Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Apr 21.
Published in final edited form as: Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007 Nov;8(11):872–883. doi: 10.1038/nrn2154

Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new perspective

Larry R Squire *,‡,§,, John T Wixted , Robert E Clark *,
PMCID: PMC2323975  NIHMSID: NIHMS42384  PMID: 17948032

Abstract

Recognition memory is widely viewed as consisting of two components, recollection and familiarity, which have been proposed to be dependent on the hippocampus and the adjacent perirhinal cortex, respectively. Here, we propose an alternative perspective: we suggest that the methods traditionally used to separate recollection from familiarity instead separate strong memories from weak memories. A review of work with humans, monkeys and rodents finds evidence for familiarity signals (as well as recollection signals) in the hippocampus and recollection signals (as well as familiarity signals) in the perirhinal cortex. We also indicate ways in which the functions of the medial temporal lobe structures are different, and suggest that these structures work together in a cooperative and complementary way.


Declarative memory refers to the capacity to consciously remember the past and depends on the integrity of the medial temporal lobe (comprising the hippocampus, the dentate gyrus and the subicular complex, together with the entorhinal, perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices, which lie along the adjacent parahippocampal gyrus). One of the most widely studied examples of declarative memory is recognition — the ability to judge a recently encountered item as having been presented previously. Recognition memory is widely viewed as consisting of two components: recollection and familiarity1,2. Recollection involves remembering specific contextual details about a prior learning episode; familiarity involves simply knowing that an item was presented, without having available any additional information about the learning episode.

Interest in this distinction greatly increased when Brown and Aggleton3 proposed a neuroanatomical basis for these two processes. Their proposal was that recollection depends on the hippocampus, whereas familiarity depends on the adjacent perirhinal cortex. Since that time, others have elaborated on this idea46, and it has become the basis for the design and analysis of a good deal of experimental work. However, alternative formulations have also been advanced about the nature of recognition memory and its anatomical foundations7,8, and a consensus has not yet emerged.

Here, we review studies of humans, monkeys and rodents that have investigated the functional organization of the medial temporal lobe using a variety of methods: lesions, single-unit activity and neuroimaging. The general organizational principles of the medial temporal lobe are similar in these species9, and it is reasonable to suppose that what is learned about medial temporal lobe function in one species has relevance to the others. Although many of the studies have been interpreted as providing support for the neuroanatomical separation of recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe, in this Review we develop an alternative perspective. We suggest that there has been a rush to judgment about the anatomical substrate of recognition memory, and that the available findings can be more simply interpreted in terms of strong and weak memories. We conclude that the structures of the medial temporal lobe operate in a more cooperative fashion than has been envisioned in recent discussions of recollection versus familiarity and the hippocampus versus perirhinal cortex.

Studies of memory impairment

Recall and recognition deficits

Perhaps the most straightforward way to test the divided-labour account of medial temporal lobe function is to assess the degree to which recall and recognition are impaired in patients with circumscribed damage to the hippocampus. Recall is generally thought to depend solely on recollection, whereas recognition is thought to depend on both recollection and familiarity1,2. Accordingly, if the hippocampus selectively supports recollection, then hippocampal damage should impair recall more than recognition. Although a few single-case studies have been advanced in support of this idea1013, group studies have provided strong evidence against it. One study was carried out with 56 patients who had suffered a brief period of hypoxia and who were thought to have lesions limited to the hippocampus, though radiological data were not available14. Although recall was initially reported to be more impaired than recognition, this conclusion depended entirely on the aberrant performance of a single one of the 55 control subjects15. This individual’s recognition memory score was more than 4.4 standard deviations below the mean score of the controls, which markedly increased the variability of the control scores. Once the obvious outlier was excluded from the analysis, recall and recognition were similarly impaired. Indeed, the P value that resulted from comparing the recall and recognition deficits in the patients shifted from 0.027 to >0.50.

A different study16 involved six patients with evidence of bilateral hippocampal damage and normal parahippocampal gyrus volumes. Again, recall and recognition were similarly impaired — a result that should not be observed if recollection is exclusively dependent on the hippocampus. A third study17 involved three patients with damage limited to the hippocampus and two patients with damage that extended into the parahippocampal gyrus. Recognition performance in patients and controls was first equated by manipulating the participants’ exposure time to the items to be remembered. Under these conditions, the recall scores of the patients matched those of the controls across three different retention intervals (30 seconds, 2 minutes and 10 minutes): that is, recall and recognition were similarly impaired. This result strongly suggests that the hippocampus is important for both recollection and familiarity.

Prior suggestions that hippocampal lesions disproportionately affect recall were based on a few single-case studies1013 and on the aforementioned group study that was compromised by the inclusion of an outlier14. When that group study (without the outlier) is considered along with the two other available group studies16,17, a consistent picture emerges in which recall and recognition are similarly impaired in patients with hippocampal lesions.

Source memory and associative memory

Source memory and associative recognition procedures have also been used to assess the status of recollection and familiarity in patients with hippocampal lesions. These procedures have also been characterized as providing tests of ‘relational memory’ — a term originally used as an alternate term for declarative memory18,19, but which has recently tended to refer more specifically to recollection5,20,21. In a source-memory procedure, studied items are presented from different sources (for example, the top or bottom of a screen). On the subsequent recognition test, subjects are first asked for an old–new decision and then asked to indicate the item’s source. The old–new decision is assumed to be based on both recollection and familiarity, but the source judgment is assumed to rely only on recollection. In an associative recognition task, subjects are typically presented with pairs of items and later asked to discriminate intact pairs from recombined pairs. Because the items of both pairs are familiar, successful discrimination between the intact and recombined pairs presumably depends only on recollection.

In a study of source memory, five patients with lesions thought to be limited to the hippocampus (based on quantitative analysis of magnetic resonance images) were impaired in both old–new decisions and source judgments22. When controls and patients were matched on old–new memory performance (by using longer study lists for the controls), source memory performance was also comparable for the two groups. Thus, as with the recall and recognition studies that were discussed above, this result suggests that recollection and familiarity are similarly impaired in patients with hippocampal lesions. Another study that used an associative recognition procedure arrived at similar conclusions23. However, other associative recognition studies have reported that recollection is differentially impaired after hippocampal lesions (for example, see REF. 24), and still others have produced both outcomes by manipulating seemingly minor details of the experimental procedure25. Further work is needed to clarify the effects of hippocampal lesions on associative-recognition performance.

Delayed non-matching to sample and novel object recognition

The effects of selective hippocampal lesions have also been explored using tasks of recognition memory where single items must be remembered independently of any context, and where one might therefore suppose that performance depends substantially on familiarity. The most widely used tasks are delayed non-matching to sample and novel object recognition (BOX 1). Studies of humans, monkeys and rodents with selective hippocampal lesions document impairment in these tasks. The impairment is more severe when the hippocampal damage occurs together with damage to adjacent cortices, including perirhinal cortex26. Nonetheless, these studies indicate that the ability to remember a single item across a delay of more than just a few minutes depends substantially on the hippocampus, even when the task has no overtly associative or contextual component. The table in BOX 1 lists the considerable evidence for this conclusion, but also identifies instances in which no impairment was detected. Important factors that might explain the discrepancies include variability in lesion size or delay interval, along with the possibility (which applies to many of the behavioural assays that are given to experimental animals) that animals can approach some tasks in fundamentally different ways (for example, by using either declarative or non-declarative memory). Nevertheless, these data show that the effects of hippocampal lesions on these tasks should not be minimized4,5,27.

Box 1 | tasks used to assess recognition memory across species.

The effects of selective hippocampal lesions have often been explored in animals using tasks that require recognition memory. The most widely used tasks in this regard are delayed non-matching to sample tasks and novel object recognition tasks. The delayed non-matching to sample task requires that the subjects first be trained to learn the non-matching rule, whereas the novel object recognition task simply exploits an animal’s innate preference for novelty. The table lists 24 studies in which performance was impaired by hippocampal lesions and 7 studies in which performance was unimpaired.

Delayed non-matching to sample

A sample object is presented and then, after a delay that ranges from a few seconds to a few minutes, the sample object is presented again together with a new object. A choice of the new object is rewarded.

