Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Am Acad Dermatol. 2007 Jul 19;57(6):990–997. doi: 10.1016/j.jaad.2007.06.007

The Influence of Parents and Peers on Adolescent Indoor Tanning Behavior: Findings from a Multi-City Sample

Katherine D Hoerster 1, Joni A Mayer 2, Susan I Woodruff 2, Vanessa Malcarne 3, Scott C Roesch 3, Elizabeth Clapp 2
PMCID: PMC2330196  NIHMSID: NIHMS35083  PMID: 17658194

Abstract

Background

Indoor tanning is common among adolescents.

Objective

This study examined the influence of parents and peers on adolescent indoor tanning.

Methods

Telephone interviews were conducted with 5274 teen-parent pairs in the 100 largest US cities. Random coefficient regression analyses were conducted to examine the relationship between parental and peer factors and adolescent indoor tanning.

Results

eens’ report of whether their parents allow them to tan indoors was the strongest predictor (aOR: 5.6), while parents’ modeling (aOR:1.2), attitudes (aOR: 1.1), and concern about teen tanning (aOR:1.9) were significantly but less strongly associated. Teens thinking most of their peers like to be tan (aOR: 1.7) and perceived percent of peers who tan indoors (aOR: 1.0) also were significantly associated. Limitations: The cross-sectional design was a limitation of this study.

Conclusion

Interventions targeting adolescent indoor tanning should address both family- and peer-related factors.


Non-melanoma skin cancers (NMSCs) were diagnosed more than 1 million times in 2006 in the United States,1 and that year there were an estimated 64,190 cases of melanoma.1 The primary risk factor for both types of skin cancer is a modifiable one—exposure to ultraviolet radiation (UVR).25 Along with a large body of research showing an association between solar UVR exposure and both NMSC and melanoma, mounting evidence indicates that UVR emitted by the lamps used in indoor tanning booths is also a significant risk factor.69 Indoor tanning also has acute health consequences, including eye and skin burns, other ocular disorders, and suppression of immune functioning.10, 11 The American Academy of Dermatology has recommended banning the non-medical use of indoor tanning booths, and as an interim measure, prohibiting use of tanning booths by minors.12 Despite this and other recommendations against the use of indoor tanning by adolescents, several researchers have found that there is a high prevalence of use within this age group.1317

Correlates of indoor tanning among adolescents have included being female,1317 being older,1317 and, for persons of White race, having darker skin tone and lower sun sensitivity.13, 15 In one study, non-White individuals (both teens and parents, with a statistically significant difference for parents only) tanned less often than did Whites.13 However, most studies on indoor tanning have not included many (if any) people of color.

Factors related to the teen’s parents and peers also have been shown to predict teen’s indoor tanning use.1317 For example, in a survey conducted in Boston, MA and Minneapolis, MN, Stryker and colleagues found that modeling (i.e., maternal indoor tanning behavior), gatekeeping cognitions (i.e., maternal concern about their adolescent tanning indoors) and gatekeeping behaviors (i.e., teen reporting that their parent would allow them to tan indoors) were more significantly associated with adolescent indoor tanning behavior than were maternal cognitive variables (e.g., knowledge about tanning consequences).16 Although in one study having friends who tan was significantly associated with indoor tanning behavior,15 little research has been done on the influence of peers in adolescent indoor tanning behavior. This represents a gap in the literature, given that peers play a significant role in other risky behaviors of adolescents (e.g., smoking behavior).18

The current study was conducted to gain further understanding of the influence parents and caregivers have on adolescent indoor tanning behavior. We performed a partial replication of the study conducted by Stryker and colleagues16 but in a geographically and ethnically diverse sample. Additionally, we examined the influence peers have on adolescent indoor tanning. We hypothesized that the findings from Stryker’s study would be replicated, with parental gatekeeping and modeling emerging as the strongest predictors of adolescent indoor tanning behavior, and parents’ attitudes toward being tan and knowledge about skin cancer risk of indoor tanning as less influential. Additionally, perceived peer tanning behavior and attitudes were expected to have a moderate influence on adolescent indoor tanning behavior.

