A particularly interesting property of many glutamatergic synapses is that their strength can be bidirectionally modified; some patterns of stimulation make synapses stronger while others make them weaker. It is generally thought that these dual processes, referred to as long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD), respectively, contribute to memory storage. Understanding the biochemical events that determine whether the synapse gets stronger or weaker has been an important goal.
It now appears likely that both LTP and LTD are triggered by the same second messenger, Ca2+, and that what determines whether LTP or LTD occurs is the level of Ca2+. Early experiments by Lynch et al. (1983) showed that LTP induction could be blocked by intracellular Ca2+ chelators, suggesting that an activity-dependent elevation of intracellular Ca2+ triggers LTP. But which second messenger triggers LTD? The proposal that LTD is also triggered by Ca2+ (Lisman, 1989) was based on the following line of reasoning. One form of LTD that works particularly well in vivo is a heterosynaptic process; the LTP induced by activating one set of synapses induces LTD (technically termed depotentiation) at inactive synapses. The weakened synapses can be quite distant, making it unlikely that what carries the message to them is a diffusible substance. It seemed more plausible that the message is carried by the spread of the depolarization produced by the active synapses. This depolarization could produce Ca2+ elevation at distant synapses by activating voltage-dependent Ca2+ channels. The resulting Ca2+ elevation would be smaller than at active synapses where Ca2+ entry through NMDA channels is a second source of Ca2+. This line of reasoning suggests that high Ca2+ elevation might trigger LTP, whereas more moderate Ca2+ elevation might trigger LTD.
But how could different Ca2+ levels produce opposite effects? The following scheme (Lisman, 1989) was built on previous ideas about how the Ca2+-activated kinase, CaMKII, might act as a molecular switch that was turned ‘on’ during LTP induction and that then strengthened the synapse (Miller & Kennedy, 1986; Lisman & Goldring, 1988). It was suggested that synaptic weakening might occur if moderate Ca2+ elevation activated a phosphatase cascade (involving calcineurin and protein phosphatase-1 (PP1)) and dephosphorylated CaMKII. In contrast, it was supposed that at the higher Ca2+ levels that occur during LTP induction, CaMKII would be switched to the ‘on’ state both because this kinase is itself activated by Ca2+ and because these higher Ca2+ levels would inactivate PP1 (through Ca2+-dependent activation of adenylate cyclase and the known ability of PKA to inhibit PP1).
These hypotheses have received increasing experimental support. Early work by Mulkey & Malenka (1992) demonstrated that the weakening processes, like the strengthening processes, could be blocked by intracellular Ca2+ buffers. More recent work using the release of Ca2+ from caged compounds (Yang et al. 1999) demonstrated that Ca2+ is sufficient to trigger both LTP and LTD. Furthermore, there has come to be general agreement that LTP involves the activation of kinase processes whereas LTD involves activation of phosphatase processes. But what about the more specific idea that it is different levels of Ca2+ that determine whether LTP or LTD occurs? The first support for this came when it was shown that the same stimulus which induces LTP would instead induce LTD if the NMDA channels were partially blocked by an antagonist (Cummings et al. 1996). Presumably this lowered the Ca2+ elevation from the LTP zone to the LTD zone. A similar conversion of LTP to LTD can be achieved by simply damping Ca2+ elevation with moderate concentrations of Ca2+ buffers. For instance, in this issue of The Journal of Physiology, Cho et al. (2001) show that a stimulus that normally induces LTP will induce LTD if the intracellular Ca2+ buffer concentration is raised to 10 mm. Finally, the uncaging experiments mentioned above provided direct evidence that the level of Ca2+ can be an important determinant of the sign of synaptic modification.
The most surprising finding of Cho et al. (2001) suggests that there may actually be three important Ca2+ levels. Specifically, their data suggest a large ‘no man's land’ between the LTP and LTD Ca2+ zones. They found that elevating buffer concentration above normal can convert a stimulus from one that does not induce LTD to one that does. The most likely interpretation is that the stimulus normally elevates [Ca2+] into the ‘no man's land’ in which neither LTP nor LTD occurs. Apparently, this low frequency stimulation at -70 mV, which is considered a weak form of stimulation, is sufficient to raise [Ca2+] to this level. Presumably adding Ca2+ buffer leads to LTD induction because it lowers the [Ca2+] into the LTD zone. The existence of such a no man's land has important implications. It is well known that LTD can be finicky and there have been major controversies regarding whether it can or cannot be induced under given conditions (age, temperature, stress, in vivo vs. in vitro). Experimenters must now consider the possibility that failure to induce LTD may be because the Ca2+ elevation is so high that it reaches the no man's land.
Cho et al. (2001) make an advance in understanding what is occurring in no man's land. One possibility is that the kinase and phosphatase are balanced at this intermediate Ca2+ level and that this balance explains why there is no synaptic modification. Alternatively this Ca2+ level may be sufficient to inactivate the phosphatase but not sufficient to activate the kinase. Cho et al. (2001) provide evidence against the balance idea by showing that inhibiting the kinase does not lead to weakening. This conclusion must be considered preliminary since there are feedback loops by which Ca2+ entry and release mechanisms can themselves be affected by kinase inhibitors and buffers. However, their results are consistent with the increasing evidence that there is a Ca2+-activated adenylate cyclase at synapses and a demonstrable importance of PKA-dependent inactivation of PP1 (Blitzer et al. 1998). Interestingly, the enzymatic control system necessary for this regulation is localized to the postsynaptic density by scaffolding proteins. Thus, the picture beginning to emerge is that detection of different Ca2+ levels and the transduction of these signals into bidirectional synaptic modification are done by a biochemical computation integral to the synapse.
References
- Blitzer RD, Connor JH, Brown GP, Wong T, Shenolikar S, Iyengar R, Landau EM. Science. 1998;280:1940–1942. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5371.1940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho K, Aggleton JP, Brown MW, Bashir ZI. Journal of Physiology. 2001;532:459–466. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0459f.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummings JA, Mulkey RM, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC. Neuron. 1996;16:825–833. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80102-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 1989;86:9574–9578. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.23.9574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman JE, Goldring MA. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 1988;85:5320–5324. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.14.5320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch G, Larson J, Kelso S, Barrioneuvo G, Schottler F. Nature. 1983;305:719–721. doi: 10.1038/305719a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller SG, Kennedy MB. Cell. 1986;44:861–870. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(86)90008-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulkey RM, Malenka RC. Neuron. 1992;9:967–975. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90248-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang SN, Tang YG, Zucker RS. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;81:781–787. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.2.781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]