An increasing bulk of evidence indicates that neurocognitive impairment is one of the core symptoms of schizophrenia 1,2. The question therefore is not whether this should be included as one of the diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia, but when and how this inclusion should occur.
One of the foremost experts in the field, Richard Keefe, proposes that the presence of neurocognitive impairment should be included in the DSM-V diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia. The essence of the suggested criterion is a consistent and severe impairment in cognitive functioning as well as a significant decline from premorbid levels. The acceptability of this criterion would be partly based on its sensitivity and specificity, which Keefe has made a case for through the demonstration of the boundary or “point of rarity” between schizophrenia and other related disorders. Although much more data would be needed to evaluate the validity of this criterion, there are still compelling reasons for it to be included in DSM-V. It would make clinicians more aware of the presence of this component and its impact on the functioning of the patient independent of the psychotic symptoms. It would likely lead to a more comprehensive assessment, management and prognostication. Although cognitive impairment is not a sine qua non of schizophrenia, as there may be a subgroup who does not show clinically relevant cognitive deficits 3,4, the structure of DSM could easily accommodate this, since most of the disorders in the manual are defined polythetically 5.
The devil is in the details of what constitutes the core neurocognitive deficits and how they can be assessed. The term “cognitive functioning” is all-encompassing and includes a wide range of information processing, memory, attention, and language functions. Studies based on neuropsychological tests have suggested that patients with schizophrenia generally fall into three cognitive subtypes: generalized impairment, executive dysfunction, and memory dysfunction 1,6,7. These domains require neuropsychological testing by trained assessors. Even with a brief battery which could be learnt by clinicians, as suggested by Keefe, transcultural variability in normative data is a relevant issue.
Cognitive impairment is already part of the diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s dementia, where clinical judgement has been found to be quite accurate when following the DSM-IV criteria 8. However, cognitive symptoms have been explicitly described in relation to functioning, while this connection between cognition and functional outcome is lacking in the criterion proposed by Keefe. There is a large body of research demonstrating a clear association between impaired cognition in schizophrenia and community functioning as well as acquisition of certain skills 9. Although the correlations between performance on individual cognitive domains and functional outcomes are generally moderate, and real-world functional outcomes may be influenced by affective symptoms, motivational and environmental and societal factors 10, it would not be inappropriate to include descriptions of some effects of cognitive impairment on social and vocational functioning, which would facilitate clinical assessment.
The DSM-IV has been criticized for not being researcher-friendly 11,12, and various proposals have been made to rectify this in the DSM-V 13-15. The inclusion of the cognitive impairment criterion would enhance the already considerable ongoing research in this area. The “official” recognition that this is a fundamental and characterizing aspect of schizophrenia would facilitate the approval and labeling process of drug regulatory bodies like the Food and Drug Administration in the US, which in turn would attract more investment from the pharmaceutical industry for research in developing cognition-enhancing drugs 16. For the criterion to be useful to researchers, however, it would have to be more precise and with explicit guidelines for reliable assessment 17.
It is time to include the criterion of cognitive impairment in DSM-V, but it will have to be in such a way as to optimize its clinical and research utility.
References
- 1.Joyce EM, Roiser J. Cognitive heterogeneity in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20:268–272. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ba4975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Goldman-Rakic PS. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6:348–357. doi: 10.1176/jnp.6.4.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Holthausen EAE, Wiersma D, Margriet M. Schizophrenic patients without neuropsychological deficits: subgroup, disease severity or cognitive compensation? Psychiatry Res. 2002;112:1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00184-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kremen SW, Seidman LJ, Faraone SV. The paradox of normal neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109:743–752. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.109.4.743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.First MB, Zimmerman M. Including laboratory tests in DSM-V diagnostic criteria. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:2041–2042. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.12.2041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Joyce EM, Hutton SB, Mutsalas SH. Cognitive heterogeneity in first-episode schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:516–522. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.6.516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Weickert TW, Goldberg TE, Gold JM. Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:907–913. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.9.907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL. Practice parameter: diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2001;56:1143–1153. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.9.1143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Green MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bowie CR, Reichenberg A, Thomas L. Determinants of real-world functional performance in schizophrenia subjects: correlations with cognition, functional capacity, and symptoms. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:418–425. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hyman S, Foreword Phillips K, First M, Pincus H, editors. Advancing DSM: dilemmas in psychiatric diagnosis. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association. 2003:XI–XIX. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Charney D, Barlow D, Botteron K. Neuroscience research agenda to guide development of a pathophysiologically based classification system. In: Kupfer D, First M, Regier D, editors. A research agenda for DSM-V. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association; 2002. pp. 31–84. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Widiger TA, Samuel DB. Diagnostic categories or dimensions? A question for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:494–504. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.First M. Beyond clinical utility: broadening the DSM-V research appendix to include alternative diagnostic constructs. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1679–1681. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Craddock N, Owen MJ. Rethinking psychosis: the disadvantages of a dichotomous classification now outweight the advantages. World Psychiatry. 2007;6:84–91. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Marder SR, Fenton W. Measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia: NIMH MATRICS initiative to support the development of agents for improving cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;7:5–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Donohoe G, Morris DW, Clarke S. Variance in neurocognitive performance is associated with dysbindin-1 in schizophrenia: a preliminary report. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:454–458. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]