Methods

Survey Procedures

As one component of a multi-level study (CITY100—Correlates of Indoor Tanning in Youth), telephone interviews were conducted from January through December of 2005. Interviews were conducted in each of the 100 largest US cities as determined by year 2000 census data.19 These cities represent 33 states plus the District of Columbia. A survey was administered to 1) a parent or legal guardian and 2) an adolescent aged 14–17 in each household, with the objective of contacting an average of 60 teen-parent pairs per city.

When a household was contacted, the mother or female caregiver was requested for the parent interview. However, when a woman was not available to take the phone call, the father or other male caregiver was interviewed. Throughout the remainder of this manuscript, parent or caregiver will be used to refer to both parents and guardians.

Survey Sampling, Inc. (SSI) provided lists of targeted age samples for each of the 100 cities. The samples were drawn from the company’s database of households, compiled from white-page phone directories cross-referenced with secondary sources (e.g., school registration lists) to identify households with the desired age range. Using the list generated by SSI, households were contacted via telephone by trained interviewers employed by Luth Research, located in San Diego, California. Although initially we considered mailing an introductory letter to each prospective household in advance of the phone call, results from a pilot study with teen-parent pairs in two non-CITY100 cities suggested that this strategy would not be cost-effective in improving the response rate in the larger study.20 Ten attempts were made to a household before it was deemed unreachable.

When a household was contacted, the interviewer first ascertained from the parent the household’s eligibility to participate. He or she then obtained oral informed consent from the parent for the parent and teen interviews, interviewed the parent, obtained oral assent from the teen, and interviewed the teen. If more than one adolescent was at the home at the time of the interview, one was randomly selected.

Interviewer performance was monitored throughout the interviewing interval to ensure high quality data. The interviews were conducted throughout a calendar year, with attempts to balance season of interview within each city, in order to minimize potential city by seasonal confounds. Adolescent and parent pairs who could not speak English were excluded. No compensation for participation was provided. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of San Diego State University and University of California, San Diego.

The Survey Instrument

The survey used in the current study was one that had been used previously by Stryker and colleagues,16 with some modifications by our research group. It covered a wide variety of domains relevant to indoor tanning behavior including, but not limited to, parent and adolescent attitudes toward and knowledge about risks associated with tanning; adolescent, parent, and peer indoor tanning behavior; and demographic variables. Prior to our main study, we conducted a small pilot test with 32 female college students to evaluate the test-retest reliability of key adolescent items; a trained interviewer administered the survey with each pilot subject twice, with a one-week interval between phone calls. Individual items are described in detail below.

Adolescent Indoor Tanning Behavior

The primary outcome variable was whether the adolescents had tanned indoors using UV lamps at a business in the last 12 months. The item read “In the past 12 months did you go to a tanning salon or other business and use the tanning lamps?”. In our pilot study, test-retest reliability of a version of this item that did not specify where the tanning occurred was excellent (Kappa=1.00, p < .001).

Demographic and Socioeconomic Characteristics

Data on age and sex were collected for all study participants. We also assessed parent education level (high school or less; some college; college graduate; advanced degree) and income (≤$40,000; $40,001–$60,000;≥$60,001), and teen race/ethnicity (approximately 15 categories that were collapsed into: Non-Hispanic White; Non-Hispanic Black; Hispanic White; Other).

Parent Influence

Parent tanning behavior—whether or not they had ever tanned indoors using UV lamps at a business—was examined to assess modeling of this behavior. As a cognitive measure of gatekeeping, parents were asked to report how concerned they would be for their teens’ health if they tanned indoors occasionally. Those parents who answered “a lot” were compared to those who answered “not a lot”; response options “a little” and “not at all” were combined to create the “not a lot” category. As a behavioral measure of gatekeeping, the adolescents were asked whether they agreed or disagreed with the statement, “My parents would allow me to tan indoors.” Reproducibility of this item during the pilot study was good (r =.88, p < .001). Two additional parental cognitive variables were assessed; parents were asked whether they agreed or disagreed with the statements, “People with a tan look more attractive” and “Indoor tanning using indoor tanning lamps could cause skin cancer”.

Peer Influence

To assess the influence that peers had on adolescent indoor tanning behavior, the adolescents were asked what percentage of their friends had ever tanned indoors using tanning lamps. Reproducibility of this item during the pilot study was excellent (r =.95, p < .001). The adolescents also were asked to report whether they agreed or disagreed with the statement, “Most of my friends like to be tan.” Reproducibility of this item during the pilot study was good (r =.76, p < .001).

Adolescent Skin Type

A standard measure with good psychometric properties based on questions about skin’s likelihood of burning and tanning was used to assess skin type.21 This scale is used to categorize individuals into one of four categories (i.e., Skin Type I: always burn, never tan; II: usually burn, then can tan if work at it; III: sometimes get a mild burn, then tan; IV: rarely burn, tan easily). Reproducibility of the skin type assessment during the pilot study was good (r =.70, p < .001).

Statistical Analysis

Initially, descriptive statistics were computed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). Univariate tests, including chi-square analyses, were conducted to assess associations between adolescent indoor tanning (i.e., did or did not tan indoors using UV lamps at a business in the last 12 months) and the demographic/skin type, parental, and peer variables. Then, to assess the combined influence of demographics, parents, and peers on adolescent indoor tanning behavior, random coefficient regression was used. This method was selected because random coefficient regression accounts for the dependence built into the nested data structure. For this two-level model, variables assessed for the adolescent and parent pairs were lower-level variables and they were nested within city (level two). Demographic/skin type variables, except income, were treated as covariates in analyses of the relationship between parental and peer predictors and adolescent indoor tanning behavior. Income was not included as a covariate because it was not considered a covariate by Stryker and colleagues. Moreover, there was a large amount of missing data for this variable and due to restrictions imposed by Hierarchical Linear Modeling, this would have omitted a considerable number of cases from our dataset. Hierarchical linear models were estimated with HLM software, version 6.01.22 Due to the nonlinear nature of the model, the Bernoulli function was selected.

Results

Cooperation Rate and Exclusion Information

Of the 8,176 households that met the initial eligibility criteria, 6,125 (74.9%) agreed to participate. Of the 6,125 interviews conducted, only 6,054 were conducted with both a parent and a teen. Only those interviews conducted with both a parent and a teen are included in the current study. An additional four cases were deleted from the dataset due to lack of clarity on the nature of relationship between the parent and adolescent. Additionally, all individuals for whom there were missing data on any of the fourteen predictor variables assessed were excluded from the current study due to restrictions imposed by Hierarchical Linear Modeling. Thus, the results reported herein are based on 5,274 adolescents and their parents. A total of 591 adolescents (11.2%) reported having tanned indoors using UV lamps at a business in the last 12 months.

Demographic and Skin Type Influence

Basic demographic data can be found in Table 1. As shown, the adolescent sample was well-balanced with respect to sex and the four age categories. Approximately 70% of the adolescents reported having skin types III or IV. Over three-fourths of the caregivers were women, including 4035 mothers, 7 stepmothers, and 53 female guardians, which included aunts, grandmothers, sisters, and guardians. A substantial number of male caregivers participated in the interview as well (1160 fathers, 8 stepfathers, and 11 male guardians, which included grandfathers, guardians, cousins, and brothers). The majority of the caregivers were between the ages of 35 and 55, had at least some college education, and had an income of 60,001 or greater.

Table I.

Demographic and Skin Type Variables

N % of Sample % Teens Tanned in Past Year Chi-Square Odds Ratio for HLM (N = 5274) (95% Confidence Intervals)
Age of Teen (years) 91.51*** 1.31 (1.18, 1.45)***
 14 1252 23.7% 6.2%
 15 1456 27.6% 8.8%
 16 1406 26.7% 13.0%
 17 1160 22.0% 17.5%
Sex of Teen 285.21*** 5.27 (4.02, 6.87)***
 Female 2783 52.8% 18.1%
 Male 2491 47.2% 3.5%
Teen Race/Ethnicity 69.70*** 1.13 (1.02, 1.25)*
 Non-Hispanic White 3740 70.9% 13.3%
 Non-Hispanic Black 345 6.5% 0.9%
 Hispanic White 231 4.4% 6.9%
 Other 958 18.2% 7.7%
Skin Type of Teen 15.24** 1.19 (1.06, 1.34)**
 I: always burn, never tan 584 11.1% 6.5%
 II: usually burn, can tan if work at it 992 18.8% 12.5%
 III: sometimes burn, then tan 1744 33.1% 11.7%
 IV: rarely burn, tan easily 1954 37.0% 11.5%
Sex of Parent .014 1.22 (.94, 1.60)
 Female 4095 77.6% 11.2%
 Male 1179 22.4% 11.1%
Age of Parent (years) 7.04 1.24 (1.04, 1.49)*
 < 35 97 1.8% 3.1%
 5–44 1790 33.9% 11.2%
 45–54 2966 56.2% 11.6%
 >55 421 8.0% 10.5%
Parent Education 17.46** .912 (.812, 1.02)
 High school or less 879 16.7% 13.4%
 Some college 1529 29.0% 12.9%
 College degree 1909 36.2% 10.2%
 Advanced degree 957 18.1% 8.6%
*

p ≤ .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001

Table 1 also presents data from the Chi-square and random coefficient regression analyses. For all but two of the demographic/skin type variables (i.e., parent education, age of parent), findings for the Chi-square and random coefficient regression analyses were concordant. Older teens were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors in the last 12 months. Girls were significantly more likely than boys to have tanned indoors in the last 12 months. Parent’s sex, on the other hand, was not significantly associated with adolescent indoor tanning behavior. Adolescent’s skin type also was significantly associated with indoor tanning behavior, with adolescents whose skin does not easily tan (and likely burns) tanning less often. Non-Hispanic White adolescents were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors in the last 12 months, and Non-Hispanic Black individuals tanned indoors significantly less than the other ethnoracial groups. Finally, the results from the Chi-square analysis indicated that there was a significant relationship between parent education and adolescent indoor tanning behavior, but this was not found in the multilevel analysis. The results from the Chi-square analysis indicated that there was not a significant relationship between age of parent and adolescent indoor tanning but the random coefficient regression analysis indicated that there was a significant relationship, with teens of young parents significantly less likely to have tanned indoors in the last 12 months.

Influence of Parents and Peers

Table 2 presents data from the Chi-square and random coefficient regression analyses relating the five parental predictors (i.e., tanning behavior, thinking that having a tan makes one more attractive, not thinking that tanning indoors poses skin cancer risk, lack of concern about adolescent tanning, and adolescent report of whether their parents would allow them to tan indoors) to whether or not the adolescent had tanned indoors using UV lamps at a business in the last 12 months. It also presents descriptive data for these variables. Findings from both bivariate and multivariate analyses indicated that all five variables of parental influence were significantly and independently associated with adolescent indoor tanning. The general pattern of results was found to be similar for boys and girls when analyzed separately. Adolescents whose parents had ever tanned indoors at a business were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors at a business in the last 12 months. Similarly, those adolescents with parents who believe that people with a tan look more attractive were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors. Adolescents whose parents reported that they would not be concerned “a lot” if their child tanned indoors occasionally, as well as adolescents who believed that their parents would allow them to tan indoors, were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors in the last 12 months. Interestingly, adolescents whose parents agreed with the statement “indoor tanning using indoor tanning lamps could cause skin cancer” were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors in the last 12 months relative to those with parents who did not agree.

Table II.

Findings for the Parental and Peer Predictors

N % of Sample % Teens Tanned Past Year Chi-Square Odds Ratio for HLM (N=5274)(95% CI)
Parent or Guardian Modeling 186.39*** 1.20 (1.13, 1.27)***
 Yes, ever tanned indoors at a business 1263 23.9% 21.8%
 No, never tanned indoors at a business 4011 76.1% 7.9%
Parental Cognitive Variables
People with a tan look more attractive 56.30*** 1.10 (1.04, 1.18)**
 Agree 3486 66.1% 13.5%
 Disagree 1788 33.9% 6.7%
Indoor tanning can cause skin cancer 4.11* .908 (.850, .966)**
 Disagree or Don’t Know 829 15.7% 9.2%
 Agree 4445 84.3% 11.6%
Gate-keeping Variables
How concerned would you be for teen’s health if he/she tanned occasionally? 166.75*** 1.93 (1.54, 2.43)***
 Not a lot 2263 42.9% 17.7%
 A lot 3011 57.1% 6.3%
My parents would allow me to tan indoors 485.72*** 5.60 (4.35, 7.19)***
 Agree 2173 41.2% 22.6%
 Disagree 3101 58.8% 3.2%
Peer Variables
Most of my friends like to be tan 109.27*** 1.73 (1.11, 2.70)**
 Agree 4199 79.6% 13.5%
 Disagree 1075 20.4% 2.2%
Perceived % of peers who tan indoorsa __ __ __ __ 1.03 (1.02, 1.04)***
*

p ≤ .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001

a

Descriptive data for this continuous variable are presented in the text

Findings from both Chi-square and random coefficient regression analyses showed that those adolescents who believed that most of their friends like to be tan were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors in the last 12 months. Adolescents also were asked to report the percentage of their peers who tan indoors, and the mean was 28.22% (SD = 30.13). Adolescents who had tanned indoors in the last 12 months reported a significantly higher mean percentage of peers who had tanned indoors (M = 64.37%, SD = 28.63%) than adolescents who had not tanned indoors in the last 12 months (M = 23.67%, SD = 28.63%), t=−34.21, p < 001. Findings from the random coefficient regression analysis indicated that perceiving a higher percentage of peers as having tanned indoors was associated with higher levels of adolescent indoor tanning behavior in the last 12 months.

Discussion

The results of the current study indicated that a significant number of adolescents from a multi-city sample had recently tanned indoors at a business. The prevalence of indoor tanning in the last year among our adolescent sample of 11.2% was similar to the rates of 10% found in two national studies.13, 15 Female teens, older teens, and teens who are less likely to burn and more likely to tan were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors, supporting data from previous studies.1317 A dramatic sex difference was found, with female adolescents 5.3 times more likely than males to have tanned indoors in the last year.

Several parental factors were significantly associated with adolescent indoor tanning behavior, suggesting that previous recommendations to target parents in intervention activities, in addition to the adolescent, may be warranted.13, 16 As suggested by Cokkinides et al.13 and Stryker et al.,16 there are multiple ways that parents may be influencing the tanning behavior of their children. In this and previous studies,13, 16 parental tanning behavior proved to be a significant predictor, possibly due to modeling.23 In our study, teens with parents who had ever tanned indoors tanned indoors in the last 12 months far more often than did teens of parents who had never tanned indoors (21.8% vs. 7.9%). Parental modeling has been found to be important in predicting health behaviors in other domains, such as obesity.24 Thus, decreasing the frequency with which parents tan indoors may subsequently decrease the frequency of that behavior in adolescents.

The other primary way that parents may influence adolescent tanning behavior is the extent to which they are gatekeepers, monitoring and attempting to prevent indoor tanning behavior in their adolescent. Mirroring results obtained by Stryker and colleagues,16 in our study adolescents’ perceptions of whether they would be allowed to tan indoors was significantly associated with their indoor tanning behavior. In fact, adolescents who said that their parents would allow them to tan indoors were 5.6 times more likely to have tanned indoors, making this the strongest association in the multivariate model. Also, teens of parents who reported that they would not be concerned “a lot” for their teens’ health if they tanned indoors occasionally were nearly twice as likely to have tanned indoors, a finding that was similar to that found by Stryker and colleagues (aOR = 1.7).16 Thus, encouraging parents to increase their gatekeeping behavior and changing their gatekeeping cognitions may help prevent adolescents from tanning indoors. Parental monitoring has been found to be important in the prevention of other risky behaviors such as substance use and antisocial behavior.25

Although Stryker and colleagues found that parental cognitive variables did not predict adolescent indoor tanning behavior when controlling for other factors in their multivariate model,16 the non-gatekeeping cognitive variables assessed in the current study were in fact significant predictors. Approximately 13.5% (compared with 6.7%) of adolescents with parents who reported that people with a tan look more attractive had tanned indoors in the last 12 months, suggesting that interventions to reduce adolescent indoor tanning behavior should target parental attitudes, in addition to parental tanning behavior and gatekeeping-related cognition and behavior. One finding contradicted our hypotheses. Adolescents of parents who reported that they thought indoor tanning posed a risk for skin cancer were significantly more likely to have tanned indoors. Although seemingly counterintuitive, these findings may simply reflect that parents whose adolescents are tanning have thought more about the health risks than parents whose adolescents had not tanned recently. These findings also may suggest that people’s UVR protective behaviors may be modified more effectively by focusing on the appearance-based, rather than the skin cancer effects, of UVR exposure. In fact, Mahler and colleagues found that an intervention using UV photographs and photoaging information was successful in improving sun protective behaviors among college students.26

A large proportion of our adolescent sample (79.6%) reported that most of their friends like to be tan, and our findings indicated that adolescents who held this belief were 1.7 times more likely to have tanned indoors. Perception of a higher percentage of peers who have tanned indoors also was associated with higher rates of indoor tanning. Likewise, Geller and colleagues found that adolescents who reported that they had friends who tanned were significantly more likely to have engaged in indoor tanning behavior.15 Along similar lines, among a group of college students, having friends, family, and partners who thought that they should tan indoors was positively and significantly associated with indoor tanning bed use.27 Thus, it appears to be the case that peer behavior and attitudes, as well as perceptions of peer behavior and attitudes, may be additional target areas for intervention. One recent sun safety intervention successfully increased sun protective behaviors in part by capitalizing on the potential impact that peers can have with one another’s health behaviors, using “sun teams” made up of 8th through 12th grade students to lead peer-education activities regarding sun protection.28

There are several limitations to the current study that should be addressed. First, our use of a cross-sectional design means that directionality and causality cannot be assumed. Although constructs from Social Cognitive Theory23 support our inferences, the findings are merely correlational; prospective data are needed to confirm our interpretations. Second, adolescent respondents may have been less forthright on the telephone when answering questions about indoor tanning than they would have been with an anonymous paper-and-pencil survey. For example, we had no way to ensure their privacy during the interview and/or they may have underreported their tanning behavior in order to please the interviewer. Also, one of our key variables assessing gatekeeping—adolescents’ report of whether or not their parents would allow them to tan indoors—is merely a proxy for parental gatekeeping behavior since adolescents, not parents, reported on parental behavior. Thus, it is possible that adolescent report of parental behavior does not accurately reflect true parental gatekeeping behavior.

Because our diverse sample was selected from the 100 largest U.S. cities and we had a respectable cooperation rate, the findings likely will generalize to U.S. urban adolescents. On the other hand, they may not generalize to those living in suburban and rural areas. Little research has examined whether there are differences in indoor tanning prevalence between individuals living in urban versus suburban or rural areas. However, Demko and colleagues did find that attending a rural high school was significantly associated with indoor tanning behavior.14 Future research should look into such potential differences. The relatively high proportion of individuals in our sample who reported a low likelihood of burning and high likelihood of tanning compared with other samples15 may reflect the ethnoracial diversity of our sample.

In summary, these data serve to support previous assertions that parents may play an important role in adolescent indoor tanning behavior. Our study also highlighted the possible influence of peers. Adolescents’ perceptions of whether or not their parents would allow them to tan indoors emerged as the strongest predictor of teen tanning in the multivariate model, with modeling, parental cognitions (both gatekeeping and non-gatekeeping), and peer factors also associated but to a lesser degree. These findings underscore the need to address both family- and peer-related factors when designing individual- and policy-level interventions to decrease indoor tanning among adolescents.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by the National Institutes of Health, National Cancer Institute, (R01CA93532 and K05CA10051).

Footnotes

The authors have no conflict of interest to disclose.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.American Cancer Society. Cancer Facts and Figures 2006. American Cancer Society: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Elwood JM. Recent developments in melanoma epidemiology, 1993. Melanoma Res. 1993;3:149–56. doi: 10.1097/00008390-199306000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Friedman RJ, Rigel DS, Bersons DS, Rivers J. Skin cancer: basal cell and squamous cell carcinoma. In: Holleb AJ, Fink DJ, Murphy GP, editors. American Cancer Society Textbook of Clinical Oncology. Atlanta (GA): American Cancer Society; 1991. pp. 290–305. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Marks R. An overview of skin cancers. Incidence and causation. Cancer Supplement. 1995;75:607–12. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(19950115)75:2+<607::aid-cncr2820751402>3.0.co;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Singletary SE, Balch CM. Malignant Melanoma. In: Holleb AJ, Fink DJ, Murphy GP, editors. American Cancer Society Textbook of Clinical Oncology. Atlanta (GA): American Cancer Society; 199. pp. 1263–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gallagher RP, Spinelli JJ, Lee TK. Tanning beds, sunlamps, and risk of cutaneous malignant melanoma. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2005;14:562–6. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-04-0564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Karagas MR, Stannard VA, Mott LA, Slattery MJ, Spencer SK, Weinstock MA. Use of tanning devices and risk of basal cell and squamous cell skin cancers. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2002;94:224–6. doi: 10.1093/jnci/94.3.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Swerdlow AJ, Weinstock MA. Do tanning lamps cause melanoma? An epidemiologic assessment. J Am Acad Dermatol. 1998;38:89–98. doi: 10.1016/s0190-9622(98)70544-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Veierod MB, Weiderpass E, Thorn M, Hansson J, Lund E, Armstrong B, et al. A prospective study of pigmentation, sun exposure, and risk of cutaneous malignant melanoma in women. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2003;95:1530–8. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djg075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Levine JA, Sorace M, Spencer J, Siegel DM. The indoor UV tanning industry: a review of skin cancer risk, health benefit claims, and regulation. J Am Acad Dermatol. 2005;53:1038–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jaad.2005.07.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Walters BI, Kelley TM. Commercial tanning facilities: A new source of eye injury. Am J Emerg Med. 1987;5:386–89. doi: 10.1016/0735-6757(87)90387-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.American Academy of Dermatology. [Accessed November 30, 2004];Position statement on indoor tanning. Available at http://www.aadassociation.org/Policy/tanning.html.
  • 13.Cokkinides VE, Weinstock MA, O’Connell MC, Thun MJ. Use of indoor tanning sunlamps by US youth, ages 11–18 years, and by their parent or guardian caregivers: prevalence and correlates. Pediatrics. 2002;109:1124–30. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.6.1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Demko CA, Borawski EA, Debanne SM, Cooper KD, Stange KC. Use of indoor tanning facilities by white adolescents in the United States. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2003;157:854–60. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.157.9.854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Geller AC, Colditz G, Oliveria S, Emmons K, Jorgensen C, Aweh GN, et al. Use of sunscreen, sunburning rates, and tanning bed use among more than 10 000 US children and adolescents. Pediatrics. 2002;109:1009–14. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.6.1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Stryker JE, Lazovich D, Forster JL, Emmons KM, Sorensen G, Demierre MF. Maternal/female caregiver influences on adolescent indoor tanning. J Adolesc Health. 2004;35:528, e1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.MMWR. Quick stats: percentage of teens aged 14–17 years who used indoor tanning devices during the preceeding 12 months, by sex and age---United States, 2005. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2006;55:1101. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Schepis TS, Rao U. Epidemiology and etiology of adolescent smoking. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2005;17:607–12. doi: 10.1097/01.mop.0000176442.49743.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Census 2000. Population by age, sex, race, and Hispanic or Latino origin for the United States. [Accessed March 10, 2007];United States Census Bureau. Available at http://www.census.gov/main/www/cen2000.html.
  • 20.Woodruff SI, Mayer JA, Clapp E. Effects of an introductory letter on response rates to a teen/parent telephone health survey. Eval Rev. 2006;30:817–23. doi: 10.1177/0193841X05285662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fitzpatrick TB. The validity and practicality of sun-reactive skin types I through VI. Arch Dermatol. 1988;124:869–71. doi: 10.1001/archderm.124.6.869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bryk A, Raudenbush SW. Hierarchical linear models. Newbury Park (CA): Sage Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive approach. Englewood Cliffs (NJ): Prentice Hall; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ritchie LD, Welk G, Styne D, Gerstein DE, Crawford PB. Family environment and pediatric overweight: what is a parent to do? J Am Diet Assoc. 2005;105:S70–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2005.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dishion TJ, McMahon RJ. Parental monitoring and the prevention of child and adolescent problem behavior: a conceptual and empirical formulation. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 1998;1:61–75. doi: 10.1023/a:1021800432380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mahler HI, Kulik JA, Harrell J, Correa A, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M. Effects of UV photographs, photoaging information, and use of sunless tanning lotion on sun protection behaviors. Arch Dermatol. 2005;141:373–80. doi: 10.1001/archderm.141.3.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hillhouse JJ, Turrisi R, Kastner M. Modeling tanning salon behavioral tendencies using appearance motivation, self-monitoring and the theory of planned behavior. Health Educ Res. 2000;15:405–14. doi: 10.1093/her/15.4.405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Olson AL, Gaffney C, Starr P, Gibson JJ, Cole BF, Dietrich AJ. SunSafe in the Middle School Years: a community-wide intervention to change early-adolescent sun protection. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e247–56. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES