forbes ME, Bennett SA, Lynch CD, Sonntag WE, Riddle DR (2001) Intracerebroventricular infusion of insulin-like growth factor-I ameliorates the age-related decline in hippocampal neurogenesis. Neuroscience 107(4):603–613 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Lim D, Fisher L, Dharamarajan A, Martins RN (2003) Can testosterone replacement decrease the memory problem of old age? Med Hypotheses 60:893–896 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Lobie PE, Zhu T, Graichen R, Goh EL (2000) Growth hormone, insulin-like growth factor 1 and the CNS: localization, function and mechanism of action. Growth Horm IGF Res 10(suppl)B:S51–S56 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Loehlin JC (1992) Latent variable models: An introduction to factor, path, and structural analysis, 187. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
- Long JS (1983) Confirmatory factor analysis: A preface to LISREL. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA
- Lynch CD, Lyons D, Khan A, Bennett SA, Sonntag WE (2001) Insulin-like growth factor-1 selectively increases glucose utilization in brains of aged animals. Endocrinology 142:506–509 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Mahajan T, Crown A, Checkley S, Farmer A, Lightman S (2004) A typical depression in growth hormone deficient adults, and the beneficial effects of growth hormone treatment on depression and quality of life. Eur J Endcrinol 151:235–332 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Mangone CA (2004) Clinical heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s disease. Different clinical profiles can predict the progression rate. Rev Neurol 38(7):675–81 [PubMed]
- Manor I, Corbex M, Eisenberg J, Gritsenkso I, Bachner-Melman R, Tyano S, Ebstein RP (2004) Association of the dopamine D5 receptor with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and scores on a continuous performance test (TOVA). Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 127(1):73–77 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Maruyama GM (1997) Basics of structural equation modeling. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA
- Meethal SV, Smith MA, Bowen RL, Atwood CS (2003) The gonadotropin connection in Alzheimer's disease. Endocrinology 26:317–326 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Messier C, Awad N, Gagnon M (2004) The relationship between atherosclerosis, heart disease, type 2 diabetes and dementia. Neurol Res 26(5):567–572 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Modrego PJ, Ferrandez J (2004) Depression in patients with mild cognitive impairment increases the risk of developing dementia of Alzheimer type: a prospective cohort study. Arch Neurol 61(8):1290–1293 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Moffat SD, Zonderman AB, Metter EJ, Kawas C, Blackman MR, Harman SM, Resnick SM (2004) Free testosterone and risk for Alzheimer disease in older men. Neurology 62(2):188–193 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Morley JE (2003) Hormones and the aging process. J Amer Ger Soc 57:5333–5337 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Morely JE, van den Berg L (eds) (2000) Endocrinology of aging. The Humana Press, Totowa, NJ, USA
- Mulert C, Juckel G, Giegling I, Pogarell O, Leicht G, Karch S, Mavrogiorgou P, Moller HJ, Hegerl U, Rujescu D (2006) A Ser9Gly polymorphism in the dopamine D3 receptor gene (DRD3) and event-related P300 potentials. Neuropsychopharmacology 31(6):1335-1344 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Mulnard RA, Citman CW, Kawas C et al. (2000) For the Alzheimer’s disease cooperative Study. Estrogen replacement therapy for the treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimer disease: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 283:1007–1015 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Murialdo G, Barreca A, Nobili F, Rollero A, Timossi, G, Gianelli MV, Copello, F, Rodriquez G, Polleri A (2001) Relationships between cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate and insulin-like growth factor-1 system in dementia. J Endocrinol Invest 24:139–146 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Nacher V (2000) Genetic association between the reduced amplitude of the P300 and the allele A1 of the gene which codifies the D2 dopamine receptor (DRD2) as possible biological markers for alcoholism. Rev Neurol 30(8):756–763 [PubMed]
- Norbury R, Cutter WJ, Compton J, Robertson DM, Craig M, Whitehead KM, Murphy DG (2003) The neuroprotective effects of estrogen on the aging brain. Exp Gerontol 38:109–117 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Okereke OL, Kang JH, Ma J, Gazizno JM, Grodstein F (2006) Midlife plasma insulin-like growth factor 1 and cognitive function in older men. J Clin Endocrinilogy Metab 91:4306–4312 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Okun MS, DeLong MRJ, Hanfelt J, Gearing M, Levey A (2004) Plasma testosterone levels in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson diseases. Neurology 62(3):411–413 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Pandian R, Nakamoto JM (2004) Rational use of the laboratory for childhood and adult growth hormone deficiency. Clinical Lab Med 24:14–174 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Papadakis MA, Grady D, Black D, Tierney MJ, Gooding GA, Schambelan M, Grunfeld C (2005) Relationship between serum insulin-like growth factor-1 levels and Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 53(10):1748–1753 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Pavel ME, Lohmann T, Hahn EG, Hoffman M (2003) Impact of growth hormone on central nervous activity, vigilance, and tiredness after short-term therapy in growth hormone in deficient adults. Horm Metab Res 35(2):114–119 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Pennanen C, Laakso MP, Kivipelto M, Ramberg J, Soininen H (2004) Serum testosterone levels in males with Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuroendocrinol 16:93–4 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Petruzzi E, Pinzani P, Orlando C, Poggesi M, Monami M, Pazzagli M, Masotti G (2002) Healthy centenarian as living model of “successful aging”: A study on fasting glycemia, C-peptide, dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate (DHEAS) and insulin–like growth factor (IGF-1). Arch Gerontol Geriatr Suppl 8:273–278 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Pike CJ (2001) Testosterone attenuates beta-amyloid toxicity in cultured hippocampal neurons. Brain Res 919:160–165 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Pinkerton JV, Henderson VW (2005) Estrogen and cognition, with a focus on Alzheimer’s disease. Semin Reprod Med 23(2):172–179 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Polich J, Bloom FE (1999) P300, alcoholism heritability, stimulus modality. Alcohol 17:149–156 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Polleri A, Gianelli MV, Murialdo G (2002) Dementia: a neuroendocrine perspective. Endocrinol Invest 25(1):73–83 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ramsey MM, Weiner JL, Moore TP, Carter CS, Sonntag WE (2004) Growth hormone treatment attenuates age-related changes in hippocampal short-term plasticity and spatial learning. Neuroscience 129(1):119–127 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rasmuson S, Nasman B, Carlstrom K, Olsson T (2002) Increased levels of adrenocortical and gonadal hormones in mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 13(2):74–79 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ravaglia G, Forti P, Maiolo F, Sacchetti L, Nativio V, Scali CR, Mariani E, Zanardi V, Stefanini A, Macini PL (2002) Dehydroepiandrosterone-sulfate serum levels and common age-related disease: results from a cross-sectional Italian study of a general elderly population. Exp Gerontol 37(5):701–712 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Raynaud-Simon A, Lafont S, Berr C, Dartigues JF, Baulieu EE, Le Bouc Y (2000) Plasma insulin-like growth factor 1 levels in the elderly: relation to plasma dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate levels, nutritional status, health and mortality. Gerontology 47(4):198–206 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rollero A, Murialdo G, Fonzi S, Garrone S, Gianelli MV, Gazzerro E, Barreca A, Polleri A (1998) Relationship between cognitive function, growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor I plasma levels in aged subjects. Neuropsychobiology 38(2):73–79 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rosario E, Chang L, Stanczyk FZ, Pike CJ (2004) Age related testosterone depletion and the development of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 292(12):1431–1432 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sano M (2000) Understanding the role of estrogen on cognition and dementia. J Neural Trans 59:223–229 (Suppl) [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sherwin BB (2003) Estrogen and cognitive functioning in women. Endocrine Reviews 24(2):133–151 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Shi L, Linville MC, Tucker EW, Sonntag WE, Brunso-Bechtold JK (2005) Differential effects of aging and insulin-like growth factor-1 on synapses in CA1 of rat hippocampus. Cereb Cortex 15(5):571–577 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Shin EJ, Jhoo JH, Nabeshima T, Jhoo WK, Kwon MS, Lim YK, Chae JS, Jung ME, Park SJ, Jang KJ, Kim HC (2005) Growth hormone releaser attenuates beta-amyloid (1–42)–induced memory impairment in mice. J Pharmacol Sci 99:117–120 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Shively CA, Bethea CL (2004) Cognition, mood disorders, and sex hormones. ILAR 45:189–199 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Simpson AH, Mills L, Noble B (2006) The role of growth factors and related agents in accelerating fracture healing. J Bone Joint Sug Br 6:701–706 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Smith JD, Levin-Allerhand JA (2003) Potential use of estrogen-like drugs for the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease. J Mol Neurosci 20:277–781 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sodergard R, Backstrom T, Shanbhag V, Carstensen H (1982) Calculation of free and bound fractions of testosterone and estradiol-17 beta to human plasma proteins at body temperature. J Steroid Biochem 16:801–810 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sonntag WE, Lynch C, Thornton P, Khan A, Bennett S, Ingram R (2000) The effects of growth hormone and IGF-1 deficiency on cerebrovascular and brain ageing. J Anat 197(4):575–585 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Sonntag WE, Lynch CD, Cefalu WT, Ingram RL, Bennett SA, Thornton PL, Khan AS (2001) Pleiotropic effects of growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor (IGF)-1 on biological aging: inferences from moderate caloric-restricted animals. Neuroscience 107:603–613 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sonntag WE, Carter CS, Ikeno Y, Ekenstedt K, Carlson CS, Loeser RF, Chakrabarty S, Lee S, Bennett C, Ingram R, Moore T, Ramsey M (2005a) Adult-onset growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor I deficiency reduces neoplastic disease, modifies age-related pathology, and increases life span. Endocrinology 146(7):2920–2932 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sonntag WE, Ramsey M, Carter CS (2005b) Growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1) and their influence on cognitive aging. Ageing Res Rev 4:195–212 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Tan RS, Pu SJ (2001) The andropause and memory loss: is there a link between androgen decline and dementia in the aging male? Asian J Andrology 3(3):169–174 [PubMed]
- Tan RS, Pu SJ (2003) A pilot study on the effects of testosterone in hypogonadal aging male patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Aging Male 6:13–17 [PubMed]
- Tang MX, Jacibs D, Stern Y et al. (1996) Effect of estrogen during menopause on risk and age at onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 34:429–432 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Thornton PL, Ingram RL, Sonntag WE (2000) Chronic [D-Ala2]-growth hormone-releasing hormone administration attenuates age-related deficits in spatial memory. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 55:B106–B112 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Tilvis RS, Kahonen-Vare MH, Jolkkonen J, Valvanne J, Pitkala KH, Strandberg TE (2004) Predictors of cognitive decline and morality of aged people over a 10-year period. J Gerontol A Biol Sci 59(3):268–274 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Tschanz JT, Treiber K, Norton MC, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Toone L, Zandi PP, Szekely CA, Lyketsos C, Breitner JC, Cache County Study Group (2005) A population study of Alzheimer’s disease: findings from the Cache County study on memory, health and aging. Care Manag J 6(2):107–114 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Vallee M, Mayo W, LeMoal M (2001) Role of pregnenolone, dehydroepiandrosterone and their sulfate esters on learning and memory in cognitive aging. Brain Res Rev 37:301–312 [DOI] [PubMed]
- van Dam PS (2005) Neurocognitive function in adults with growth hormone deficiency. Horm Res 64(Suppl 3):109–114 [DOI] [PubMed]
- van Dam PS, Aleman A. (2004) Insulin-like growth factor-I, cognition and brain aging. Eur Pharmacol 490(1–3):87–95 [DOI] [PubMed]
- van Dam PS, de Winter CF, de Vries R, van der Grond J, Drent Ml, Lijffijt M, Kenemans JL, Aleman A, de Haan EH, Koppeshaar HP (2005) Childhood–onset growth hormone deficiency, cognitive function and brain N-acetylaspartate. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30(4):357–363 [DOI] [PubMed]
- van den Beid AW, de Jong FH, Grobbee DE, Pois HAP, Lamberts SW (2000) Measures of bioavailable serum testosterone and estradiol and their relationships with muscle strength, bone density, and body composition in elderly men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 85:3276–3282 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Wang PN, Liao SQ, Liu RD et al. (2000) Effects of estrogen on cognition, mood and cerebral blood flow in AD: a controlled study. Neurology 54:2061–2066 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Watanabe T, Yamamoto H, Idei T, Iguchi T, Katagiri T (2004) Influence of insulin-like growth factor-1 and hepatocyte growth factor on carotid atherosclerosis and cognitive function in the elderly. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 18(1):67–74 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Weill-Engerer S, David JP, Sazdovitch V, Liere P, Eychenne B, Pianos A, Schumacher M, Delacourte A, Baulieu EE, Akwa Y (2002) Neurosteroid quantification in human brain regions: comparison between Alzheimer’s and nondemented patients. J Chin Endocrinol Metab 87(11):5138–5143 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Whitmer RA, Sidney S, Selby J, Johnston SC, Yaffe K (2005) Midlife cardiovascular risk factors and risk of dementia in late life. Neurology 64(2):277–281 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Wolkowitz OM, Kramer JH, Reus VI (2003) DHEA treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Neurology 6:1071–1076 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Yai JL, Rasmuson S, Andrew R, Graham M, Noble J, Olsson T, Fuchs E, Lathe R, Seckl JR (2003) Dehydroepiandrosterone 7-hydroxylase CYP7B: predominant expression in primate hippocampus and reduced expressions in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscience 121(2):307–314 [DOI] [PubMed]