Novel object recognition

Two identical objects are presented for visual inspection (in the case of humans or monkeys) or exploration (in the case of rodents). After a delay that ranges from a few seconds to several hours, the original object is presented again together with a new (novel) object. Normally humans and other animals preferentially look at (or explore) the novel object, indicating that they remember the original object.

Species Studies in which hippocampal lesions impaired performance Studies in which hippocampal lesions did not impair performance
Delayed non-matching to sample
Humans 97
Monkeys 98–102 103
Rats 104–107 108,109
Novel object recognition
Humans 110,111
Monkeys 101,102
Rats or mice 112–115,116*,117–119,120,121 122–124,125§
*

Impairment when the lesion was made after the familiarization phase but not when the lesion was made before the familiarization phase.

Complicated by the fact that the lesion group explored the objects less than the control group during the initial familiarization phase.

§

No impairment under standard conditions but impairment when the test box was novel.

Analysis of the receiver operating characteristic

A new approach to investigating the neuroanatomical basis of recollection and familiarity involves analysing the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) in recognition-memory tasks. An ROC is a plot of the hit rate (the rate at which old items are correctly identified as old) versus the false-alarm rate (the rate at which new items are incorrectly identified as old) for decisions made with different levels of confidence (FIG. 1). Analyses of ROC curves from recognition-memory tasks have historically been associated with signal-detection theory28. As illustrated in FIG. 1, signal-detection theory holds that each old item (or target) or new item ( foil) is associated with a particular memory strength, which reflects the degree of certainty that an item did or did not appear on a recently presented list. Figure 1 shows that the distributions of memory strength for targets and foils overlap. An item is declared to be old if its memory strength exceeds some criterion value; otherwise it is declared to be new. According to traditional signal-detection theory, recognition decisions are based on a single strength variable, but the theory is also fully compatible with two-component views of recognition on the assumption that recollection and familiarity are both continuous processes that combine to determine the memory strength of a test item8,29.

Figure 1. Signal-detection theory and the receiver operating characteristic.

Figure 1

aThe signal-detection representation128 of a strong memory condition. The targets (old items) and foils (new items) in a recognition memory test are presumed to have varying degrees of memory strength (the subjective certainty that an item was or was not previously presented), and the mean and variance of the target distribution are greater than those of the foil distribution. A test item that generates a memory strength that exceeds a criterion value (indicated by the vertical line labelled c) is declared to be ‘old’. Otherwise, the test item is declared to be ‘new’. Confidence ratings (indicated by vertical dashed lines) of one to six range from ‘sure new’ to ‘sure old’. Items with memory strength to the left of the left-most vertical line are given ‘high confidence new’ responses. Items with memory strength to the right of the right-most vertical line are given ‘high confidence old’ responses. The right-hand panel shows a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve constructed from a subject’s confidence ratings. Five pairs of hit and false-alarm rates are computed from the six-point confidence rating scale. The left-most point represents the hit and false-alarm rates for targets and foils that receive a rating of 6. The second point represents the proportion of targets and foils that receive a rating of either 5 or 6, and so on. As is almost always the case in strong memory conditions129, the resulting ROC curve in this example is asymmetrical. b. The signal-detection representation of a weak memory condition, in which the means and variances of the target and foil distributions are more similar and the ROC curve is more symmetrical.

A more recently formulated two-component theory that also describes ROC data reasonably well holds that recollection occurs only when the strength of an item in memory exceeds a high threshold and that familiarity is a signal-detection process30. That is, recollection is conceptualized as a discontinuous variable and is assumed to occur with a discrete probability. Items that are strongly associated with the experimental context during learning have a higher probability of being recollected than items that are encoded in isolation. As illustrated in FIG. 2, when an item generates recollection of its prior occurrence, the subject is assumed to make a ‘high confidence old’ decision. Alternatively, when an item fails to generate recollection, the subject is assumed to make an old–new decision based only on familiarity, with the level of confidence determined by the item’s degree of familiarity. Thus, the high threshold/signal-detection model assumes that individual recognition decisions are based either on recollection or on familiarity, not on a combination of the two. In this model, the target and foil distributions are assumed to have equal variance.

Figure 2. High threshold/signal-detection theory and the receiver operating characteristic.

Figure 2

aThe high threshold/signal-detection representation30 of a strong memory condition that involves both recollection and familiarity. Recollection occurs with some discrete probability (R; in this example, R = 0.5). When a test item generates recollection, a ‘high confidence old’ decision (which is equivalent to a confidence rating of six on a six-point scale) is made. When a test item fails to generate recollection (a situation that occurs with probability equal to 1 − R), the decision is based on familiarity. Decisions based on familiarity are characterized by a signal-detection model in which the targets (the old items) and foils (the new items) are presumed to have different average levels of familiarity but equivalent variances. A test item that generates a familiarity value exceeding a criterion value (indicated by the solid vertical line labelled c) is declared to be ‘old’. Otherwise, the test item is declared to be ‘new’. Thus, whereas most confidence ratings of six are based on recollection in this example (the 50% of the targets that are recollected receive this rating), a few additional ratings of six are based on familiarity. The right-hand panel shows the predicted asymmetrical ROC curve, which is similar to the asymmetrical ROC curve predicted by the traditional signal-detection model when memory is strong (FIG. 1a). b. The high threshold/signal-detection representation of a condition in which recollection is not involved (that is, in which R = 0) but familiarity can be used to discriminate targets and foils. The right-hand panel shows the predicted symmetrical ROC curve, which is similar to the symmetrical ROC curve predicted by the traditional signal-detection model when memory is weak (FIG. 1b). Although the high-threshold/signal-detection model fits ROC data reasonably well, recent findings favour the traditional signal-detection model3437.

A number of studies that used the high-threshold/signal-detection model to interpret ROC data have concluded that hippocampal lesions selectively impair recollection14,3133. The key finding is that the asymmetry of the ROC curve is greater for control subjects than for patients with hippocampal lesions. According to the high threshold/signal-detection model, familiarity-based responses (those given in the absence of recollection) would yield a symmetrical ROC curve, because the target and foil familiarity distributions have equal variance. Any recollection-based responses are assumed to support decisions made with high confidence, and those additional high-confidence decisions distort the shape of the ROC curve (making it asymmetrical)30. Accordingly, the more symmetrical ROC curve produced by amnesic patients is thought to indicate that their recognition performance is based more on familiarity than is the recognition performance of controls. Indeed, the familiarity-based performance of amnesic patients is often estimated to be unimpaired in these studies.

In contrast to this view, traditional signal detection theory, which has in recent years been strongly supported over the high threshold/signal-detection model3437, suggests that a symmetrical ROC curve reflects weak memory rather than the absence of recollection (FIG. 1). Further, signal-detection theory holds that an asymmetrical ROC curve implies only that the target and foil distributions have unequal variance, which is generally a sign of a strong memory. It does not imply that recognition is supported by recollection.

The idea that the target distribution typically has greater variance than the foil distribution is easily understood. The targets can be thought of as foils that have had memory strength added to them as a result of having appeared on a study list. The target distribution would have the same variance as the foil distribution if each item on the list had the same amount of strength added to it during study. However, it is more plausible to suppose that the amount of added strength differs across items. In this case, the target distribution would shift to the right, its variance would increase, and the ROC curve would become more asymmetrical. Typical ROC curves are asymmetrical, and they become more asymmetrical as overall memory strength increases38 (FIG. 3). These considerations imply that the symmetrical ROC curves produced by patients with hippocampal lesions simply reflect the fact that their memories are weaker (in both recollection and familiarity).

Figure 3. Roc data as a function of memory strength.

Figure 3

In the study depicted, young adults (in groups of 19–24) studied 50-item word lists and then took a recognition-memory test involving the 50 old words and 50 new words after one of five retention intervals. The ROC curve was asymmetrical after the shortest retention interval (1 hour) and became more symmetrical as the interval grew and performance decreased (from 83% correct at 1 hour to 53% correct at 8 weeks). Performance was significantly above chance after all retention intervals. These findings differ from a previous study in rats32 which suggested that the ROC curve might be linear after a relatively long retention interval (75 minutes). The ROC data illustrated here are better fit by a curvilinear function based on signal-detection theory. Figure reproduced, with permission, from REF. 41 © (2006) Elsevier Science.

If the traditional signal-detection interpretation of ROC data is correct, then the alternative high threshold/signal-detection model30 misidentifies strong and weak memories as memories that are based on recollection and familiarity, respectively. Although it is likely that, on average, recollection contributes greater strength to an item’s memory than does familiarity, the defining characteristic of recollection and familiarity is not their relative strength. Indeed, one can experience a strong sense of familiarity even in the absence of recollection1. Further, although strong memories are often accompanied by recollection, considerable evidence indicates that some degree of recollection often accompanies weaker memories as well36,39,40.

These differing interpretations of the ROC evidence were tested in a recent study41 of recognition memory in patients with selective hippocampal lesions. The ROC curve for the patients was closer to the diagonal line (reflecting weaker memory; see FIG. 1b) and was more symmetrical than the ROC curve of matched controls, thereby replicating prior results. Notably, however, when the memory strength of the patients was increased to a level near that of controls (by using a shorter study list), the patient ROC curve became just as asymmetrical as the control ROC curve. The signal-detection model8,29 and the high threshold/signal-detection model30 offer compatible interpretations of this result.

According to the signal-detection account, the symmetrical ROC curve for the patients in the weak memory condition reflects the fact that the target and foil distributions have equal variance (as in FIG. 1b), precisely because memory is weak (in both recollection and familiarity). The asymmetrical ROC curve for the patients in the strong memory condition reflects the fact that the target distribution has greater variance than the foil distribution (as in FIG. 1a), precisely because memory is strong (in both recollection and familiarity). This result suggests that recollection and familiarity combine in typical fashion in the strong memory condition. That is, both recollection and familiarity are operative in the presence of hippocampal lesions.

According to the high threshold/signal-detection model30, the fact that the patient ROC curve in the strong memory condition was as asymmetrical as that of the controls implies that recollection and familiarity were the same for both groups (FIG. 2a). Quantitative mRI analysis performed on five of the six patients indicated an average bilateral reduction in hippocampal volume of 44%. On the basis of two other patients with similar bilateral hippocampal volume loss, for whom detailed postmortem neurohistological information was obtained42, this degree of volume loss reflects nearly complete loss of hippocampal neurons. If the five patients in the study also had complete loss of hippocampal neurons, then the high threshold/signal-detection model would predict a symmetrical ROC curve even in the strong memory condition. Further, even if some degree of hippocampal function was retained in the patients, and if the hippocampus selectively supports recollection, then the ROC curve in the strong memory condition should still have been less asymmetrical (reflecting partially impaired recollection) than the ROC curve of controls. Instead, the patient and control ROC curves were equally asymmetrical. Thus, according to both models, the ROC curves that were observed for the patients suggest that the component processes of recognition memory, including recollection, are operative in the absence of the hippocampus.

This finding differs from an earlier study in rats32. In agreement with studies of memory-impaired patients14,41, the ROC curve calculated for rats with hippocampal lesions was symmetrical (reflecting either weak memory or familiarity-based responding, depending on which theory is used to interpret the result). However, in contrast to the just-discussed study in patients41, the ROC curve for control rats with weakened memory (following a long retention interval) was nearly linear. The linear ROC, which is inconsistent with signal-detection theory and which the high threshold/signal-detection model interprets to reflect purely recollection-based responding30, was the critical result that led to the conclusion that the hippocampus is necessary for recollection to occur. Yet there is reason to view the linear old–new ROC cautiously, as it has never been observed in the human literature (see FIG. 3).

Analysis of remember–know judgments

Other studies have used the remember–know procedure to investigate the neuroanatomical basis of recollection and familiarity. In this procedure, subjects are asked to judge whether an item is old or new, and then for each item judged to be old they are asked whether they remember the item (recollection) or simply know that the item has been presented (familiarity). Several studies using this procedure have concluded that hippocampal lesions impair recollection but impair familiarity to either a lesser degree or not at all1214,31,43,44. However, these conclusions depend entirely on the assumption that subjective judgments of ‘remember’ and ‘know’ do indeed reflect recollection and familiarity, respectively. This assumption is based on the finding that behavioural manipulations thought to selectively influence recollection often affect only ‘remember’ responses, whereas manipulations thought to selectively influence familiarity affect only ‘know’ responses (for a review, see REF. 45).

Signal-detection theory interprets remember–know judgments differently 4648 and readily accounts for these same findings (FIG. 4). Specifically, a large reduction in ‘remember’ responses, together with little or no reduction in ‘know’ responses, happens naturally when strong memories become weak memories (for example, as time passes after learning). This occurs because ‘remember’ responses are associated with items in the right-hand tail of the target distribution (the strong memories that fall above some high decision criterion), whereas ‘know’ responses reflect a segment of the target distribution that falls below the high criterion but above some lower criterion. As the mean of the target distribution decreases (that is, as memory weakens), the proportion of targets that fall above the high criterion decreases rapidly, whereas the segment of the target distribution that falls between the high and low criteria remains largely unchanged. Accordingly, a differential loss of ‘remember’ responses should not imply a specific deficit in recollection. Indeed, there is considerable evidence that remember–know judgments index memory strength and are not reliable markers of qualitatively different processes, such as recollection and familiarity4650.

Figure 4. The signal-detection interpretation of remember–know judgments.

Figure 4

aA strong memory condition. ‘Remember’ judgments are made for items that exceed a high memory-strength criterion (labelled R), whereas ‘know’ judgments are made for items that exceed a lower criterion (labelled K) but not the high criterion. Items that fall below the K criterion are judged new. The ‘remember’ hit rate is the proportion of the target distribution that exceeds the R criterion (representing strong memories), and the ‘know’ hit rate is the proportion of the target distribution that falls between the K and R criteria (representing weak memories). b. In a weaker memory condition (as might occur, for example, after less extensive training), both criteria shift to the left, with the K criterion remaining approximately midway between the means of the target and foil distributions (its typical location for an unbiased subject). As a result, the ‘remember’ hit rate decreases, and the ‘remember’ false-alarm rate increases. This illustration explains why memory-impaired patients with hippocampal lesions often have a high ‘remember’ false-alarm rate compared with controls16. In contrast to the ‘remember’ hit rate, the ‘know’ hit rate can actually increase in a weak memory condition, as illustrated in this example. If estimates of recollection and familiarity were derived from data like these (as they often are), such estimates would suggest that recollection is greatly reduced in the weak memory condition whereas familiarity is relatively unaffected. However, a simpler explanation of data like these is that memory is weaker overall.

Single-unit recording and neuroimaging

Additional evidence that bears on the neuroanatomical basis of recollection and familiarity is provided by studies that have recorded neural activity in the hippocampus and perirhinal cortex during learning and retrieval. Two different techniques have been used: single-unit neurophysiology and functional MRI (fmRI).

Neural activity in the hippocampus

There is strong support for the idea that the hippocampus has a role in associative recollection. For example, in one study, monkeys learned rapidly to form arbitrary associations between complex visual scenes and one of four spatial locations51. Of 89 hippocampal cells that responded selectively to scenes, 25 signalled new learning with changes in firing rate that closely paralleled behavioural learning. Similar results have come from human neuro-imaging studies that used source-memory procedures to identify neural correlates of recollection. Typically, items that are recognized as old together with their correct source information (which requires recollection) are associated with increased activity in the hippocampus, both at encoding22,5254 and retrieval5557, relative to the activity associated with misses, or relative to the activity associated with correct item judgments when the source information is incorrect.

Other evidence comes from studies that made use of the idea that recollection-based decisions can be identified by ‘remember’ responses or by ‘old’ decisions made with high confidence30,58. Several studies have found that hippocampal activity is increased for such responses, relative to either ‘know’ responses or to ‘old’ decisions made with low confidence (or, in some cases, relative to misses)5964.

Although there is broad agreement about the role of the hippocampus in recollection, its role in familiarity is debated. Some studies appear to support the idea that familiarity does not involve the hippocampus. For example, in neuroimaging studies of source memory in which brain activity was recorded during encoding, items that are later correctly recognized as ‘old’ but with incorrect source information (that is, responses that are assumed to be familiarity-based) are often not associated with increased hippocampal activity relative to items that are later missed (for examples, see REFS 52,53, but see REF. 22 for an exception). It is generally assumed that such responses are based on familiarity, because the task-relevant source information was not recollected (although one cannot exclude the possibility that some other source information about the item was recollected). In addition, several remember–know studies have found that hippocampal activity at encoding for items that will later be given ‘know’ responses often fails to exceed the activity associated with items that will later be missed (for examples, see REFS 59,65, but see REF. 66 for an exception). A similar pattern is evident for ‘know’ responses (or for decisions made with low confidence) at retrieval6064,67. If these recognition responses are presumed to reflect familiarity-based decisions, such a result would imply that the hippocampus is not involved in familiarity4,5.

However, as described above, we propose that the methods intended to separate recollection-based decisions from familiarity-based decisions, including remember–know judgments, high or low confidence ratings and the presence or absence of source recollection, instead separate strong memories from weak memories. We suggest that strong memories are associated with increased hippocampal activity, regardless of whether they reflect strong familiarity, strong recollection or a combination of the two. Further, weak memories are often not associated with detectably increased activity in the hippocampus, regardless of whether such memories reflect weak familiarity, weak recollection or a weak combination of the two. Accordingly, the failure to detect increased hippocampal activity for items that are recognized only in association with a ‘know’ response, or only with low confidence or without correct source information could have more to do with the failure to detect weak memory than with the presence or absence of recollection. An implication of this view is that the typical relationship between memory strength and neural activity in the hippocampus (as measured by fmRI) is nonlinear (FIG. 5).

Figure 5. Characteristic nonlinear relationships between fMri activity and memory strength.

Figure 5

aIn the hippocampus, the relationship between functional MRI (fMRI) activity and memory strength at encoding is such that there is often a relatively steep increase in activity at the high end of the memory-strength scale (with high strength indicated by ‘remember’ responses or by hits with correct source information)52,53,65. Thus, the slope of the fMRI response is steeper when memory is strong than when it is weak. We suggest that this relationship reflects nonlinear properties of the measurement scale that arise for reasons unrelated to the distinction between recollection and familiarity. The same relationship has been observed even for purely recollection-based tasks68. b. In the perirhinal cortex, the relationship between fMRI activity and memory strength at encoding is such that there is often a relatively steep increase in activity when memories are weak, but a more shallow increase when memories are stronger5254. This nonlinear pattern has also been observed for purely recollection-based tasks82. c. In the hippocampus, the relationship between fMRI activity and memory strength at retrieval is generally the same as is observed at encoding60,63,64. d. In the perirhinal cortex, the relationship between fMRI activity and memory strength at retrieval is such that there is often a decrease in activity as memory strength increases60,63,78. This pattern holds true for item-based memory tasks and may be indicative of novelty detection. The same pattern has also been observed in the anterior hippocampus (not shown)60. Lastly, unlike item-based memory tasks, the relationship between fMRI activity and memory strength in perirhinal cortex tends to be positively sloped for recollection-based associative memory tasks (not shown)82,83.

A neuroimaging study of associative learning illustrates this point68. While being scanned, participants learned face–name associations and then were shown a studied face along with two possible choices: the correct name and another name that had been presented earlier with a different face. Because the two names were equally familiar, it is reasonable to assume that successful performance depended on recollection. Participants were first asked to choose the correct name and then to indicate high or low confidence in their decision. Accuracy was higher for high-confidence decisions (approximately 78% correct) than for low-confidence decisions (approximately 58% correct); however, both kinds of decision were significantly more accurate than would be expected by chance. Thus, behavioural accuracy could be detected in both conditions, even though the low-confidence decisions would have included more random noise owing to guessing than the high-confidence decisions. By contrast, only the high-confidence correct decisions were associated with increased hippocampal activity (relative to incorrect decisions), as measured by fmRI. Thus, fmRI does not readily detect weak memory in the hippocampus, even when memory is based on recollection and can be detected by behavioural measures.

Similarly, another study used a variant of the remember–know procedure and found at retrieval that the activity associated with strong recollection (which was designated by the subject as ‘remember 2’) was elevated relative to the activity associated with ‘know’ responses in a region within and adjacent to the hippocampus67. However, the activity associated with weaker recollection (which was designated by the subject as ‘remember 1’) did not differ from the activity associated with ‘know’ responses. These findings are consistent with the idea that the hippocampal activity associated with relatively weak memory — whether it be weak recollection or weak familiarity — is hard to detect with fmRI. Single-unit recording methods might be more suited to detecting such activity.

In a recent study 69, the activity of single hippocampal neurons was recorded using depth electrodes in epileptic patients who were being evaluated for surgery. The patients saw a sequence of twelve visual images, each of which was presented in one of four quadrants on a computer screen. About 30 minutes later, the patients made old–new decisions for the 12 studied images and 12 new images and were also asked to make a source memory judgment — specifically, to recollect the spatial location (the quadrant) in which the image had been presented. Two kinds of neurons were identified in the hippocampus: neurons that signalled novelty by increasing their firing to new items, and other neurons that signalled prior occurrence by increasing their firing to old items (other hippocampal neurons have been found that decrease their firing in response to either new or old items70). Importantly, the neurons that responded to prior occurrence exhibited increased firing even on trials when the recollection of spatial location failed, and even across test sessions when spatial recollection performance was no better than chance. Thus, successful recollection of spatial location was not required for hippocampal neurons to exhibit item recognition. These results provide direct evidence that the hippocampus is involved in item recognition even when recollection is absent for a major feature of the task.

Single-unit data from monkeys and rats underscore what has been learned from humans. The hippocampus does sometimes signal stimulus-specific information, particularly in associative-memory tasks51. Nevertheless, in single-item recognition tasks, hippocampal neurons tend not to signal specific information about the stimuli, but rather to signal whether a stimulus is familiar or not71. In one study in monkeys72, 34% of hippocampal cells responded differently depending on whether the stimulus was familiar or novel. Similarly, in rats73, 13% of responsive hippocampal cells responded differentially to familiar and novel stimuli. This response pattern has been referred to as an ‘abstracted’ recognition signal, because what is signalled is a prior occurrence, not specific information about the nature of the familiar event.

Other work shows that, during memory tasks, hippocampal neurons also signal multiple relevant aspects of the task structure. For example, in one notable study74, rats performed an olfactory recognition task in which the odour stimuli could appear at several different locations on an open platform. Some hippocampal neurons (14%) conveyed the ‘abstracted’ recognition signal, others (20%) signalled prior occurrence or novelty in conjunction with information about the location in which the odour had been presented, and still others signalled a specific location (34%) or a specific odour (11%). Many other neurons (29%) responded when a stimulus was approached, regardless of which odour or location was involved. These data show that the hippocampus signals virtually all aspects of an ongoing behavioural episode, including whether a stimulus is familiar or novel.

Neural activity in the perirhinal cortex

Studies in monkeys show that single neurons in the inferotemporal cortex (a region that includes perirhinal cortex) respond frequently to a visual stimulus when it is first presented and then less frequently as the stimulus becomes more familiar75. These responses are stimulus selective75,76, and they can persist for more than 24 hours77. Accordingly, such responses are thought to provide a suitable index of familiarity. The fact that perirhinal neurons tend to respond to a repeated item in a stimulus-selective fashion, whereas hippocampal neurons tend to convey a non-selective (abstracted) recognition signal suggests that the two regions make distinct, and potentially complementary, contributions to recognition performance.

Several fmRI studies have shown a similar effect in human perirhinal cortex. Specifically, activity at retrieval is inversely related to memory strength — that is, activity is strongest for novel items and weakest for old items that are correctly recognized57,60,63,7880. In one study 60, the same relationship between memory strength and neural activity was found in the anterior hippocampus. These results are consistent with the idea that the perirhinal cortex (and perhaps the hippocampus as well) responds to item familiarity, which might be signalled in part by a reduced novelty response81. It is also possible that the detection of novelty is distinct from the detection of familiarity60. Indeed, one could suppose that familiarity is signalled by an increased neuronal firing rate that is associated with old stimuli, as was observed for neurons in the human hippocampus69, whereas the detection of novelty is associated with an increased firing rate to new stimuli, as is commonly observed for neurons in perirhinal cortex3.

There has been less agreement about whether the perirhinal cortex also has a role in recollection. Evidence against the idea comes mainly from fmRI studies. For example, activity in the perirhinal cortex at encoding is often no different for items that will later be strongly remembered than for items that will later be weakly remembered but nevertheless exceeds the activity associated with items that will later not be remembered22,5254. Such findings have often been taken to suggest that perirhinal activity influences the subsequent familiarity of an item but does not additionally contribute to its later recollection.

However, as discussed above, these studies confound memory strength with the presence or absence of recollection. An alternative interpretation of these results holds that the relationship between memory strength and activity in the perirhinal cortex is nonlinear (that is, that the fmRI signal is relatively insensitive to changes in memory strength at the high end of the scale), not that the perirhinal cortex is uninvolved in the recollection process (FIG. 5). For example, in one study54 the level of perirhinal activity at encoding predicted higher levels of confidence in subsequent ‘old’ decisions, but only up to a confidence rating of five on a six-point scale. The high threshold/signal-detection model30, which was used to interpret these data, holds that confidence ratings as high as five are based entirely on familiarity, because recollection reliably yields a confidence rating of six. However, considerable evidence shows that confidence ratings of 4 and 5 are associated with lower degrees of source recollection, not with the absence of source recollection8,36,40. Thus, although there is evidence that activity in perirhinal cortex does not always distinguish between items that will be strongly remembered and items that will be weakly remembered, there is a simple explanation for such a finding, based on memory strength, which is unrelated to the distinction between recollection and familiarity.

A more direct way to evaluate the role of the perirhinal cortex in recollection is to use an associative-learning procedure where the familiarity of the test items provides no information and where it is reasonable to assume that successful performance is based on recollection. In one study of this kind82, volunteers learned arbitrary associations between kaleidoscopic images and spatial locations on a computer screen (similar to an earlier study that was carried out with monkeys51). Activity in right perirhinal cortex, as well as bilaterally in the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex, increased as accuracy improved on the task. Interestingly, a nonlinear relationship between memory strength and neural activity was observed in the perirhinal cortex. Thus, even in this purely recollection-based task, perirhinal activity increased rapidly early in learning and then levelled off as learning continued and memory strength increased. The authors point out that the nonlinear relationship between neural activity and memory strength might simply reflect the fact that the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOlD) signal for the perirhinal cortex is not linearly related to memory strength. Similarly, our view is that a monotonic relationship between memory strength and neural activity, as measured by fmRI, is meaningful, but that differences in the nonlinear shape of that function across different brain structures should not imply qualitative differences in the memory processes that they subserve (BOX 2). This nonlinearity is not always observed (for example, in one study83 it was observed at encoding but not at retrieval), but it is observed often enough to suggest that the fmRI signal in perirhinal cortex is relatively insensitive to variations in memory strength at the high end of the scale.

Box 2 | Interpreting nonlinear response functions.

Functional MRI (fMRI) blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals correlate with neural activity in the brain, but the precise nature of the relationship is not well understood. Accordingly, attaching theoretical significance to different nonlinear shapes of otherwise monotonically comparable functions (for example, two increasing functions, such as in FIG. 5a,b) is problematic. Different nonlinearities in the hippocampus and the perirhinal cortex would imply different functionality only under the untested assumption that the relationship between the underlying neural activity and the fMRI signal is the same for both structures. Caution on this point has been expressed previously, on the grounds that the BOLD response might depend nonlinearly on the neural signal, and further that this nonlinearity might differ across brain regions, even when the regions lie in close proximity126,127. By contrast, a qualitative difference in the relationship between the BOLD signal and memory strength, such as an increasing function in the hippocampus and a decreasing function in the perirhinal cortex (FIG. 5c,d), is likely to be meaningful. In this case, an increasing function would seem to indicate that higher neural activity is associated with stronger memory, whereas a decreasing function would seem to indicate that higher neural activity is associated with increased novelty.

Numerous other imaging studies have also observed activity in medial temporal lobe structures, including perirhinal cortex, that is related to successful associative recollection (for examples, see REFS 8387, but see REF. 88 for a possible exception). These studies are advantaged by not relying on any particular cognitive model to identify recollection-based responses. In addition, they do not confound the presence or absence of recollection with the strength of memory.

Other evidence of a role for the perirhinal cortex in associative recollection comes from single-unit studies in monkeys. In one study 89, monkeys learned a pair-association task. Altogether, the monkeys learned 12 stimulus–stimulus associations (that is, there were 24 stimuli in all), with the individual stimuli of each pair serving as the cue stimulus on some trials and as the correct stimulus on other trials. Neurons in both perirhinal cortex (14% of 510 neurons) and the immediately adjacent unimodal visual area TE (19% of 1,858 neurons) responded selectively to at least 1 of the 24 stimuli during the cue period. Crucially, many of those same neurons were also preferentially activated by the cue’s paired associate. The percentage of these ‘pair coding’ neurons was much higher in perirhinal cortex (33% of cue-selective neurons) than in area TE (4.9% of cue-selective neurons). This finding suggests that perirhinal cortex is specifically involved in the process of associative encoding, perhaps as the result of convergence onto perirhinal neurons from separate area TE neurons that encode the two visual stimuli. Importantly, a subsequent study found that ‘pair-coding’ neurons in perirhinal cortex (as well as in area TE) develop progressively over the course of several hours and in parallel with behavioural learning90. The fact that many of the neurons in perirhinal cortex exhibited pair-coding responses as soon as they exhibited stimulus selectivity suggests that the associative-memory signals originate in the perirhinal cortex without feedback from other areas89.

In another study91, monkeys learned arbitrary associations between complex scenes and one of four spatial locations on a computer screen (as in the study discussed earlier51). About 30% of scene-selective cells in perirhinal cortex signalled learning by changing their firing rate in parallel with behavioural learning (for comparison, in the hippocampus, the percentage was 28%). These findings demonstrate the existence of associative-learning signals in perirhinal cortex even in tasks that are learned relatively rapidly. The results are also consistent with lesion studies showing that damage to the perirhinal cortex in monkeys impairs the learning of new associations92,93. Thus, although it is true that the perirhinal cortex is involved in familiarity responses, it is also clearly involved in associative recollection.

Conclusions

The idea that directly adjacent and interconnected structures of the medial temporal lobe selectively subserve recollection and familiarity has generated a great deal of apparently contradictory research in recent years. Part of the dispute is traceable to the fact that a number of neuroimaging studies and studies of human amnesia, which appear to support a divided-labour account of medial temporal lobe function, have depended for their interpretation on the validity of controversial cognitive models. In this article we have presented a different perspective. We suggest that the methods that have typically been used to distinguish between recollection and familiarity instead distinguish between strong memories and weak memories. Further, nonlinear fmRI signals in the medial temporal lobe have often been taken to indicate qualitative distinctions between memory processes. However, this interpretation depends on the assumption that the fmRI signal is linearly related to memory strength. A simpler possibility is that nonlinearities indicate that the fmRI signal does not provide a linear measurement scale. Studies that are not dependent on particular models, such as direct comparisons of recall versus recognition deficits in patients with hippocampal lesions, and single-unit studies of recollection and familiarity, find that both the hippocampus and the perirhinal cortex are associated with recollection and familiarity.

Although the function of medial temporal lobe structures cannot be sharply dichotomized according to recollection and familiarity, this does not imply that these structures have the same function. For example, neurons in perirhinal cortex and the hippocampus differ in their response to novel stimuli and familiar stimuli. Neurons in the perirhinal cortex tend to signal novelty and then return to baseline as an item becomes more familiar77. Neurons in the hippocampus are more responsive to familiar stimuli, sometimes increasing their firing rate and sometimes decreasing their firing rate below baseline69,70. What these differences imply about the functional organization of the medial temporal lobe is not yet clear, but they are not related in any obvious way to the distinction between recollection and familiarity.

Another difference between the hippocampus and the perirhinal cortex lies in the degree to which the two structures code information in stimulus-specific or more abstract forms. Whereas perirhinal neurons often respond in a stimulus-selective manner75,76, the neurons of the hippocampus are less stimulus-selective and are more likely to signal prior occurrence, regardless of which stimulus is presented69,71,94. Although these signals in the hippocampus are abstract, they nevertheless appear to be directly involved in both recollection and familiarity, and do not merely reflect encoding of the outcome of recognition experiences, as recently suggested5. The evidence that these signals are directly involved in recollection and familiarity comes from tests of the effect of hippocampal lesions on tasks that depend on these processes. As discussed earlier, hippocampal lesions impair recognition (which is partially based on familiarity) to the same extent that they impair recall (which is based on recollection).

Our Review emphasizes that recollection and familiarity signals are evident in both the perirhinal cortex and the hippocampus. In place of approaches that focus on these constructs, we suggest an alternative approach to understanding the function of medial temporal lobe structures. Underwood95 argued that memory is best understood as a collection of specific stimulus attributes. Thus, spatial location, frequency, recency, modality, semantic class and stimulus associations are all attributes of an episode thought to collectively form the memory trace. Single-unit recording studies suggest that a similarly broad range of attributes is in fact encoded by the neurons of the medial temporal lobe96. Single-unit studies further suggest that these attributes are associated by neurons of the perirhinal cortex (for example, pair-coding neurons89) as well as by the hippocampus (for example, neurons that encode conjunctions74). These considerations suggest that searching for the anatomical correlates of recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe might be a less fruitful way to understand its organization than investigating how the hippocampus, the perirhinal cortex and other medial temporal lobe structures participate in encoding and retrieving the attributes of memory.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Medical Research Service of the Department of Veterans Affairs, the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Science Foundation, the James S. McDonnell Foundation and the Metropolitan Life Foundation. We thank W. Suzuki for her helpful comments.

Recognition

The ability to distinguish a previously presented stimulus from one that was not previously presented.

Recall

The ability to remember a previously presented stimulus in the absence of that stimulus.

Hypoxia

A condition in which there is insufficient oxygen in blood or tissue.

Source memory

The recollection of information about how, when or where a particular stimulus was presented (that is, its source).

Associative recognition

The ability to distinguish a pair of stimuli that have previously been presented together from another pair of stimuli whose items were also previously presented, but as part of different pairs.

Old–new decision

The decision as to whether a test item in a recognition test has been presented before. If the subject thinks it has, the item is termed ‘old’; if not, it is termed ‘new’.

Target

An item on a recognition memory test that appeared on a list presented earlier (that is, an ‘old’ item).

Foil

An item on a recognition memory test that did not appear on a list presented earlier (that is, a ‘new’ item).

Single-unit neurophysiology

A method used to measure the activity of individual neurons in awake, behaving animals. This method has excellent spatial and temporal resolution but can survey the activity of relatively small numbers of neurons.

Functional MRI

(fMRI). An imaging technique that measures changes in haemoglobin oxygenation as blood flows to functioning areas of the brain.

References

  • 1.Mandler G. Recognizing: the judgment of previous occurrence. Psychol Rev. 1980;87:252–271. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Atkinson RC, Juola JF. In: Contemporary Developments in Mathematical Psychology. Krantz DH, Atkinson RC, Suppes P, editors. Freeman; San Francisco: 1974. pp. 243–290. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Brown MW, Aggleton JP. Recognition memory: what are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? . Nature Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:51–61. doi: 10.1038/35049064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Aggleton AP, Brown MW. Interleaving brain systems for episodic and recognition memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:455–463. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AR, Ranganath C. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rugg MD, Yonelinas AP. Human recognition memory: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:313–319. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00131-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Squire LR, Stark CEL, Clark RE. The medial temporal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:279–306. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wixted JT. Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory. Psychol Rev. 2007;114:152–176. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Burwell RD, Suzuki WA, Insausti R, Amaral DG. In: Perception, Memory, and Emotion: Frontiers in Neuroscience. Ono T, McNaughton BL, Molotchnikoff S, Rolls ET, Nishijo H, editors. Elsevier: New York; 1996. pp. 95–110. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Baddeley A, Vargha-Khadem F, Mishkin M. Preserved recognition in a case of developmental amnesia: implications for the acquisition of semantic memory? . J Cogn Neurosci. 2001;13:357–369. doi: 10.1162/08989290151137403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bastin C, et al. Dissociation between recall and recognition memory performance in an amnesic patient with hippocampal damage following carbon monoxide poisoning. Neurocase. 2004;10:330–344. doi: 10.1080/13554790490507650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Holdstock JS, et al. Under what conditions is recognition spared relative to recall after selective hippocampal damage in humans? Hippocampus. 2002;12:341–351. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mayes AR, Holdstock JS, Isaac CL, Hunkin NM, Roberts N. Relative sparing of item recognition memory in a patient with adult-onset damage limited to the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2002;12:325–340. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Yonelinas AP, et al. Effects of extensive temporal lobe damage or mild hypoxia on recollection and familiarity. Nature Neurosci. 2002;5:1236–1241. doi: 10.1038/nn961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wixted JT, Squire LR. Recall and recognition are equally impaired in patients with selective hippocampal damage. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2004;4:58–66. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.1.58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Manns JR, Hopkins RO, Reed JM, Kitchener EG, Squire LR. Recognition memory and the human hippocampus. Neuron. 2003;37:171–180. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01147-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kopelman MD, et al. Recall and recognition memory in amnesia: patients with hippocampal, medial temporal, temporal lobe or frontal pathology. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1232–1246. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Eichenbaum H, Otto T, Cohen NJ. Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system. Behav Brain Sci. 1994;17:449–472. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cohen NJ, et al. Hippocampal system and declarative (relational) memory: summarizing the data from functional neuroimaging studies. Hippocampus. 1999;9:83–98. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:1<83::AID-HIPO9>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Giovanello KS, Schnye DM, Verfaellie M. A critical role for the anterior hippocampus in relational memory: evidence from a fMRI study comparing associative and item recognition. Hippocampus. 2004;14:5–8. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Yonelinas AP, Parks CM. Receiver operating characteristics (ROCs) in recognition memory: a review. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:800–832. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gold JJ, et al. Item memory, source memory, and the medial temporal lobe: concordant findings from fMRI and memory-impaired patients. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:9351–9356. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602716103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Stark CE, Bayley PJ, Squire LR. Recognition memory for single items and for associations is similarly impaired following damage limited to the hippocampal region. Learn Mem. 2002;9:238–242. doi: 10.1101/lm.51802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kan IP, Giovanello KS, Schnyer DM, Makris N, Verfaellie M. Role of the medial temporal lobes in relational memory: neuropsychological evidence from a cued recognition paradigm. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2589–2597. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gold JJ, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. Single-item memory, associative memory, and the human hippocampus. Learn Mem. 2006;13:644–649. doi: 10.1101/lm.258406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Ramus S. Severity of memory impairment in monkeys as a function of locus and extent of damage within the medial temporal lobe memory system. Hippocampus. 1994;4:483–495. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mumby DG. Perspectives on object-recognition memory following hippocampal damage: lessons from studies in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2001;127:159–181. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00367-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Egan JP. Recognition memory and the operating characteristic. (Tech. Note AFCRC-TN-58–51) Bloomington: Indiana Univ., Hearing and Communication Laboratory; 1958. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rotello CM, Macmillan NA, Reeder JA. Sum-difference theory of remembering and knowing: a two-dimensional signal detection model. Psychol Rev. 2004;111:588–616. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Yonelinas AP. Receiver-operating characteristics in recognition memory: evidence for a dual-process model. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1994;20:1341–1354. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.20.6.1341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Aggleton JP, et al. Sparing of the familiarity component of recognition memory in a patient with hippocampal pathology. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1810–1823. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Fortin NJ, Wright SP, Eichenbaum H. Recollection-like memory retrieval in rats is dependent on the hippocampus. Nature. 2004;431:188–191. doi: 10.1038/nature02853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Yonelinas AP, Kroll NE, Dobbins I, Lazzara M, Knight RT. Recollection and familiarity deficits in amnesia: convergence of remember–know, process dissociation, and receiver operating characteristic data. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:323–339. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Heathcote A. Item recognition memory and the ROC. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003;29:1210–1230. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.6.1210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rotello CM, Macmillan NA, Reeder JA, Wong M. The remember response: subject to bias, graded, and not a process-pure indicator of recollection. Psychon Bull Rev. 2005;12:865–873. doi: 10.3758/bf03196778. This paper provides a demonstration that ‘remember’ responses are based on a continuous memory-strength signal, not a threshold recollection signal. The reported experiments also show why, in the past, some results have seemed to suggest otherwise. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Slotnick SD, Dodson CS. Support for a continuous (single-process) model of recognition memory and source memory. Mem Cogn. 2005;33:151–170. doi: 10.3758/bf03195305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Smith DG, Duncan MJ. Testing theories of recognition memory by predicting performance across paradigms. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004;30:615–625. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Glanzer M, Kim K, Hilford A, Adams JK. Slope of the receiver-operating characteristic in recognition memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1999;25:500–513. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Koriat A, Levy-Sadot R, Edry E, de Marcas G. What do we know about what we cannot remember? Accessing the semantic attributes of words that cannot be recalled. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003;29:1095–1105. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.6.1095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Slotnick SD, Klein SA, Dodson CS, Shimamura AP. An analysis of signal detection and threshold models of source memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2000;26:1499–1517. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.6.1499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Wais PE, Wixted JT, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. The hippocampus supports both the recollection and the familiarity components of recognition memory. Neuron. 2006;49:459–466. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.12.020. This study shows that the component processes (recollection and familiarity) that determine the shape of the ROC are both operative in memory-impaired patients with hippocampal lesions. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Rempel-Clower NL, Zola SM, Squire LR, Amara DG. Three cases of enduring memory impairment after bilateral damage limited to the hippocampal formation. J Neurosci. 1996;16:5233–5255. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-16-05233.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Holdstock JS, Mayes AR, Gong Q, Roberts N, Kapur N. Item recognition is less impaired than recall and associative recognition in a patient with selective hippocampal damage. Hippocampus. 2005;15:203–215. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Moscovitch D, McAndrews MP. Material-specific deficits in remembering in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and excisions. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:1335–1342. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00213-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gardiner J, Richardson-Klavehn A. In: The Oxford Handbook of Memory. Tulving E, Craik FIM, editors. Oxford Univ. Press; New York: 2000. pp. 229–244. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Donaldson W. The role of decision processes in remembering and knowing. Mem Cogn. 1996;24:523–533. doi: 10.3758/bf03200940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Dunn JC. Remember–know: a matter of confidence. Psychol Rev. 2004;111:524–542. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wixted JT, Stretch V. In defense of the signal detection interpretation of remember/know judgments. Psychon Bull Rev. 2004;11:616–641. doi: 10.3758/bf03196616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rotello CM, Macmillan NA, Hicks JL, Hautus M. Interpreting the effects of response bias on remember–know judgments using signal-detection and threshold models. Mem Cogn. 2006;34:1598–1614. doi: 10.3758/bf03195923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wais PE, Mickes L, Wixted JT. Remember/know judgments probe degrees of recollection. J Cogn Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20041. (in the press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wirth S, et al. Single neurons in the monkey hippocampus and learning of new associations. Science. 2003;300:1578–1581. doi: 10.1126/science.1084324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Davachi L, Mitchell JP, Wagner AD. Multiple routes to memory: distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:2157–2162. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0337195100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kensinger EA, Schacter DL. Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. J Neurosci. 2006;26:2564–2570. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5241-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Ranganath C, et al. Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity within the medial temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia. 2003;42:2–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cansino S, Maquet P, Dolan RJ, Rugg MD. Brain activity underlying encoding and retrieval of source memory. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:1048–1056. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.10.1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Dobbins IG, Rice HJ, Wagner AD, Schacter DL. Memory orientation and success: separable neurocognitive components underlying episodic recognition. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:318–333. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00164-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Weis S, et al. Process dissociation between contextual retrieval and item recognition. Neuroreport. 2004;15:2729–2733. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Yonelinas AP. The nature of recollection and familiarity: a review of 30 years of research. J Mem Lang. 2002;46:441–517. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Otten LJ. Fragments of a larger whole: retrieval cues constrain observed neural correlates of memory encoding. Cereb Cortex. 2006;17:2030–2038. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Daselaar SM, Fleck MS, Cabeza R. Triple dissociation in the medial temporal lobes: recollection, familiarity, and novelty. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:1902–1911. doi: 10.1152/jn.01029.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Eldridge LL, Knowlton BJ, Furmanski CS, Bookheimer SY, Engel SA. Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nature Neurosci. 2000;3:1149–1152. doi: 10.1038/80671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Henson RN, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Josephs O, Dolan R. Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: an event-related fMRI study. J Neurosci. 1999;19:3962–3972. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-03962.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Montaldi D, Spencer TJ, Roberts N, Mayes AR. The neural system that mediates familiarity memory. Hippocampus. 2006;16:504–520. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Yonelinas AP, Otten L, Shaw KN, Rugg MD. Separating the brain regions involved in recollection and familiarity-strength in recognition memory. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3002–3008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5295-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Uncapher MR, Rugg MD. Encoding and the durability of episodic memory: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7260–7267. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1641-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Eldridge LL, Engel SA, Zeineh MM, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ. A dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes in the human hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3280–3286. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3420-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Vilberg KL, Rugg MD. Dissociation of the neural correlates of recognition memory according to familiarity, recollection, and amount of recollected information. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2216–2225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Sperling R, et al. Putting names to faces: successful encoding of associative memories activates the anterior hippocampal formation. Neuroimage. 2003;20:1400–1410. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00391-4. This study of associative learning provides fMRI evidence that strong, recollection-based memories produce detectable hippocampal activity whereas weak, recollection-based memories do not. In this study, the strength of the memory, not recollection or familiarity, determined whether hippocampal activity was detected. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Rutishauser U, Mamelak AN, Schuman EN. Single-trial learning of novel stimuli by individual neurons of the human hippocampus–amygdala complex. Neuron. 2006;49:805–813. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.015. This paper provides single-unit evidence of a familiarity signal in the human hippocampus during recognition memory performance. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Viskontas IV, Knowlton BJ, Steinmetz PN, Fried I. Differences in mnemonic processing by neurons in the human hippocampus and parahippocampal regions. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1654–1662. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Suzuki WA, Eichenbaum H. The neurophysiology of memory. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2000;911:175–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06726.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Wilson FAW, Brown MW, Riches IP. In: Cellular Mechanisms of Conditioning and Behavioral Plasticity. Woody CD, Alkon DL, McGaugh JL, editors. Plenum; New York: 1988. pp. 313–328. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Otto T, Eichenbaum H. Neuronal activity in the hippocampus during delayed nonmatching to sample performance in rats: evidence for hippocampal processing in recognition memory. Hippocampus. 1992;2:323–334. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Wood ER, Dudchenko PA, Eichenbaum H. The global record of memory in hippocampal neuronal activity. Nature. 1999;397:613–616. doi: 10.1038/17605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Miller EK, Li L, Desimone R. A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. Science. 1991;254:1377– 1379. doi: 10.1126/science.1962197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Miller EK, Li L, Desimone R. Activity of neurons in anterior inferior temporal cortex during a short-term memory task. J Neurosci. 1993;13:1460–1478. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-04-01460.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Xiang JZ, Brown MW. Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37:657–676. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00030-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Gonsalves BD, Kahn I, Curran T, Norman KA, Wagner AD. Memory strength and repetition suppression: multimodal imaging of medial temporal cortical contributions to recognition. Neuron. 2005;47:751–761. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Henson RN, Cansino S, Herron JE, Robb WG, Rugg MD. A familiarity signal in human anterior medial temporal cortex? Hippocampus. 2003;13:301–304. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Henson RN, Hornberger M, Rugg MD. Further dissociating the processes in recognition memory: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17:1058–1073. doi: 10.1162/0898929054475208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Fernández G, Tendolkar I. The rhinal cortex: ‘gatekeeper’ of the declarative memory system. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:358–362. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Law JR, et al. fMRI activity during the gradual acquisition and expression of paired-associate memory. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5720–5729. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4935-04.2005. This study demonstrates activity in the perirhinal cortex and other medial temporal lobe structures during a recollection-based task of associative learning. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Kirwan CB, Stark CEL. Medial temporal lobe activation during encoding and retrieval of novel face–name pairs. Hippocampus. 2004;14:919–930. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Düzel E, et al. Human hippocampal and parahippocampal activity during visual associative recognition memory for spatial and nonspatial stimulus configurations. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9439–9444. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-28-09439.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Jackson O, Schacter DL. Encoding activity in anterior medial temporal lobe supports subsequent associative recognition. Neuroimage. 2004;21:456–462. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Staresina BP, Davachi L. Differential encoding mechanisms for subsequent associative recognition and free recall. J Neurosci. 2006;26:9162–9172. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2877-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Tendolkar I, et al. Probing the neural correlates of associative memory formation: a parametrically analyzed event-related functional MRI study. Brain Res. 2007;1142:159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Kohler S, Danckert S, Gati JS, Ravi RS. Novelty responses to relational and non-relational information in the hippocampus and the parahippocampal region: a comparison based on event-related fMRI. Hippocampus. 2005;15:763–774. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Naya Y, Yoshida M, Miyashita Y. Forward processing of long-term associative memory in monkey inferotemporal cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23:2861–2871. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-07-02861.2003. This study provides single-unit evidence from monkeys of associative learning signals in the perirhinal cortex. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Messinger A, Squire LR, Zola SM, Albright TD. Neuronal representations of stimulus associations develop in the temporal lobe during learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:12239–12244. doi: 10.1073/pnas.211431098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Yanike M, Smith AC, Brown EN, Suzuki WA. Changes in the selectivity of learning-related cells in perirhinal cortex during location-scene task in primates. Soc Neurosci Abst. 2006:574.22. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Murray EA, Baxter MG, Gaffan D. Monkeys with rhinal cortex damage or neurotoxic hippocampal lesions are impaired on spatial scene learning and object reversals. Behav Neurosci. 1998;112:1291–1303. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.112.6.1291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Murray EA, Gaffan D, Mishkin M. Neural substrates of visual stimulus–stimulus association in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci. 1993;13:4549–4561. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-10-04549.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Suzuki WA. In: Hippocampal Place Fields: Relevance to Learning and Memory. Mizumori S, editor. Oxford Univ. Press; (in the press) [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Underwood B. Attributes of memory. Psychol Rev. 1969;76:559–573. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Eichenbaum H, Dudchenko P, Wood E, Shapiro M, Tanila H. The hippocampus, memory, and place cells: is it spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron. 1999;23:209–226. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80773-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S, Chen K. Human amnesia and animal models of amnesia: performance of amnesic patients on tests designed for the monkey. Behav Neurosci. 1988;11:210–221. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.102.2.210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Rempel NL, Clower RP, Amaral DG. Enduring memory impairment in monkeys after ischemic damage to the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1992;9:4355–4370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-07-02582.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Alvarez P, Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR. Damage limited to the hippocampal region produces long-lasting memory impairment in monkeys. J Neurosci. 1995;15:3796–3807. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-05-03796.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Beason-Held LL, Rosene DL, Killiany RJ, Moss MB. Hippocampal formation lesions produce memory impairment in the rhesus monkey. Hippocampus. 1999;9:562–574. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:5<562::AID-HIPO10>3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Zola SM, et al. Impaired recognition memory in monkeys after damage limited to the hippocampal region. J Neurosci. 2000;20:451–463. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00451.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Nemanic S, Alvarado MC, Bachevalier J. The hippocampal/parahippocampal regions and recognition memory: insights from visual paired comparison versus object delayed nonmatching in monkeys. J Neurosci. 2004;24:2013–2026. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3763-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Murray EA, Mishkin M. Object recognition and location memory in monkeys with excitotoxic lesions of the amygdala and hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1998;18:6568–6582. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-16-06568.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Mumby DG, Wood ER, Pinel JPJ. Object-recognition memory is only mildly impaired in rats with lesions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Psychobiology. 1992;20:18–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Mumby DG, Pinel JPJ, Kornecook TJ, Shen MJ, Redila VA. Memory deficits following lesions of hippocampus or amygdala in rat: assessment by an object-memory test battery. Psychobiology. 1995;23:26–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Clark RE, West AN, Zola SM, Squire LR. Rats with lesions of the hippocampus are impaired on the delayed nonmatching-to-sample task. Hippocampus. 2001;11:176–186. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Prusky GT, Douglas RM, Nelson L, Shabanpoor A, Sutherland RJ. Visual memory task for rats reveals an essential role for hippocampus and perirhinal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:5064–5068. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308528101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Mumby DG, et al. Ischemia-induced object-recognition deficits in rats are attenuated by hippocampal ablation before or soon after ischemia. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110:266–281. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.110.2.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Duva CA, et al. Disruption of spatial but not object-recognition memory by neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1997;111:1184–1196. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.111.6.1184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.McKee RD, Squire LR. On the development of declarative memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1993;19:397–404. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.19.2.397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Pascalis O, Hunkin NM, Holdstock JS, Isaac CL, Mayes AR. Visual paired comparison performance is impaired in a patient with selective hippocampal lesions and relatively intact item recognition. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1293–1300. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Clark RE, Zola SM, Squire LR. Impaired recognition memory in rats after damage to the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2000;20:8853–8860. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08853.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Rampon C, et al. Enrichment induces structural changes and recovery from nonspatial memory deficits in CA1 NMDAR1-knockout mice. Nature Neurosci. 2000;3:238–244. doi: 10.1038/72945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Gould TJ, et al. Effects of hippocampal lesions on patterned motor learning in the rat. Brain Res Bull. 2002;58:581–586. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(02)00832-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Baker KB, Kim JJ. Effects of stress and hippocampal NMDA receptor antagonism on recognition memory in rats. Learn Mem. 2002;9:58–65. doi: 10.1101/lm.46102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Gaskin S, Tremblay A, Mumby DG. Retrograde and anterograde object recognition in rats with hippocampal lesions. Hippocampus. 2003;13:962–969. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Hammond RS, Tull LE, Stackman RW. On the delay-dependent involvement of the hippocampus in object recognition memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;82:26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Broadbent NJ, Squire LR, Clark RE. Spatial memory, recognition memory, and the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:14515–14520. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406344101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.de Lima MN, Luft T, Roesler R, Schroder N. Temporary inactivation reveals an essential role of the dorsal hippocampus in consolidation of object recognition memory. Neurosci Lett. 2006;405:142–146. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Ainge JA, et al. The role of the hippocampus in object recognition in rats: examination of the influence of task parameters and lesion size. Behav Brain Res. 2006;167:183–195. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Rossato JI, et al. On the role of hippocampal protein synthesis in the consolidation and reconsolidation of object recognition memory. Learn Mem. 2007;14:36–46. doi: 10.1101/lm.422607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Winters BD, Forwood SE, Cowell RA, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ. Double dissociation between the effects of peri-postrhinal cortex and hippocampal lesions on tests of object recognition and spatial memory: heterogeneity of function within the temporal lobe. J Neurosci. 2004;24:5901–5908. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1346-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Forwood SE, Winters BD, Bussey TJ. Hippocampal lesions that abolish spatial maze performance spare object recognition memory at delays of up to 48 hours. Hippocampus. 2005;15:347–355. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Mumby DG, Tremblay A, Lecluse V, Lehmann H. Hippocampal damage and anterograde object-recognition in rats after long retention intervals. Hippocampus. 2005;15:1050–1056. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.O’Brien N, Lehmann H, Lecluse V, Mumby DG. Enhanced context-dependency of object recognition in rats with hippocampal lesions. Behav Brain Res. 2006;170:156–162. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Logothetis NK, Wandell BA. Interpreting the BOLD signal. Annu Rev Physiol. 2004;66:735–769. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.66.082602.092845. This paper reviews the relationship between neural activity and the fMRI BOLD signal. A key conclusion is that the BOLD response can depend nonlinearly on the neural signal and that this nonlinearity can differ across brain regions. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Heeger DJ, Ress D. What does fMRI tell us about neuronal activity? . Nature Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:142–151. doi: 10.1038/nrn730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Macmillan NA, Creelman CD. Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. 2. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Ratcliff R, Sheu CF, Gronlund SD. Testing global memory models using ROC curves. Psychol Rev. 1992;99:518–535. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.3.518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES