Abstract
We summarize and integrate research on cognitive vulnerability to depression among children and adolescents. We first review prospective longitudinal studies of the most researched cognitive vulnerability factors (attributional style, dysfunctional attitudes, and self-perception) and depression among youth. We next review research on information processing biases in youth. We propose that the integration of these two literatures will result in a more adequate test of cognitive vulnerability models. Last, we outline a program of research addressing methodological, statistical, and scientific limitations in the cognitive vulnerability literature.
Keywords: Cognitive vulnerability, Information processing, Depression
1. Introduction
Using a developmental and cognitive science framework, we review and integrate recent research on cognitive vulnerability to depression among children and adolescents. First, we review prospective longitudinal studies assessing relations between putative cognitive vulnerabilities and the occurrence of depression. We then propose that the incorporation of experimental paradigms and the assessment of information processing biases among children may facilitate the testing of alternative cognitive vulnerability models. Last, we outline a program of research addressing methodological, statistical, and scientific limitations in this literature.
1.1. Development of depression
Major depressive disorder (MDD) often begins during adolescence, is chronic and recurrent, and frequently places youth at risk for recurrent MDD during adulthood. Between 20% and 50% percent of adolescents report experiencing subsyndromal levels of depression (Kessler, Avenevoli, & Merikangas, 2001; Petersen, Compas, Brooks-Gunn, & Stemmler, 1993). Lifetime prevalence rates of 1.5% (e.g., Costello et al., 1996) and 7% (e.g., Kessler et al., 2005) have been reported for depressive disorders among children and adolescents, respectively. Depression during adolescence shares features of the depression that occurs during adulthood (e.g., Lewinsohn, Allen, Seeley, & Gotlib, 1999; Pine, Cohen, Gurley, Brook, & Ma, 1998). Adolescence, then, represents a critical period of vulnerability. Seventy-five percent of adults with MDD experience their first depressive episode during childhood or adolescence, whereas only 25% experience onset of MDD in adulthood (Kim-Cohen et al., 2003). That said, patterns of depressive symptoms may differ over the course of development given the cognitive, social, emotional, and biological changes that transpire (Cicchetti & Toth, 1998; Weiss & Garber, 2003). In addition, the observed gender difference in depression emerges in early adolescence (e.g., Angold, Erkanli, Silberg, Eaves, & Costello, 2002; Weissman, Warner, Wickramaratne, Moreau, & Olfson, 1997). Depression is a common, persistent, and pernicious occurrence in the lives of youth. Any comprehensive model of vulnerability for depression must address these observations.
1.2. Theoretical hypotheses of cognitive vulnerability models
Several models have been proposed to explain the development and maintenance of depression among youth. Cognitive vulnerability models stand at the forefront of research activity. Beck (1967) defined cognitive vulnerability as the presence of maladaptive self-schema reflecting themes of helplessness and unlovability that become activated by negative life events or negative moods. Many cognitive vulnerability theories employ a vulnerability-stress paradigm (e.g., Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Beck, 1967), whereby cognitive factors interact with environmental stressors to increase risk for emotional disorders. Indeed, stressful life experiences predict depression among children and adolescents (see Grant et al., 2004a; Grant, Compas, Thurm, McMahon, & Gipson, 2004b). This relationship appears to be bidirectional, as depressive symptoms also predict increases in objectively assessed stressors among youth (Grant et al., 2004b). The assessment of stress in the study of cognitive vulnerability is crucial, as exposure to mild uncontrollable stress during adolescence can impair cognitive functioning (Steinberg, 2004).
Specific criteria for defining cognitive vulnerability factors have been put forth. First, a cognitive vulnerability factor must temporally precede depression and exhibit stability over time (e.g., Alloy et al., 1999; Ingram, Miranda, & Segal, 1998). Second, construct validity must be established, in that the cognitive vulnerability factor must demonstrate predictive validity not better accounted for by an extraneous variable. Third, cognitive vulnerability factors are believed to be specific to particular disorders. For example, if dysfunctional attitudes serve as a vulnerability for depression, they must not also predict conduct disorder (discriminant validity). Fourth, vulnerability is viewed as an endogenous process that is conceptualized as latent (e.g., Ingram et al., 1998). That is, the vulnerability must represent an enduring characteristic of the child, and not their family, relationships, or environment. These general and specific theoretical hypotheses guide the scientific exploration of cognitive vulnerability.
1.3. Cognitive development
Adolescence represents a phase wherein cognitive vulnerability may occur and is likely to emerge. The cognitive developmental ‘prerequisites’ are present and can emerge (Alloy, Abramson, Walshaw, Keyser, & Gerstein, 2006). The adolescent transition is characterized by the emergence of a more self-directed and self-regulated mind (Keating, 2004). Self-regulation is advanced through an executive suite of capabilities (Donald, 2001) as central conceptual structures coordinate across domains (Keating, 1996). A developing executive control monitors and manages cognitive resources during adolescence (Kuhn & Pease, 2006). Thus, across development, increasing executive functioning ability translates to an increasingly ‘top down’ mode of cognition.
A parallel coordination of information processing, or ‘bottom-up’ components allows for the strategic use of emerging cognitive resources. In such, information processing and cognitive reasoning operate in concert. Indeed, recent work reveals that advances in level of reasoning are supported by ‘bottom-up’ changes in efficiency and working memory (Demetriou, Christou, Spanoudis, & Platsidou, 2002). These processes, in turn, are reciprocally affected by ‘top down’ cognition. Moreover, processing speed increases from early childhood through mid-adolescence (Demetriou et al., 2002). For example, adolescents are better able to ignore irrelevant stimuli on the Stroop task than children (Demetriou, Efklides, & Platsidou, 1993). Increasing processing speed leads to greater efficiency in top down processes, including social problem solving. Such research suggests that adolescents possess the cognitive prerequisites for vulnerabilities to depression. If cognitive vulnerability is a valid model representing the onset of depression among youth, we expect children to be capable of demonstrating maladaptive cognitions, which, in interaction with the stress of the adolescent transition, result in depressive disorders.
2. Attributional style
Attributional style is the cognitive vulnerability factor that has garnered the most research and theoretical attention. First proposed as a means of addressing limitations in the original theory of learned helplessness (Seligman, 1975), Abramson et al. (1978) proposed that depressed individuals attribute negative events to internal, global, and stable causes. In contrast, viewing the causes of negative events as external, specific, and unstable represents a positive or adaptive style, and may buffer or protect an individual from depression. A negative attributional style is hypothesized to be a proximal contributor to the onset of depression. In further theoretical modification, hopelessness theory (Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989), introduced three separate cognitions that contribute to depression—a low sense of efficacy following life stress, viewing negative events as having negative consequences, and viewing the causes of these events as global and stable. These cognitions evolve into hopelessness, which is hypothesized to be a proximal and sufficient cause of depression. Hopelessness depression reflects motivational deficits and dysphoric affect stemming from a negative attributional style (Abramson, Alloy, & Metalsky, 1988). An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress depression model (Hankin & Abramson, 2001) accounts for the general development of depression as well as specific outcomes, such as the emergence of a gender difference in depression. In this model, negative events contribute to initial elevations of general negative affect. Cognitive vulnerabilities such as a negative attributional style, then, moderate risk for depression. Depression, in turn, can lead to more negative life events, creating a pathogenic cycle. The aforementioned models all seek to explicate the relations between attributional style and depression.
2.1. Review
Twenty-one studies support the prospective effects of attributional style on depressed mood in children and adolescents (see Table 1). Young children are capable of drawing inferences about events and evidence potential for demonstrating a stable attributional style. In one study, third graders demonstrated greater pessimism in their attributions than seventh graders, were more likely to catastrophize, and viewed themselves as flawed following negative life events (Abela & Payne, 2003). Consistent with prediction, the interaction of life events and attributional style appears to be a more potent predictor of depression as children age (Abela, 2001; Conley, Haines, Hilt, & Metalsky, 2001; Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, & Seligman, 1992). Adolescents who demonstrate a positive attributional style in sixth grade continue along the same linear trajectory (in the positive direction) over time (Garber et al., 2002). Similarly, adolescents who manifest a negative attributional style in sixth grade continue on a trajectory to a more negative attributional style. Taken together, findings indicate that children and adolescents are capable of developing the attributions that have been linked with depression. Longitudinal evidence suggests that as early as middle childhood, negative attributions may place children on a trajectory toward an increasingly negative and pernicious attributional style.
Table 1.
Study | Sample | N | Age (range) and mean |
Depression status |
Depression measure |
Vulnerability measure |
Time frame |
Control baseline depression? |
Life stress interaction tested? |
Summary of findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Attributional style | ||||||||||
Abela (2001) | School | 382 | 3rd (8.9) and 7th (12.9) | Sx | CDI | CCSQ, CASQ | 6 w | X | CLES | CASQ × CLES predicted increases in depressive sx in 7th, but not 3rd graders. Attributions about consequences × CLES predicted depression in 3rd and 7th, Attributions about self × CLES predicted depression increases in 3rd and 7th grade girls, but not boys |
Abela and Payne (2003) | School | 314 | 3rd (8.8) and 7th (12.8) | Sx | CDI | CCSQ, CASQ | 6 w | X | CHAS | Gender and SE moderators of ‘weakest link’ × CHAS. Boys with low SE and girls with high SE significant interaction predicting increases in HS depression sx |
Abela and Sarin (2002) | School | 79 | (12.3) | Sx | CDI | CCSQ, CASQ | 10 w | X | CLES × weakest link | CLES × ‘weakest link’ predicted increases in HS depression sx only |
Abela, Skitch, Adams, and Hankin (2006) | Children of depressed parent | 140 | 6–14 (9.8) | Sx | CDI | CASQ, CCSQ | 1 y | X | Stress of parental onset of depression | Gender × weaklink × parental depression stressor. Children with depressogenic inferential styles greater elevations in depression following elevations in parent's level of depression. Stronger association in girls |
Bennett and Bates (1995) | School | 95 | 11–13 (12) | Sx | CDI | CASQ-R | 6 m | X | CHS, LES | CHS × CASQ predicted CDI, no main effect of CASQ. More support found against vulnerability models |
Brozina and Abela (2006) | School | 480 | 8–13 (10.5) | Sx | CDI | CASQ, CCSQ | 6 w | X | CHAS | CASQ × CHAS to predict increase in depression, but only for those with low depression at t1 |
Conley et al. (2001) | School | 147 | 5–10 (8.2) | Sx | CDI | CASI, CASQ-R | 3 w | X | DHQ | CASI main effect, CASI × DHQ × age slightly higher levels of depression in older children than for younger (all findings for positive subscale) |
Dixon, and Ahrens (1992) | Summer camp | 84 | 9–12(11) | Sx | CDI | KASTAN-R | 1 m | X | Own measure | Hassles and hassles × attribution significant |
Garber et al. (2002) | School, but 78% of children had mothers with a history of mood disorders | 240 | (11.9) | Sx | CDI | CASQ | 5 y | X | LEIA | Adolescents becoming more negative in CASQ over time also reporting higher depression over time. Initial level of stress associated with trajectory of depression sx. Reciprocal models also supported |
Gibb and Alloy (2006) | School | 448 | 9–11(9.8) | Sx | CDI | CASQ-R | 6 m | X | Peer victimization, CTQ-EA | In 4th graders CASQ mediates only, 5th grade mediator and moderator effects. Depression also predicted negative changes in CASQ. Best fitting models include reciprocal relations |
Gibb et al. (2006) | School | 448 | 9–11(9.8) | Sx | CDI | CASQ-R | 6 m | X | Peer victimization, CTQ-EA | Depression predicted negative change in CASQ-R |
Hammen et al. (1988) | Children of depressed mothers | 79 | (12.5) | Dx | K-SADS, CDI | CASQ | 6 m | X | Brown and Harris | Main effect for stress, but not CASQ |
Hankin, Abramson, and Siler (2001) | School | 270 | 14–18 (16.2) | Sx | BDI, HDSQ-R | CASQ-R | 5 w | X | APES | CASQ × APES predicted increases in depression sx. Gender moderated this interaction for BDI, so held for boys but not girls. Held for HS depression sx for both genders, HS did not mediate relations |
Hilsman and Garber (1995) | School | 439 | (11.4) | Sx | CES-D, Depressive Adjective Checklist | CASQ | Approx 2 w | X | Grade deficit stressor | Depressive sx predicted by interactions of negative explanatory style with stressors |
Lewinsohn et al. (1994) | School | 1508 | (16.5) | Dx | K-SADS, CES-D | Items from KASTAN-R | 1 y | X | Measured, but no interactions assessed | Attributions predicted current, past, and first episode depression. OR for current highest |
Lewinsohn et al. (2001) | School | 1507 | (16.6) | Dx | K-SADS, CES-D | CASQ | 1 y | X | Brown and Harris | CASQ × LE predict MDD onset, but at higher levels of stress |
Mccarty, Vander Stoep, and McCauley (2007) | School | 331 | (12.0) | Sx | MFQ | CASQ-R | 1 y | X | Support for ‘scar’ models. Baseline depression predictive of more negative attributional style | |
Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, and Seligman et al. (1986) | School | 168 | (8–11) | Sx | CDI | CASQ | 1 y | X | LEQ | CASQ predicted depression changes at some time points over the year. Interaction with life stress was significant |
Nolen-Hoeksema et al. (1992) | School | 352 | 3rd and 4th graders | Sx | CDI | CASQ | 5 y | X | LEQ | As children grew older, pessimistic explanatory style significant predictor of later depression. Among younger children, life events only. Modest support for diathesis–stress. Some evidence of scar hypothesis |
Panak and Garber (1992) | School | 521 | 3rd–5th graders | Sx | CDI | CASQ | 1 y | X | Peer rejection (a developmental life stress assessment) | Interaction between stress (increase in peer rejection) and CASQ predicted depression |
Prinstein and Aikins (2004) | School | 158 | 15–17 (16.31) | Sx | CDI | CASQ | 17 m | X | Peer rejection (a developmental life stress assessment) | Peer rejection × CASQ predictive of dep. for girls. Main effect for attributional style overall |
Prinstein, Chea, and Guyer (2005) | School | 159 | 15–17 (16.31) | Sx | CDI | Own | 17 m | X | Peer victimization | Higher levels of critical self-referent attributions associated with prospective increases in depressive symptoms under conditions of high levels of boys' peer victimization |
Robinson, Garber, and Hilsman (1995) | School | 371 | (12.0) | Sx | CDI | CASQ | 15 m | X | Developed for study based on Compas (1987) major events and hassles, transition to junior high | Main effect stressors, perceived self-worth. CASQ main and interaction with stressors, 3 way interaction with self-worth |
Schwartz and Koenig (1996) | School | 397 | 14–18 (16.0) | Sx | BDI | ASQ | 1.5 m | X | LEQ | Attributions predicted depression |
Southall and Roberts (2002) | School | 115 | 14–19 (16.5) | Sx | BDI | CASQ | 14 w | X | LES | 3 way significant interaction and SE × CASQ in non-symptomatic at baseline |
Spence, Sheffield, and Donovan (2002) | School | 733 | 12–14 (12.91) | Sx | BDI | CASQ-R | 1 y | X | NLE | CASQ-R main effect |
Toner and Heaven (2005) | School | 112 | (12.5) | Sx | “the way I feel” based on ILQ and BDS | PASS-1 peer-social attributional style scale | 2 y | A post-hoc combined generality attribution (both positive and negative events to stable/global factors) predicted depression | ||
Dysfunctional attitudes | ||||||||||
Abela and Skitch (2007) | Children of depressed parents | 140 | 6–14 (10.0) | Sx | CDI | CDAS | Every 6 w for 1 y | X | CHAS | CDAS × CHAS significant for children with low self-esteem, also significant for children with low CDAS and high self-esteem |
Abela and Sullivan (2003) | School | 184 | (12.8) | Sx | CDI | CDAS | 6 w | X | CHAS | CDAS × CHAS predicted increased depression, in those with high, but not low levels of self-esteem |
Lewinsohn et al. (2001) | School | 1507 | (16.6) | Dx | K-SADS, CES-D | DAS | 1 y | X | Brown and Harris | DAS × NLE (p=.086) trend at high levels of stress CASQ × LE predict MDD onset at higher levels of stress, little effect vs. low levels of stress |
Lewinsohn et al. (1994) | School | 1508 | (16.5) | Dx | K-SADS, CES-D | Items from DAS, labeled ‘pessimism’ | 1 y | X | Measured, but no interactions assessed | ‘Pessimism’ predicted current, past, and first episode depression. OR for current highest |
Marcotte et al. (2006) | School | 644 | 13–16 (14.13) | Sx | BDI | DAS | 2 y | Girls who became depressed (met clinical BDI cutoff) over study demonstrated increased DAS scores | ||
McCreary et al. (2004) | School | 219 | (11.8) | Sx | BHIF, DISC-IV Conduct Disorder Module | CAPS | 1 y | X | Socially prescribed perfectionism significantly predicted (via partial correlations) both depression and anxiety sx in African American boys. SPP showed specificity for predicting depression after controlling for t2 anxiety and conduct sx | |
Self-perception | ||||||||||
Cole et al. (1996) | School | 631 | (8.9) at beginning of study | Sx | CDI | Harter's SPPC | 4 y | X | Self-perceived competence negatively related to later depression | |
Cole et al. (1997) | School | 1459 | 3rd (8.8) and 6th (11.9) | Sx | CDI | Harter's SPPC | 3 y | X | Underestimated competency predicted increases in depression in 1 of 6 grade levels. Depression predicted increases in underestimation over time in all grade levels | |
Cole et al. (2001) | School | 807 | 3rd (8.9) and 6th (11.9) graders | Sx | CDI | Harter's SPPC | 3 y | X | Academic overestimation significant at 4 of 6 time points in predicting depression, however, reverse relation greater beta weights | |
Cole, Martin et al. (1998) | School | 617 | 3rd (8.4) and 6th (11.4) graders | Sx | CDI | Harter's SPPC, PNMC, TRS | 6 m | X | Self-perceptions of competence predicted change in depression scores over time. These competencies mediated relation between competency appraisals by others and depression | |
Cole, Martin et al. (1999) | School | 945 | 3rd (8.37) and 6th (11.36) graders | Sx | CDI, parent CDI, PNID, TRID | Harter's SPPC, PNMC, TRS | 6 m | X | Social competence (latent variable of all cognitive measures) predicted depression for 6th graders | |
Hilsman and Garber (1995) | School | 439 | (11.4) | Sx | CES-D, Depressive Adjective Checklist | Harter's SPPC | Approx 2w | X | Grade deficit stressor | Students who reported lack of competence expressed more depression after receiving unacceptable grades |
Hoffman et al. (2000) | School | 360 | 10.8–13.5 (11.9) | Sx | CDI | Harter's SPPC, TRS, PRS, PNMC | Approx 6 m | X | Reflective and discrepant self-appraisals predicted subsequent depressive symptoms. Depression predicted discrepant, but not reflective | |
Kistner et al. (1999) | School | 108, 68 at follow-up | 4th and 5th graders at start | Sx | CDI, RADS | Harter's SPPC | 7 y | X | Perceived acceptance, but not actual acceptance, predicted depression 7 years later | |
Kistner et al. (2006) | School | 667 | (9.4) | Sx | CDI | Harter's SPPC | 6 m | X | Inaccurate self-perceptions predicted increases in depressive symptoms and depressive symptoms predicted decreased accuracy. Bias did not predict change in depression, rather depression predicted negative bias | |
Kistner, David-Ferdon, Lopez, and Dunkl (2007) | School | 641 | 3rd–5th graders | Sx | CDI | Harter's SPPC | 6 m | X | Self-perception did not predict changes in depression | |
Lewinsohn et al. (1994) | School | 1508 | (16.5) | Dx | K-SADS, CES-D | Items from Harter's SPPC | 1 y | X | Measured, but no interactions assessed | Social self-perception predicted current depression and previous depression, but not first onset |
Measelle, Ablow, Cowan, and Cowan (1998) | Community | 97 | (4.6) | Sx | BPI | BPI | 3 y | Depression-anxiety subscale significantly negatively correlated with competence and motivation in Kindergarten and 1st grade | ||
McGrath and Repetti (2002) | School | 248 | (9.5) | Sx | CDI, TRF | Harter's SPPC | 3 y | X | Negative self-perceptions and underestimations not associated with change in depression | |
Ohannessian et al. (1999) | School | 75 | (11.8) | Sx | CES-D | Harter's SSPC | 1 y | Gender predicts depression until self-perception of athletic competence is added as covariate | ||
Tram and Cole (2000) | School | 468 | 13–17 (14.5) | Sx | CDI, PNID, TRID | Harter's SPPC | Approx 6 m | X | APES | Self-perceived competence predicted change in depressive symptoms |
Note. Age ranges and means reported when available. Grades reported when age data was not available. Sx = symptoms; Dx = diagnosis; CDI = Children's Depression Inventory; CCSQ = Children's Cognitive Style Questionnaire; CASQ = Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire; CASQ-R = Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire-Revised; CLES = The Children's Negative Life Events Scale; CHAS and CHS = Hassles Scale for Children; weakest = weakest link; LES = Life Events Scale; CASI = The Children's Attributional Style Interview; DHQ = Daily Hassles Questionnaire; KASTAN-R = Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire-Revised; LEIA = Life Events Interview for Adolescents; CTQ-EA = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire — Emotional Abuse Subscale; K-SADS = Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children; Brown and Harris = Brown and Harris; 1978; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; HDSQ-R = Hopelessness Depressive Symptoms Questionnaire-Revised; APES = Adolescent Perceived Events Scale; CES-D = The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children; MFQ = The Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; LEQ = Life Events Questionnaire; NLE = Negative Life Events assessed as modified version of Life Event Record; ILQ = Illinois Loneliness Questionnaire; BDS = Birleson Depression Scale; PASS-1 = Peer-social Attributional Style Scale; ASQ = Attributional Style Questionnaire; CDAS = Children's Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale; BHIF = Baltimore How I Feel; DISC-IV = NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV; CAPS = Child and Adolescent Perfectionism Scale; CRSQ = Children's Response Styles Questionnaire; RADS = Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale; PSI = The Problem-Solving Inventory; SSLES = Secondary School Students' Life Events Scale; Harter's SPPC = Harter's Self-perception Profile for Children; PNMC = Peer Nominations Measure of Competence; TRS = Teacher's Rating Scale of Child's Actual Behavior; PRS = Parental Rating Scale; PNMC = Peer Nominations of Multiple Competencies; BPI = Berkeley Puppet Interview; PNID = Peer Nomination Index of Depression; TRID = Teacher's Rating Index of Depression.
The nature and strength of associations between attributional style and depression may vary with development in function and/or content. We note Cole and Turner's (Cole & Turner, 1993; Turner & Cole, 1994) suggestion that attributional style mediates, rather than moderates, associations between life stress and depression in early- to mid-childhood. From this perspective, young children's negative attributions stem from adverse life events and are internalized as negative attributional styles, creating vulnerability to depression. During adolescence, a more stable attributional style interacts with life stress to produce depressive symptoms (Cole & Turner, 1993; Turner & Cole, 1994). Recent evidence supports this model. Attributional style mediated and moderated relations between life events and depression among fifth graders; whereas only a mediational role was present in fourth graders (Gibb & Alloy, 2006). Congruent with Cole and Turner's (1993; Turner & Cole, 1994) model, these observations suggest a developmental shift in the relations between cognition, life events, and mood.
On the other hand, we note the ‘weakest link’ hypothesis, which proposes that an individual's most depressogenic inference leads to vulnerability to depression (Abela & Sarin, 2002). As such, an individual's most depressogenic inference represents their degree of vulnerability. This model is descriptive in that individual attributions (global, stable, and self) appear relatively independent among youth (Abela, 2001). In contrast, stability of the weakest link among youth is moderate (test retest reliability=0.38) across six weeks. The weakest link model allows for the possibility that attributional errors can vary from setting to setting. In an investigation of this hypothesis among seventh graders, the interaction of life stress by weakest link predicted increases in hopelessness depression symptoms (Abela & Sarin, 2002). Recent tests of this model are also supportive (Abela & Payne, 2003; Abela et al., 2006). As such, the weakest link hypothesis represents a refinement of attributional models of cognitive vulnerability, but would be strengthened through: 1) explication of why a child would develop one weak link, as opposed to another, and 2) how a child's weakest link may vary over time and across settings.
Developmental psychopathology models suggest that the cognitive, social, environmental, and self-regulatory factors associated with risk to depression transactionally influence one another. The integration of reciprocal models to the study of attributional style represents an important conceptual advance. Latent factor growth modeling of longitudinal data illustrates the parallel increases in negative attributional style and depression severity. Adolescents with initially higher and increasing levels of depressive symptoms also demonstrate increasingly negative attributions (Garber, Keiley, & Martin, 2002). Attributional style and depression may be mutually dependent. In a comparison of mediation, moderation, and reciprocal models, a reciprocal model gained the most support, wherein initial levels of depressive symptoms predicted residual change in levels of stress and attributional style over the follow-up (Gibb & Alloy, 2006). Evidence also supports depression as leading to negative attributional style (Bennett & Bates, 1995; Gibb et al., 2006; McCarty et al., 2007; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1992). In sum, reciprocal models allow for dynamic modeling within a developmental psychopathology perspective.
Studies reviewed thus far support relations between attributions and depressive symptoms among youth. Attributions are related to past episodes and predict current and first episodes of depression (Lewinsohn, Clarke, Seeley, & Rohde, 1994). The interaction of life events and attributional style predict onset of MDD, but only at high levels of stress (Lewinsohn, Joiner, & Rohde, 2001). In the only study of children, evidence did not support attributional style as a vulnerability to MDD onset (Hammen, Adrian, & Hiroto, 1988). An attributional weakest link does, however, predict depressive symptoms among children of depressed parents (Abela et al., 2006). Such findings are preliminary and warrant replication.
Turning to measurement, we note several developments. Almost every study reviewed uses the Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire (CASQ; Seligman et al., 1984; CASQ-R; Kaslow, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). Many researchers, however, call for better instruments, acknowledging the psychometric weaknesses of the CASQ. The CASQ measures attributional style in line with the reformulated theory of learned helplessness, rather than hopelessness theory. One new measure, the Adolescent Cognitive Style Questionnaire (ACSQ; Hankin & Abramson, 2002), assesses the entire negative attributional construct proposed by hopelessness theory. Moreover, the CASQ has poor internal consistency (coefficient alphas=0.4–0.6; Gladstone & Kaslow, 1995). According to guidelines (e.g., Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994), internal consistencies below 0.7 may lead to increases in Type II error. Recently developed measures include the Children's Cognitive Style Questionnaire (CCSQ; Abela, 2001) and the Children's Attributional Style Interview (CASI; Conley et al., 2001) allow for more reliable assessment of attributional style. Future research can rely on these more psychometrically sound measures, thereby increasing the likelihood of accurately detecting developmental differences in attributional style.
It is noteworthy, however, that despite measurement limitations, the bulk of the current evidence remains supportive of attributional style as a cognitive vulnerability. Evidence is more consistent among adolescent, as opposed to child, samples. Research in this area will be facilitated by the recent development of structured interviews assessing the attributional style of young children (Conley et. al, 2001).
3. Dysfunctional attitudes
Evidence supporting dysfunctional attitudes as a vulnerability to depression derives from a set of studies. Following theory, which places dysfunctional attitudes at the center of the etiology of depression (Beck 1967, 1983), prospective studies with adults support the role of dysfunctional attitudes in the development of depression (Alloy et al., 1999; Joiner, Metalsky, Lew, & Klocek, 1999; Kwon & Oei, 1994). Results from the Temple–Wisconsin Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression project (CVD; Alloy et al., 2006) indicate that first onset of a depressive disorder is significantly more likely among individuals with high levels of dysfunctional attitudes and a negative attributional style than among individuals with low levels of these cognitions. In another example, increases in depression symptoms result from the interaction of dysfunctional attitudes and a negative university admissions life stressor among high school seniors (Abela & D'Alessandro, 2002). These data support dysfunctional attitudes as a cognitive vulnerability to depression among adults.
3.1. Review
Six studies among youth (Abela & Sullivan, 2003; Lewinsohn et al., 1994; Lewinsohn et al., 2001; McCreary, Joiner, Schmidt, & Ialongo, 2004) prospectively assess the effects of dysfunctional attitudes on depression (see Table 1). In all studies, dysfunctional attitudes, either alone or in interaction with life stress, predict depression. The interaction of dysfunctional attitudes and life stress predicts a diagnosis of depression at the level of a trend (Lewinsohn et al., 2001). This example is a conservative test; however, as the effects of important covariates such as co-morbid non-mood disorders and family psychiatric history are controlled. ‘Pessimism’ (which includes items from Weissman and Beck's (1978) scale) predicts first onset of depression (Lewinsohn et al., 1994). Higher levels of dysfunctional attitudes are observed among girls who met clinical cutoff scores for depression over the course of two years (Marcotte, Levesque, & Fortin, 2006). Overall, evidence supports dysfunctional attitudes in the prediction of MDD among youth.
However, it is not clear that dysfunctional attitudes serve as a cognitive vulnerability strictly to first onset depression, since ‘pessimism’ is also associated with current and past depression (Lewinsohn et al., 1994). More pernicious and stable maladaptive cognitions may result from repeated activation during episodes of depressed mood. Dysfunctional attitudes may increase vulnerability to first onset of major depression during adolescence, as well as increase vulnerability to future episodes. Parallel to findings within the attributional style literature, dysfunctional attitudes may transactionally relate to mood.
Dysfunctional attitudes have not been explored in children until recently. This work is facilitated by the development of the Children's Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (CDAS; D'Allessandro, & Abela, 2000). A test of Beck's cognitive diathesis–stress theory of depression revealed a significant interaction of dysfunctional attitudes by hassles, but only among children with high self-esteem (Abela & Sullivan, 2003). The self-esteem effect ran contrary to hypotheses. Another study identified a significant interaction – this time in line with hypotheses – among children with high levels of dysfunctional attitudes and low levels of self-esteem in the prediction of moderately severe depression (Abela & Skitch, 2007). Contrary to hypothesis, however, this interaction also applied to children with low levels of dysfunctional attitudes and high self-esteem. Age did not modify these relations despite children as young as six being included, suggesting that young children can experience the deleterious effects of dysfunctional attitudes. In sum, studies with children yield complex results, which may be due to developmental or measurement issues. Self-esteem in particular appears to impact the way in which dysfunctional attitudes impact children at risk for depression. Within adolescent samples, dysfunctional attitudes appear to represent a vulnerability factor; however, it has yet to be demonstrated that the reverse relation is not also true.
4. Self-perception
A competency-based model asserts that negative events in a child's life lead to maladaptive self-cognitions that predispose a child to depression (Cole, 1990). Cole's model posits that negative self-perceptions regarding competence may serve as a cognitive vulnerability factor for depression. This reasoning is congruent with cognitive models of depression in emphasizing self-schemata as proximal to depression onset (e.g., Abramson et al., 1978, 1988). Negative self-perceptions are believed to result from the negative competency evaluations of significant others, such as parents and teachers. A child's self-perception of competency may interact with others' appraisals to influence depression. Studies testing Cole's model represent a majority of existing research in this area. We examine the existing literature to evaluate self-perception as a cognitive vulnerability to depression in youth.
Definitional issues are important when reviewing this literature. Self-concept and self-esteem represent broad constructs and encompass a range of components including cognitive processes, personality style, affective processes, and motivational domains. Harter's (1985) scale of self-perceived competence is one of the most frequently employed measures of self-esteem among youth. Germane to research on vulnerability for depression are scales assessing perceptions of personal competence, rather than omnibus measures of self-worth. Accordingly, we do not review studies that include only Harter's general self-worth scale. This stems from a desire to incorporate a wide range of possible cognitive vulnerabilities, yet still restrict our review to cognitive, as opposed to personality, phenomena.
4.1. Review
Table 1 presents prospective studies that explore self-perception and depression. Evidence supporting negative self-perception as a proximal vulnerability to depressive symptoms is found in eight (Cole, Jacquez, & Maschman, 2001; Cole, Martin, & Powers, 1997; Cole, Martin, Powers, & Truglio, 1996; Hilsman & Garber, 1995; Kistner, Balthazor, Risi, & Burton, 1999; Measelle et al., 1998; Ohannessian, Lerner, Lerner, & von Eye, 1999; Tram & Cole, 2000) of the fourteen studies. Mixed evidence is found in three studies (Cole, Martin, Peeke, Seroczynski, & Fier, 1999; Hoffman, Cole, Martin, Tram, & Seroczynski, 2000; Kistner, David-Ferdon, Repper, & Joiner, 2006), with an additional three studies (Cole, Martin, Peeke, Seroczynski, & Hoffman, 1998; Lewinsohn et al., 1994; McGrath & Repetti, 2002) presenting evidence that depression predicts self-perception. Thus, the empirical base for self-perception as a cognitive vulnerability in youth is decidedly mixed.
Evidence that self-perception results from depression, or that the relation may be reciprocal, is drawn from a few well-designed studies. Among fourth graders followed prospectively through sixth grade, depression symptoms predict change in children's negative self-evaluations (McGrath & Repetti, 2002). Reciprocal relations are also illustrated in a study of third and fifth graders (Kistner et al., 2006). Similarly, inaccurate self-perception predicts increases in depressive symptoms and depressive symptoms, in turn, predict decreased accuracy in self-perception (Kistner et al., 2006). Academic overestimation predicts depression at many grade levels, but the reverse relation yields stronger effects (Cole, 1999). Underestimated competency predicts increases in depression within few grade levels; however, the reverse relation is found in all grades (Cole, 1998). Partial support for reciprocal models is also found by Hoffman et al. (2000). These studies support the reciprocal, or transactional, relations between self-perception and depression.
Yet another possibility is that relations shift across development with self-perception leading to depression earlier in development, while later in development the opposite relation may emerge. A developmental task of middle childhood is the construction of a personal sense of one's own competencies (Garber, 1984). Children may become increasingly capable of drawing realistic judgments about their competence as they grow. From a developmental psychopathology framework, it is plausible that normative developmental stressors influence emerging perception regarding competence, thereby increasing risk of depressive symptoms (e.g., Cicchetti & Toth, 1998). As such, inaccurate or negative self-perception may serve as a mediator of the relation between life stress and depression. Tram and Cole (2000) assessed these effects among ninth graders. Support was consistent for a mediational model whereby negative events predicted changes in self-perception, and self-perception predicted changes in depressive symptoms. In contrast, little evidence for moderation emerged. In this regard, self-perception may represent a mechanism, whereby salient life events affect mood.
Few studies examining relations between self-perception and mood have included simultaneous assessment of life events. This is surprising and may contribute to discrepant findings, as adverse events may be necessary to activate a child's latent negative self-perception in a particular domain. In one study, a ‘grade deficit stressor’ was employed in a sample of sixth graders wherein children were asked to define their own level of acceptable grades (Hilsman & Garber, 1995). Children with negative self-perception of their academic achievement expressed more depressive symptoms after receiving unacceptable grades than did students without negative self-perception. The inclusion of life stress assessment in the study of self-perception must be incorporated in future research to adequately test the role of self-perception as a cognitive vulnerability factor.
We propose that the presence of reciprocal, or transactional, relations between self-perception and depression may account for the mixed state of the literature. Findings may also differ depending on which aspects of self-perception are studied, at which points of development assessments are conducted, and what levels of baseline depression are included. Temporal and causal precedence have not yet been well-established. The literature offers support for self-perception as contributing to depression, but offers parallel support for self-perception as resulting from depression. Self-perception does not predict onset of first-episode MDD among youth (Lewinsohn et al., 1994). Self-perception does, however, relate to current and past depression. Thus, it is currently unclear whether Cole's (1990) model maps well onto a clinical diagnosis of depression. The possibility remains that self-perception may simply represent a concomitant of depressed mood. We also believe, in line with Cole (1990), that early in development, self-perception may serve as a mechanism, whereby life events affect levels of depression. That is, negative self-perception may mediate this relationship. Later in development, self-perception may come to moderate this association.
Methodological difficulties may also contribute to discrepancies between studies. Harter's (1985) measure, although systematically constructed from a strong conceptual base, may contribute to divergent findings. Some subscales demonstrate minimally acceptable reliability (Harter, 1985), warranting psychometric refinement. Harter's (1985) multifaceted scale may also lead to confusion among younger children. Researchers may wish to use the Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence and Acceptance for Young Children (PSPCSA; Harter, 2002; Harter & Pike, 1984), which does not rely entirely on language comprehension and allows for assessment of young children. Lastly, it is possible that the construct of self-perception is too conceptually broad. Theoretical coherence in this area of study may lead to more congruent findings. In sum, research on self-perception among youth has not conclusively established whether it represents a cognitive vulnerability factor.
5. An integrative perspective
Overall, this body of literature implicates several cognitive factors in vulnerability for depression among youth. As we have seen, research to date is not definitive and has produced complex results. Over-reliance on self-report measures is a notable weakness. It is clear that children are capable of experiencing the type of cognitions under discussion; however, whether or not a child can reliably hold these cognitions consciously in mind and report them is currently debatable. Similarly, the questionable reliability of extant measures is problematic.
Due to these challenges, we propose that incorporating information processing paradigms into the study of cognitive vulnerability allows for a more developmentally sensitive and adequate test. Indeed, we note that Clark and Beck (1999) define the cognitive model “as an information processing theory… understood in terms of the structures, processes, and products involved in the representation and transformation of information” (pp. 109–110). According to the cognitive model, biases in information processing are core aspects of depression. Information processing paradigms, such as the Stroop and dot-probe tasks, measure cognitive processes outside of the child's awareness. Such measures may offer more objective tests of cognitive operations, while also allowing for experimental manipulation. Lower order phenomena, such as attentional biases, may represent the mechanisms that lead to cognitive vulnerability products. If this is the case, the developmental study of these processes would allow researchers to explore the origins of maladaptive cognitions. Moreover, information processing biases may represent markers for broader cognitive vulnerability factors. Studies incorporating both self-report cognitive content and information processing paradigms allow researchers to refine cognitive vulnerability models, fostering theoretical and empirical advancements in psychopathology research.
On the other hand, information processing paradigms often suffer from questionable reliability and validity. Relations between theoretical constructs and information processing paradigms are often unclear (Vasey, Dalgleish, & Silverman, 2003). Convergent validity with other measures hypothesized to tap similar underlying concepts is lacking. Similarly, little is known about the reliability of information processing measures (Vasey et al., 2003). This low reliability can reduce statistical power (e.g., Nicewander & Price, 1983). Moreover, as Vasey et al. (2003) point out, the clinical utility of these measures in children is not established. Nevertheless, information processing paradigms offer a valuable framework for conceptualizing and studying cognition in psychopathology (Vasey et al., 2003). While experimental paradigms have pitfalls, they do not have the same pitfalls as self-report methods. It is likely that higher order and lower order cognition reciprocally relate in the development of specific cognitive vulnerabilities. As we have seen, top down cognition constrains the manner in which information is processed at lower levels of the system, and vice versa (e.g., Dalgleish, 2002). The consequences of these two processes in interaction may be greater than either cognitive process in isolation. Integration of these methods would allow for the assessment of such interactive effects.
An integrative cognitive approach to vulnerability for depression represents a critical step in establishing a cognitive science base to guide empirically supported treatments (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 2007; Matthews, 2006). In order to advance this integrative perspective, we briefly review the information processing literature testing emotional stimuli in children and adolescents. The information processing studies we review are not longitudinal. However, we explore cross-sectional findings and discuss how these paradigms can be applied to the study of cognitive vulnerability. Samples include children and adolescents with a diagnosis of depression, varying levels of depressive symptoms, recovered depressed, and children of depressed mothers. We do not review the procedures involved in information paradigms, but refer the reader to Matthews and MacLeod (1994) and Garber and Kaminski (2000).
5.1. Review
Table 2 presents studies of information processing biases and depression among children and adolescents. Level of depression is associated with greater recall of negative information relative to positive information in youth (Bishop, Dalgleish, & Yule, 2004; Cole & Jordan, 1995; Drummond, Dritschel, Astell, O'Carroll, & Dalgleish, 2006; Rudolph, Hammen, & Burge, 1997; Taylor & Ingram, 1999; Zupan, Hammen, & Jaenicke, 1987; for null results see Dalgleish et al., 2003; and Hammen & Zupan, 1984). This association is also found among children and adolescents with a diagnosis of MDD (Neshat-Doost, Moradi, Taghavi, Yule, & Dalgleish, 2000). Depressed youth demonstrate higher rates of rehearsal of negative memories (Kuyken & Howell, 2000). They recall positive information less well (Whitman & Leitenberg, 1990) and reveal significantly fewer positive autobiographical memories (Drummond et al., 2006). Lower rates of positive adjective endorsement also occur among psychiatric inpatient youth (Gencoz, Voelz, Gencoz, Pettit, & Joiner, 2001). Furthermore, depressed youth rate more negative words as self-descriptive (Timbremont & Braet, 2004). Youth at risk for depression demonstrate memory biases for negative self-descriptions (Hammen, 1988). These self-descriptions can interact with life stress to result in onset or exacerbation of depression (Hammen & Goodman-Brown, 1990). In sum, information processing paradigms reveal a bias toward negative stimuli among youth with symptoms or a diagnosis of depression.
Table 2.
Study | N | Age (range) mean | Depression measure | Sample | Cognitive measure | Summary of findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bishop et al. (2004) | 121 | (5–11) | DSRS | High and low non-clinically depressed children as assessed by DSRS) from school | Emotional stories recall task | Higher depression showed enhanced recall of negative stories relative to positive stories compared to low depressed group. |
Cole and Jordan (1995) | 394 | (9–15) | CDI | School | Incidental recall | Higher depression recall fewer positive self-referential words, only significant in grade 8. |
Dalgleish et al. (1997) | 80 | (9–18) | DRSS | 15 diagnosed depressed, 22 diagnosed anxious, 43 controls | Dot probe, Stroop, subjective probability, and word memory | Depressed estimated negative events equally likely to happen to them as others. |
Dalgleish et al. (1998) | 71 | (9–18) | MFQ | 24 recovered depressed (K-SADS) and 47 school control | Subjective Probability Questionnaire | Recovered depressed rate negative events as less likely to occur, all estimated negative more likely to happen to others than themselves. |
Dalgleish et al. (2003) | 67 | (7–18) | DSRS | MDD, PTSD, GAD patients and school control | Dot probe, Stroop, subjective probability, and word memory | No depression biases in comparison to other groups, but depressed group was significantly higher than others on anxiety self-report. |
Drummond et al. (2006) | 70 | (7–11) | CDI | School sample with CDI | cutoff AMT | Young dysphoric children recall more specific negative and fewer specific positive. Older dysphoric difficulty in retrieving specific negative memories. In general, children's specific negative recall improves with age. Dysphoric had significantly fewer positive AMs. |
Gencoz et al. (2001) | 58 | (9–17) | CDI | Psychiatric inpatients with various chart diagnoses, 44% non-bipolar mood | SRET | Lower rates of positive adjective endorsement and recall associated with depression, but not anxiety. |
Hadwin et al. (2003) | 38 in exp 1 | (6–10) | CDI | School | Visual search for threatening and non-threatening faces | No effects of depression on search time. |
Hammen (1988) | 79 | (8–16) | CDI, K-SADS | 16 children of depressed mothers, 10 of bipolar, 18 of medically ill mothers, 35 of normal | Self-Schema Task | When controlling for baseline CDI, trend toward self-schema scores as predictor of affective diagnosis. |
Hammen and Goodman-Brown (1990) | 64 | (8–16) | K-SADS | 12 children of depressed mothers, 12 of bipolar, 14 of medically ill, 26 of normal | Self-Schema Task | Significant association between onset or exacerbation of depression and the experience of stressors relevant to child's self-schema. Particularly marked effect for children of depressed mothers. |
Hammen and Zupan (1984) | 61 | (7–12) | CDI | School | Incidental Recall, Self-Schema Task, | Non-depressed higher proportion of positive self-descriptive words (but not significant), no evidence of negative bias in depressed. |
Hughes et al. (1990) | 322 | (10–13) | CDI, PNID | School with cutoff | Recall and recognition | Significant interaction between group status and valence of story events for recognition. Depression symptoms impair memory for negative story events. |
Joorman et al. (2007) | 41 | (9–14) | CDI-S | 21 girls of mothers with recurrent MDD, 20 girls of mothers with no history of Axis 1 | Dot probe with faces | High risk daughters selectively attended to negative facial expressions. Control daughters selectively attended to positive facial expressions |
Kelvin et al. (1999) | 102 | 14.0 | MFQ | Community sample defined as at risk by level of emotionality | AMALSS with prime | Adolescents with high emotionality endorsed more negative self-descriptors after a dysphoric, but not neutral, mood induction. |
Kuyken and Howell (2000) | 65 | (12–18) | BDI-II | DSM-IV diagnosis of MDD recruited from community | AMT | Depressed more likely to retrieve memories from observer perspective and more recent time period. Also rehearsed negative memories and rated as more personally important. |
Kuyken et al. (2006) | 62 | (12–18) | BDI-II | DSM-IV diagnosis from SCID recruited from community | AMT | MDD and no trauma demonstrated overgeneral memory bias. |
Kyte et al. (2005) | 79 | 15.3 | MFQ | 30 patients diagnosed with first onset MDD (K-SADS) and school control | CANTAB, WCS, affective Go-No-Go, decision making | Depressed greater attention to sad stimuli, more impulsive when making decisions. |
Ladouceur et al. (2005) | 75 | (8–16) | CDI, BDI | 16 MDD diagnosed (K-SADS) from inpatient and outpatient clinics: 17 anxiety, 24 co-morbid depression and anxiety, 18 low-risk normal control recruited from community | Emotional n-back (working memory task) | MDD and co-morbid significantly longer reaction times on negative emotional backgrounds compared to neutral. |
Martin et al. (2003) | 63 | 3rd and 6th graders | CDI | Children with low and high peer-rated popularity | Emotional Stroop with negative social words | For unpopular children, greater friendship valuing and greater negative social word Stroop interference predicted increases in depressive symptoms across 6 months. |
Murray et al. (2001) | 95 | 5.0 | None | 55 children of mothers with depression (SADS-L) and 40 control | Verbal and nonverbal expressions card game | When dealt a losing hand, exposed children made more negative expressions compared to non-exposed children. |
Neshat-Doost et al. (1997) | 64 | (9–18) | CDI, MFQ | 19 DSM-IV/ICD-10 depressed, CDI, MFQ cutoff, 19 mixed anxious/depressed and 26 controls recruited from clinics | Stroop | No significant differences. |
Neshat-Doost et al. (1998) | 38 | (10–17) | CDI, DSRS, MFQ | 19 patients with ICD-10 diagnosis and cutoff score on CDI, DSRS, and MFQ and 19 school controls | Recall and recognition memory tasks | Depressed group recalled significantly more negative adjectives, this depression related bias became stronger with age. No bias in recognition task. |
Neshat-Doost et al. (2000) | 55 | (9–17) | DSRS | 19 with DSM-IV MDD diagnosis, 24 school controls | Attentional dot-probe task | No support for attentional bias. |
Park et al. (2002) | 155 | (12–17) | MFQ, HDRS | 96 clinically referred adolescents with MDD, 26 non-depressed psychiatric, and 33 community controls (K-SADS) | AMT | Adolescents with current MDD more categoric overgeneral memories than controls, but not more than non-depressed. |
Park, Goodyer, and Teasdale (2004) | 134 | (12–17) | MFQ, HDRS | 75 first onset MDD, 26 non-depressed psychiatric assessed (K-SADS-PL) and 33 community controls | AMT with prime | Rumination increased overgeneral memories to negative cues in MDD patients. |
Perez-Edgar et al. (2006) | 33 | (6–10) | None | 16 children of parents with child onset depression (COD) diagnosed (SCID) versus community control | Posner task under neutral and affective conditions | Children of parents with COD were slower in their response rates compared with control children, subtle deficits in selective attention. |
Pine et al. (2004) | 152 | (9–19) | PARIS | 19 children of depressed parents (SCID) and 133 anxiety or healthy | Face memory task | MDD offspring significant deficits in memory selectively for fearful faces, but not happy or angry faces. MDD not associated with face-memory accuracy. |
Possel et al. (2006) | 92 | (13–15) | SBB-DES | School children divided into groups based on depression measure | SRET | No significant differences on SRET. |
Prieto, Cole, and Tageson (1992) | 50 | (8–12) | CDI | 15 clinic-depressed, 18 clinic-non-depressed, and 17 control children | Self-Schema Task | Significantly more positive words recalled by non-depressed children. This relation did not hold after effects of word recognition controlled. |
Reid et al. (2006) | 133 | (8–14) | CDI | School | Self-Schema Task, Attentional dot probe, Attribution vignettes | Psychopathology (externalizing and internalizing) was associated with hypervigilance for threat cues, negative interpretations of social situations, internal attributions, perception of hostile intent, and recall of negative self-descriptions. None of these relations were specific to depression. |
Rudolph et al. (1997) | 81 | (8–12) | CDI | School | Story task with recall and level of processing task | Depressed children significantly greater recall of negative maternal attributes than non-depressed on recall from story task. Non-depressed demonstrated greater recall of positive attributes on level of processing task. |
Swales et al. (2001) | 46 | 14.2 | BDI | Residential patients with ICD-10 psychiatric diagnosis and school control | AMT | More depressed less specific, but many patients recalling the same traumatic memory (parasuicide) over and over. |
Taghavi et al. (1999) | 67 | (9–18) | DSRS | 24 anxious, 19 mixed anxious-depressed primary diagnosis DSM-IV, 24 normal school controls | Attentional dot-probe task | No attentional bias in mixed anxious-depressed children. |
Taylor and Ingram (1999) | 86 | (8–12) | CDI | 40 offspring of depressed mothers (SCID) high-risk and 46 low-risk | SRET with prime | Primed at risk children demonstrated a less positive self-concept and a higher proportion of negative endorsed words recalled. |
Timbremont and Braet (2004) | 44 | (8–16) | CDI | 19 depressed, 15 never depressed, and 10 remitted depressed (CDI cutoff) in residential facility | SRET with prime | Remitted depressed rated more negative words as self-descriptive. |
Vrielynck, Deplus, and Philippot (2007) | 60 | (9–13) 11.5 | MDI-C | 15 depressed children, 25 never depressed clinical, and 20 control | AMT | Depressed children gave fewer specific memories. |
Whitman and Leitenberg (1990) | 52 | 4th–6th grade | CDI | 26 with score at least 1 SD above normative mean on CDI and 26 school controls | Word association task | Evidence that depressed recall positive less well. |
Zupan, Hammen, and Jaenicke (1987) | 81 | (8–16) | CDI | 20 children of women with depression (K-SADS), 21 not depressed | Self-schema incidental memory task | Depressed demonstrated recall of negative self-descriptive adjectives. |
Note. Age ranges and means reported when available. Grades reported when age data was not available. AMT = autobiographical memory task; SRET = self-referent encoding task; AMALSS = Assessment of Mood Activated Latent Self-schema; CANTAB = Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery; WCS = Wisconsin Card Sorting Task; DSRS = The Depression Self-Rating Scale; K-SADS = Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children; CDI = Children's Depression Inventory; SCID = Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders; SADS-L = Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia — Long; MFQ = The Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; ICD-10 = International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision; DSM-IV = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders — 4th edition.
Information processing paradigms also highlight overgeneral memory biases among depressed children and adolescents (Kuyken, Howell, & Dalgleish, 2006; Park, Goodyer, & Teasdale, 2002). Rumination appears to exacerbate this effect (Park et al., 2002). There is also evidence that depression impairs the memory of negative events. Children and adolescents with clinically significant levels of depression (as measured by the CDI) show impaired memory for negative events (Hughes, Worchel, Stanton, Stanton, & Hall, 1990). Depression severity is also related to less specific negative memories among adolescents in residential treatment (Swales, Williams, & Wood, 2001). Deficits in memory for fearful faces are exhibited among children of depressed parents (Pine et al., 2004). In contrast, no such effect is found in reaction times to the detection of threatening versus non-threatening faces (Hadwin et al., 2003). Lastly, depressed youth present with significantly longer reaction times to negative emotional working memory tasks compared to neutral tasks (Ladouceur et al., 2005). Thus, depression is related to memory biases in youth, but these relations are complex and worthy of additional study.
Research regarding relations between deployment of attention and mood is similarly complex. More attention is given to negative stimuli by depressed youth than the non-depressed (Kyte, Goodyer, & Sahakian, 2005), and slower response rates on an attention cuing task are found among children of parents with depression (Perez-Edgar, Fox, Cohn, & Kovacs, 2006). However, studies with attentional dot-probe tasks for words do not support attentional biases in depressed youth (Dalgleish et al., 2003; Neshat-Doost et al., 2000; Taghavi, Neshat-Doost, Moradi, Yule, & Dalgleish, 1999). Children with MDD are more easily distracted by negative pictures than neutral pictures, whereas control children are more distracted by positive pictures (Ladouceur et al., 2005). Moreover, recent work suggests that daughters of depressed mothers selectively attend to negative facial expressions, whereas control daughters selectively attend to positive facial expressions (Joorman, Talbot, & Gotlib, 2007). In this study, a dot-probe task was used, but faces instead of words were selected as stimuli. Of note, facial expression paradigms result in more consistent findings than verbal within the adult literature, as do studies using longer stimulus durations (e.g., Gotlib et al., 2004). In sum, relations between depression and attention in youth are not yet well understood.
This body of literature affirms that symptomatically depressed youth demonstrate memory and attention biases. However, these studies are overwhelmingly cross-sectional and do not explicate whether these biases contribute to the etiology or maintenance of depression. Results from longitudinal studies with adults suggest that information processing paradigms may reveal key processing biases underlying depression. Cognitive biases predict change in depressive symptoms in community samples (Rude, Wenzlaff, Gibbs, Vane, & Whitney, 2002). Pregnant mothers recalling more negative words on a self-referential encoding task demonstrate more symptoms of depression following childbirth (Bellow & Hill, 1991). Among adults with MDD, greater recall of positive words on the self-referential encoding task uniquely predicts a decrease in depression symptoms (Johnson, Joorman, & Gotlib, 2007). A tendency to shift attention toward negative information following an emotional prime interacts with life stress to predict increases in symptoms of depression seven weeks later (Beevers & Carver, 2003). Thus, there is good evidence that information processing paradigms are useful in predicting depression longitudinally among adults.
Among the youth-focused information processing research, only one study followed participants longitudinally and tested relations with depression (Hammen, 1988). A child's memory bias toward negative self-descriptions predicted affective diagnosis across six months (Hammen, 1988). However, this relation was only observed at the level of a trend. In a related literature, Martin et al. (2003) observed higher levels of Stroop interference among children who attached greater importance to friendships and whose peers rated them as having few friends. This interference predicts increases in depressive symptoms over a six month period. Martin and colleagues propose that the Stroop paradigm may be more sensitive to children's social concerns than traditional paper-and-pencil measures. A limited body of evidence suggests that information processing paradigms may predict depression across time. Clearly, more research is necessary to establish the prospective relations between these lower order processes and depression among youth.
The relations between information processing and cognitive content measures are relatively unknown among youth as well. In an adult example, individuals at high cognitive risk for depression in the CVD exhibited more self-referent information processing biases than individuals with low cognitive risk (Alloy, Abramson, Murray, Whitehouse, & Hogan, 1997). Similarly, among high cognitive risk participants, the self-referent information processing task battery (SRIP; Alloy et al., 1997) partially mediated cognitive risk effects (Steinberg, Oelrich, Alloy, & Abramson, 2004). Within the same sample, the negative SRIP composite interacted with cognitive risk to predict first onset, but not recurrence of depression. While there is some evidence that these constructs relate among adults, how these cognitive processes relate among youth is currently unknown. A recent study (Reid, Salmon, & Lovibond, 2006), however, interviewed children about their attributional style and gathered data on attention allocation and memory recall. Information processing and cognitive content biases were congruent and associated with psychopathology, although not to depression specifically. The integrative study of information processing biases and cognitive content vulnerabilities may result in theoretical refinement of cognitive models of depression among youth.
We conclude that information processing paradigms offer a useful tool for investigating cognitive vulnerability for depression among youth. However, the information processing studies reviewed assess youth with a range of internalizing diagnoses. Many of these studies did not propose specific hypotheses in relation to differing diagnoses, clouding the theoretical utility of results. Moreover, a notable distinction between the cognitive content and information processing literature is the use of emotional priming. Emotional priming – the experimental induction of mood for the purposes of tapping latent cognition – is used successfully within the information processing literature, but has not yet been incorporated into prospective longitudinal studies of cognitive vulnerability among youth. On the other hand, information processing studies rarely investigate the effects of life stress on the accuracy of information processing. Emotional priming prior to measuring cognitive content via questionnaires allows for a more powerful and adequate test of cognitive vulnerability models, as the induction of a negative mood state may allow individuals to report latent schema (e.g., Persons & Miranda, 1992). Integrating the assessment of stress, or implementing a stressor in the lab, would similarly result in an effective assessment of cognitive biases within information processing paradigms. Consequently, a combination of information processing paradigms and self-report questionnaires permits researchers to evaluate whether these higher and lower level cognitions do, in fact, influence one another in the development of psychopathology.
6. Discussion
Several cognitive models of vulnerability for depression have been proposed during recent years, and have attracted empirical attention. Derived from research on cognitive concomitants of depression among adults, they propose that the establishment of maladaptive schema, negative attributional style, and impaired self-perception may place youth at risk for major depression. Although evidence supporting each of these models has emerged, conceptual challenges remain, and no single model adequately accounts for the full range of factors implicated in risk for depression.
We envision significant advances in our understanding of cognitive vulnerability for depression through the incorporation of information processing paradigms into existing models. Capturing the relations between vulnerability factors and depression may be facilitated through the study of youth within a cognitive-developmental framework. Interdisciplinary collaboration between developmental and clinical scientists will allow for broad conceptual integration.
The use of emotional priming strategies and methodologies borrowed from research in experimental cognitive psychology may shed light on cognitive vulnerability factors among young children. According to Beck et al. (1979), schemata “may be latent but can be activated by specific circumstances which are analogous to experiences initially responsible for embedding the negative attitude” (p. 16). Germane to a discussion of latent schemata, the mood-state hypothesis proposes that cognitive vulnerabilities are accessible only during negative mood states (Persons & Miranda, 1992). This hypothesis is based on an associative-network model in which mood states cue related thoughts (e.g., Bower, 1987). Persons and Miranda (1992) argue that, because depressogenic cognitions develop when one experiences negative affect, cognition and affect are linked in memory. When not experiencing a negative mood, cognitions related to negative affect may be inactive. Adolescents may experience more negative life events than children (e.g., Ge et al., 1994), offering opportunities for latent cognitive schemata to become activated. The lower average rates of stress in early childhood raise the possibility that cognitive vulnerability factors may be present, but not activated. If, despite emotional priming, cognitive vulnerability factors do not appear to be associated with depressive symptoms among children as they are with adults, a developmental difference would be identified. Given the possibility that lower order and higher order cognition appear to develop in tandem and reciprocally influence one another, the inclusion of information processing paradigms will clarify the role of lower order and higher order cognitive vulnerabilities. In sum, latent measurement of cognitive vulnerability through information processing paradigms may allow for a broader understanding of cognition in psychopathology and address theoretical hypotheses regarding cognitive vulnerability models.
Have the theoretical hypotheses of cognitive vulnerability models gained strong support in studies of youth (e.g., Alloy et al., 1999; Ingram et al., 1998)? First, the stability of cognitive vulnerability factors among children and adolescents remains unclear. Data suggest that a degree of stability exists within adolescent samples, whereas among pre-pubertal youth, only short term stability of cognitive content have been reported. We note that Just et al. (2001) question the assumption that cognitive vulnerability represents an immutable trait. We propose that in the downward extension of cognitive vulnerability models to youth, allowance must be made for the variable nature of developing cognition. Second, findings to date have not demonstrated temporal precedence of proposed cognitive vulnerability factors. Future research must assess prior episodes and follow young samples to make the detection of these phenomena feasible and likely. Third, research suggests that a number of cognitive factors may be implicated in vulnerability for depression. These factors have rarely been examined simultaneously within the same sample. Little is known, then, about how they may interact in placing youth at risk for depression. Literatures surrounding each of the putative risk factors have evolved independently. Synthesis, through integrative research paradigms is needed. Fourth, some evidence has emerged for the specificity criterion (e.g., Gencoz et al., 2001; Robinson et al., 1995). Given the centrality of this hypothesis to cognitive models of psychopathology, additional research is needed. Last, the endogenous and latent nature of cognitive vulnerability in youth is likely. Cognitive vulnerability factors appear to reside within the child. However, an increased attention to ecologically valid life stress assessment, as well as the incorporation of information processing paradigms and emotional priming into research design, allows for more thorough investigation. In sum, the central hypotheses of cognitive vulnerability models have yet to be put to the test. We believe cognitive vulnerability research incorporating information processing paradigms will more adequately address these theoretical hypotheses.
6.1. Future research
Several lines of research will advance study and are detailed in Table 3. Framing the study of cognitive vulnerability within the broader domain of cognitive development will be essential. Age is, at best, a crude marker of ontogenetic change (Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). Specific markers of cognitive development must be realized. Sensitivity must be given to how depression and cognitive vulnerability manifest in relation to the development of cognitive capacities and the attainments of childhood and adolescence (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002). In addition to the integration of information processing paradigms, the inclusion of neuropsychological measurement will be informative. Tracking normative trajectories of cognitive and brain maturity will embed research on the etiology of mood disorders within a developmental context. For example, a widely used tool, the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB; see http://www.camcog.com), assesses executive functioning (for an example see Kyte, Goodyer, & Sahakian, 2005). Such measurement allows for analysis of specific links between changes in depression and the development of cognitive abilities. In sum, these lines of inquiry move scientists and clinicians toward a cognitive and developmental base for the study and treatment of psychopathology.
Table 3.
Assessment |
|
Analyses |
|
Interdisciplinary collaboration |
|
In addition, integration with research in developmental biology and functional neuroimaging will advance our understanding of the development of psychopathology among youth. For example, cortisol and psychophysiology assessment within studies of cognitive vulnerability may promote the identification of possible phenotypic and endophenotypic markers of risk (e.g., Gottesman & Gould, 2003). As Bearden and Freimer (2006) note, the study of heritable traits that can be reliably measured is fruitful in the study of psychiatric disorders. Identifying specific processes and behaviors, or ‘behavioral endophenotypes’, advances the study of psychopathology (Cacioppo et al., 2007; Prathikanti & Weinberger, 2005). In line with this argument, information processing paradigms may allow for pre-morbid identification of vulnerability at younger ages. Linking these literatures is imperative work for psychopathology researchers.
The study of cognitive models of vulnerability for depression has illuminated mechanisms and factors important for adolescent health. Much work, however, remains. This area is complex whereby individual, group, developmental, biological, and environmental factors impact cognition and depression. What is clear from existing cognitive vulnerability research is that adolescence represents a phase wherein cognitive vulnerability is likely to become apparent. However, given the present difficulty establishing temporal primacy of cognitive diatheses to depression, we recommend research with younger populations (e.g., ages 7–12 years) that incorporates information processing paradigms. A developmental framework permits assessment of the child, accounting for their stage of growth within the dynamic, continuous, and reciprocal interactions of the environment. Such investigation firmly places cognitive vulnerability research within the developmental psychopathology perspective. Not only will this work advance the scientific study of psychopathology, but it will produce implications for treatment and prevention of disorder among youth (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 2007; Matthews, 2006).
Acknowledgments
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by a F31 (MH075308) to Rachel H. Jacobs.
References
- Abela JRZ. The hopelessness theory of depression: A test of the diathesis–stress and causal mediation components in third and seventh grade children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;29:241–254. doi: 10.1023/a:1010333815728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abela JRZ, D'Alessandro DU. Beck's cognitive theory of depression: A test of the diathesis–stress and causal mediation components. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2002;41:111–128. doi: 10.1348/014466502163912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abela JRZ, Payne AV. A test of the integration of the hopelessness and self-esteem theories of depression in schoolchildren. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2003;27:519–535. [Google Scholar]
- Abela JRZ, Sarin S. Cognitive vulnerability to hopelessness depression: A chain is only as strong as its weakest link. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2002;26:811–829. [Google Scholar]
- Abela JRZ, Skitch SA. Dysfunctional attitudes, self-esteem, and hassles: Cognitive vulnerability to depression in children of affectively ill parents. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45:1127–1140. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abela JRZ, Skitch SA, Adams P, Hankin BL. The timing of parent and child depression: A hopelessness theory perspective. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:253–263. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abela JRZ, Sullivan C. A test of Beck's cognitive diathesis–stress theory of depression in early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2003;23:384–404. [Google Scholar]
- Abramson LY, Alloy LB, Metalsky GI. The cognitive diathesis–stress theories of depression: Toward an adequate evaluation of the theories' validities. In: Alloy LB, editor. Cognitive processes in depression. New York: Guilford Press; 1988. pp. 3–30. [Google Scholar]
- Abramson LY, Metalsky GI, Alloy LB. Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review. 1989;96:358–372. [Google Scholar]
- Abramson LY, Seligman MEP, Teasdale J. Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1978;87:49–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Murray LA, Whitehouse WG, Hogan ME. Self-referent information-processing in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Cognition and Emotion. 1997;11:539–568. [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Walshaw PD, Keyser J, Gerstein RK. A cognitive vulnerability–stress perspective on bipolar spectrum disorders in a normative adolescent brain, cognitive, and emotional development context. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:1055–1103. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LY, Abramson LY, Whitehouse WG, Hogan ME, Tashman NA, Steinberg DL, et al. Depressogenic cognitive styles: Predictive validity, information processing and personality characteristics, and developmental origins. Behavior Research and Therapy. 1999;37:503–531. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00157-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angold A, Erkanli A, Silberg J, Eaves L, Costello EJ. Depression scale scores in 8–17-year-olds: Effects of age and gender. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:1052–1063. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bearden CE, Freimer NB. Endophenotypes for psychiatric disorders: Ready for primetime? Trends in Genetics. 2006;22:306–313. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2006.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT. Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT. Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. In: Clayton PJ, Barrett JE, editors. Treatment of depression: Old controversies and new approaches. New York: Raven Press; 1983. pp. 265–290. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF, Emery G. Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Beevers CG, Carver CS. Attentional bias and mood persistence as prospective predictors of dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2003;27:619–637. [Google Scholar]
- Bellow M, Hill AB. Schematic processing and the prediction of depression following childbirth. Personality and Individual Differences. 1991;12:943–949. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett DS, Bates JE. Prospective models of depressive symptoms in early adolescence: Attributional style, stress, and support. Journal of Early Adolescence. 1995;15:299–315. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop SJ, Dalgleish T, Yule W. Memory for emotional stories in high and low depressed children. Memory. 2004;12:214–230. doi: 10.1080/09658210244000667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower GH. Commentary on mood and memory. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1987;25:443–455. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(87)90052-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brozina K, Abela JRZ. Symptoms of depression and anxiety in children: Specificity of the hopelessness theory. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:515–527. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown GW, Harris TO. Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. New York: Wiley; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Amaral DG, Blanchard JJ, Cameron JL, Carter CS, Crews D, et al. Social neuroscience progress and implications for mental health. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2007;2:99–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00032.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:6–20. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Toth SL. The development of depression in children and adolescents. American Psychologist. 1998;53:221–241. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark DA, Beck AT. Scientific foundations of cognitive theory of depression. New York: John Wiley; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA. Relation of social and academic competence to depressive symptoms in childhood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1990;99:422–429. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.99.4.422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA, Jacquez FM, Maschman TL. Social origins of depressive cognitions: A longitudinal study of self-perceived competence in children. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001;25:377–395. [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA, Jordan AE. Competence and memory: Integrating psychosocial and cognitive correlates of child depression. Child Development. 1995;66:459–473. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00883.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA, Martin JM, Peeke LA, Seroczynski AD, Fier J. Children's over- and underestimation of academic competence: A longitudinal study of gender differences, depression, and anxiety. Child Development. 1999;70:459–473. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA, Martin JM, Peeke LA, Seroczynski AD, Hoffman K. Are negative cognitive errors predictive or reflective of depressive symptoms in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998;107:481–496. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.3.481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA, Martin JM, Powers B. A competency-based model of child depression: A longitudinal study of peer, parent, teacher, and self-evaluations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:505–514. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01537.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA, Martin JM, Powers B, Truglio R. Modeling causal relations between academic and social competence and depression: A multitrait–multimethod longitudinal study of children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1996;105:258–270. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.105.2.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA, Turner JE. Models of cognitive mediation and moderation in child depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993;102:271–281. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.102.2.271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conley CS, Haines BA, Hilt LM, Metalsky GI. The Children's Attributional Style Interview: Developmental tests of cognitive diathesis–stress theories of depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;20:445–463. doi: 10.1023/a:1010451604161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costello EJ, Angold A, Burns BJ, Erkanli A, Stangl DK, Tweed DL. The Great Smoky Mountains Study of Youth: Functional impairment and serious emotional disturbance. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1996;53:1137–1143. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830120077013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Alessandro DU, Abela JRZ. The Children's Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. McGill University; 2000. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Dalgleish T. Information processing approaches to emotion. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH, editors. Handbook of Affective Sciences. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 661–673. [Google Scholar]
- Dalgleish T, Neshat-Doost H, Taghavi R, Moradi A, Yule W, Canterbury R, et al. Information processing in recovered depressed children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39:1031–1035. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalgleish T, Taghavi R, Neshat-Doost H, Moradi A, Canterbury R, Yule W. Patterns of processing bias for emotional information across clinical disorders: A comparison of attention, memory, and prospective cognition in children and adolescents with depression, generalized anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2003;32:10–21. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3201_02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalgleish T, Taghavi R, Neshat-Doost H, Moradi A, Yule W, Canterbury R. Information processing in clinically depressed and anxious children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:535–541. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01540.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demetriou A, Christou C, Spanoudis G, Platsidou M. The development of mental processing: Efficiency, working memory, and thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2002;67(1) Serial No. 268. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demetriou A, Efklides A, Platsidou M. The architecture and dynamics of developing mind: Experiential structuralism as a frame for unifying cognitive development theories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1993;58(56) Serial No. 234. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dixon JF, Ahrens AH. Stress and attributional style as predictors of self-reported depression in children. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1992;16:623–634. [Google Scholar]
- Donald M. A mind so rare: The development of human consciousness. New York: Norton; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Drummond LE, Dritschel B, Astell A, O'Carroll RE, Dalgleish T. Effects of age, dysphoria, and emotion-focusing on autobiographical memory specificity in children. Cognition and Emotion. 2006;20:488–505. doi: 10.1080/02699930500341342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garber J. Classification of childhood psychopathology: A developmental perspective. Child Development. 1984;55:30–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garber J, Kaminski KM. Laboratory and performance-based measures of depression in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2000;29:509–525. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP2904_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garber J, Keiley MK, Martin NC. Developmental trajectories of adolescents' depressive symptoms: Predictors of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:79–95. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ge X, Lorenz FO, Conger RD, Elder GH, Simons RL. Trajectories of stressful life events and depressive symptoms during adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30:467–483. [Google Scholar]
- Gencoz T, Voelz ZR, Gencoz F, Pettit JW, Joiner TE. Specificity of information processing styles to depressive symptoms in youth psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;29:255–262. doi: 10.1023/a:1010385832566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibb BE, Alloy LB. A prospective test of the hopelessness theory of depression in children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:264–274. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibb BE, Alloy LB, Walshaw PD, Comer JS, Shen GH, Villari A. Predictors of attributional style change in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34:425–439. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9022-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gladstone TR, Kaslow NJ. Depression and attributions in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1995;25:297–305. doi: 10.1007/BF01447664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gotlib IH, Kasch KL, Traill S, Joormann J, Arnow BA, Johnson SL. Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:386–398. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant KE, Compas BE, Stuhlmacher AF, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Halper JA. Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Measurement issues and prospective effects. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004a;33:412–425. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3302_23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant KE, Compas BE, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Gipson PY. Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Measurement issues and prospective effects. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004b;33:412–425. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3302_23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadwin JA, Donnelly N, French CC, Richards A, Watts A, Daley D. The influence of children's self-report trait anxiety and depression on visual search for emotional faces. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:432–444. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammen C. Self-cognitions, stressful events, and the prediction of depression in children of depressed mothers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1988;6:347–360. doi: 10.1007/BF00913805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammen C, Adrian C, Hiroto D. A longitudinal test of the attributional vulnerability model in children at risk for depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1988;27:37–46. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1988.tb00751.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammen C, Goodman-Brown T. Self-schemas and vulnerability to specific life stress in children at risk for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1990;14:215–227. [Google Scholar]
- Hammen C, Zupan BA. Self-schemas, depression, and the processing of personal information in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1984;37:598–608. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90079-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability–transactional stress theory. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:773–796. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Measuring cognitive vulnerability to depression in adolescence: Reliability, validity, and gender differences. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2002;31:491–504. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3104_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Siler M. A prospective test of the hopelessness theory of depression in adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001;25:607–632. [Google Scholar]
- Harter S. Manual for the Self Perception Profile for Children. Denver: CO: University of Denver; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Harter S. The Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence and Social Acceptance for Young Children. University of Denver, Dept. of Psychology; 2002. Available from Susan Harter, PhD, University of Denver, Department of Psychology 2155 S Race Street, Denver, CO 80208-0204; sharter@psy.du.edu. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harter S, Pike R. The Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence and Social Acceptance for young children. Child Development. 1984;55:1969–1982. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilsman R, Garber J. A test of the cognitive diathesis–stress model of depression in children: Academic stressors, attributional style, perceived competence, and control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:370–380. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.2.370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman KB, Cole DA, Martin JM, Tram J, Seroczynski AD. Are the discrepancies between self- and others' appraisals of competence predictive or reflective of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: A longitudinal study, Part II. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109:651–662. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.109.4.651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes J, Worchel F, Stanton S, Stanton H, Hall B. Selective memory for positive and negative story content in children with high self- and peer-ratings of symptoms of depression. School Psychology Quarterly. 1990;5:265–279. [Google Scholar]
- Ingram RE, Miranda J, Segal ZV. Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York: Guilford Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SL, Joorman J, Gotlib IH. Does processing of emotional stimuli predict symptomatic improvement and diagnostic recovery from major depression? Emotion. 2007;7:201–206. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joiner TE, Metasky GI, Lew A, Klocek J. Testing the causal mediation component of Beck's theory of depression: Evidence for specific mediation. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1999;23:401–412. [Google Scholar]
- Joorman J, Talbot L, Gotlib IH. Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:135–143. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Just N, Abramson LY, Alloy LB. Remitted depression studies as tests of the cognitive vulnerability hypotheses of depression onset: A critique and conceptual analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2001;21:63–83. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00035-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaslow NJ, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire — revised (CASQ-R) Emory University; Atlanta: 1991. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Keating DP. Central conceptual structures: Seeking developmental integration. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1996;61:276–282. [Google Scholar]
- Keating DP. Handbook of adolescent psychology. 2nd. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2004. Cognitive and brain development; pp. 45–84. [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Avenevoli S, Merikangas KR. Mood disorders in children and adolescents: An epidemiologic perspective. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;49:1002–1014. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01129-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelvin RG, Goodyer IM, Teasdale JD, Brechin D. Latent negative self-schema and high emotionality in well adolescents at risk for psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;40:959–968. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder: Developmental follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:709–717. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kistner J, Balthazor M, Risi S, Burton C. Predicting dysphoria in adolescence from actual and perceived peer acceptance in childhood. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1999;28:94–104. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2801_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kistner JA, David-Ferdon CF, Lopez CM, Dunkel SB. Ethnic and sex differences in children's depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2007;36:171–181. doi: 10.1080/15374410701274942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kistner JA, David-Ferdon CF, Repper KK, Joiner TE. Bias and accuracy of children's perceptions of peer acceptance: Prospective associations with depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34:349–361. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9028-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn D, Pease M. Do children and adults learn differently? Journal of Cognition and Development. 2006;7:309–312. [Google Scholar]
- Kuyken W, Howell R. Facets of autobiographical memory in adolescents with major depressive disorder and never-depressed controls. Cognition and Emotion. 2000;20:466–487. doi: 10.1080/02699930500342639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuyken W, Howell R, Dalgleish T. Overgeneral autobiographical memory in depressed adolescents with versus without, a reported history of trauma. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:387–396. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwon SM, Oei TPS. The roles of two levels of cognition in the development, maintenance, and treatment of depression. Clinical Psychology Review. 1994;14:331–358. [Google Scholar]
- Kyte ZA, Goodyer IM, Sahakian BJ. Selected executive skills in adolescents with recent first episode major depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:995–1005. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00400.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ladouceur CD, Dahl RE, Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Ryan ND, Casey BJ. Altered emotional processing in pediatric anxiety, depression, and comorbid anxiety-depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2005;33:165–177. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-1825-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewinsohn PM, Allen NB, Seeley JR, Gotlib IH. First onset versus recurrence of depression: Differential processes of psychosocial risk. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:483–489. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.3.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewinsohn PM, Clarke GN, Seeley JR, Rohde P. Major depression in community adolescents: Age at onset, episode duration, and time to recurrence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1994;33:809–818. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199407000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewinsohn PM, Joiner TE, Rohde P. Evaluation of cognitive diathesis–stress models in predicting major depressive disorder in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:203–215. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcotte D, Levesque N, Fortin L. Variations of cognitive distortions and school performance in depressed and non-depressed high school adolescents: A two-year longitudinal study. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2006;30:211–225. [Google Scholar]
- Martin JM, Cole DA, Clausen A, Logan J, Strosher HLW. Moderators of the relation between popularity and depressive symptoms in children: Processing strength and friendship value. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2003;31:471–483. doi: 10.1023/a:1025407730451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews A. Towards an experimental cognitive science of CBT. Behavior Therapy. 2006;37:314–318. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Reviews of Psychology. 1994;45:25–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.45.020194.000325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarty CA, Vander Stoep A, McCauley E. Cognitive features associated with depressive symptoms in adolescence: Directionality and specificity. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2007;36:147–158. doi: 10.1080/15374410701274926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCreary BT, Joiner TE, Schmidt NB, Ialongo NS. The structure and correlates of perfectionism in African American children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33:313–324. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3302_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGrath EP, Repetti RL. A longitudinal study of children's depressive symptoms, self-perceptions, and cognitive distortions about the self. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:77–87. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.1.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Measelle JR, Ablow JC, Cowan PA, Cowan CP. Assessing young children's views of their academic, social, and emotional lives: An evaluation of the self-perception scales of the Berkeley Puppet Interview. Child Development. 1998;69:1556–1576. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray L, Woolgar M, Cooper P, Hipwell A. Cognitive vulnerability to depression in 5-year-old children of depressed mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:891–899. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neshat-Doost HT, Moradi AR, Taghavi MR, Yule W, Dalgleish T. Lack of attentional bias for emotional information in clinically depressed children and adolescents on the dot probe task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41:363–368. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neshat-Doost HT, Taghavi MR, Moradi AR, Yule W, Dalgleish T. The performance of clinically depressed children and adolescents on the modified Stroop paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences. 1997;23:753–759. [Google Scholar]
- Neshat-Doost HT, Taghavi MR, Moradi AR, Yule W, Dalgleish T. Memory for emotional trait adjectives in clinically depressed youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998;107:642–650. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.4.642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicewander WA, Price JM. Reliability of measurement and the power of statistical tests: Some new results. Psychological Bulletin. 1983;94:524–533. [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS, Seligman MEP. Learned helplessness in children: A longitudinal study of depression, achievement, and explanatory style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:435–442. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.2.435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS, Seligman MEP. Predictors and consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1992;101:405–422. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.101.3.405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric theory. 3rd. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Ohannessian CM, Lerner RM, Lerner JV, von Eye A. Does self-competence predict gender differences in adolescent depression and anxiety? Journal of Adolescence. 1999;22:397–411. doi: 10.1006/jado.1999.0231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panak WF, Garber J. Role of aggression, rejection, and attributions in the prediction of depression in children. Development and Psychopathology. 1992;4:145–165. [Google Scholar]
- Park RJ, Goodyer IM, Teasdale JD. Categoric overgeneral autobiographical memory in adolescents with major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32:267–276. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701005189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park RJ, Goodyer IM, Teasdale JD. Effects of induced rumination and distraction on mood and overgeneral autobiographical memory in adolescent major depressive disorder and controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:996–1006. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00291.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perez-Edgar K, Fox NA, Cohn JF, Kovacs M. Behavioral and electrophysiological markers of selective attention in children of parents with a history of depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60:1131–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Persons JB, Miranda J. Cognitive theories of vulnerability to depression: Reconciling negative evidence. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1992;16:485–502. [Google Scholar]
- Petersen AC, Compas BE, Brooks-Gunn J, Stemmler M. Depression in adolescence. American Psychologist. 1993;48:155–168. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.48.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pine DS, Lissek S, Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Moulton JL, Guardino M, et al. Face memory and emotion: Associations with major depression in children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:1199–1208. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00311.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook J, Ma Y. The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55:56–64. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Possel P, Seemann S, Ahrens S, Hautzinger M. Testing the causal mediation component of Dodge's Social Information Processing Model of Social Competence and Depression. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2006;35:849–859. [Google Scholar]
- Prathikanti S, Weinberger DR. Psychiatric genetics — the new era: Genetic research and some clinical implications. British Medical Bulletin. 2005;73:107–122. doi: 10.1093/bmb/ldh055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prieto SL, Cole DA, Tageson CW. Depressive self-schemas in clinic and nonclinic children. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1992;16 [Google Scholar]
- Prinstein MJ, Aikins JW. Cognitive moderators of the longitudinal association between peer rejection and adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2004;32:147–158. doi: 10.1023/b:jacp.0000019767.55592.63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prinstein MJ, Chea CSL, Guyer AE. Peer victimization, cue interpretation, and internalizing symptoms: Preliminary concurrent and longitudinal findings for children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34:11–24. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid SC, Salmon K, Lovibond PF. Cognitive biases in childhood anxiety, depression, and aggression: Are they pervasive or specific? Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2006;30:551–549. [Google Scholar]
- Robinson NS, Garber J, Hilsman R. Cognitions and stress: Direct and moderating effects on depressive versus externalizing symptoms during the junior high school transition. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:453–463. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.3.453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rude SS, Wenzlaff RM, Gibbs B, Vane J, Whitney T. Depressive negative processing biases predict subsequent depression. Cognition and Emotion. 2002;16:423–440. [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph KD, Hammen C, Burge D. A cognitive-interpersonal approach to depressive symptoms in preadolescent children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1997;25:33–45. doi: 10.1023/a:1025755307508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Sroufe LA. Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and psychopathology. 2000;12:265–296. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz JA, Koenig LJ. Response styles and negative affect among adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1996;20:13–36. [Google Scholar]
- Seligman MEP. Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: Freeman; 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Seligman MEP, Peterson C, Kaslow NJ, Tenenbaum RL, Alloy LB, Abramson LY. Attributional style and depressive symptoms among children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1984;93:235–241. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.93.2.235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spence SH, Sheffield J, Donovan C. Problem-solving orientation and attributional style: Moderators of the impact of negative life events on the development of depressive symptoms in adolescence? Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2002;31:219–229. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3102_07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Southall D, Roberts JE. Attributional style and self-esteem in vulnerability to adolescent depressive symptoms following life stress: A 14-week prospective study. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2002;26:563–579. [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? Academy of Sciences Annals of the New York. 2004;1021:51–58. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg JA, Oelrich C, Alloy LB, Abramson LY. Negative self-referent information processing as a moderator vs Mediator of negative cognitive styles in predicting prospective onsets of depression. Temple University; 2004. Manuscript in preparation. [Google Scholar]
- Swales MA, Williams JMG, Wood P. Specificity of autobiographical memory and mood disturbance in adolescents. Cognition and Emotion. 2001;15:321–331. [Google Scholar]
- Taghavi R, Neshat-Doost HT, Moradi AR, Yule W, Dalgleish T. Biases in visual attention in children and adolescents with clinical anxiety and mixed depression–anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1999;27:215–223. doi: 10.1023/a:1021952407074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor L, Ingram RE. Cognitive reactivity and depressotypic information processing in children of depressed mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:202–210. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.2.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timbremont B, Braet C. Cognitive vulnerability in remitted depressed children and adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2004;42:423–437. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00151-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toner MA, Heaven PCL. Peer-social attributional predictors of socio-emotional adjustment in early adolescence; a two-year longitudinal study. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;38:579–590. [Google Scholar]
- Tram JM, Cole DA. Self-perceived competence and the relation between life events and depressive symptoms in adolescence: Mediator or moderator? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109:753–760. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.109.4.753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner JE, Cole DA. Developmental differences in cognitive diatheses for child depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1994;22:15–32. doi: 10.1007/BF02169254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vasey MW, Dalgleish T, Silverman WK. Research on information-processing factors in child and adolescent psychopathology: A critical commentary. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2003;32:81–93. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3201_08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrielynck N, Deplus S, Philippot P. Overgeneral autobiographical memory and depressive disorder in children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2007;36:95–105. doi: 10.1080/15374410709336572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss B, Garber J. Developmental differences in the phenomenology of depression. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:403–430. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman AN, Beck AT. Development and validation of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale; Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy; Chicago. 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Weissman MM, Warner V, Wickramaratne P, Moreau D, Olfson M. Offspring of depressed parents: 10 years later. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1997;54:932–940. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830220054009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitman PB, Leitenberg H. Negatively biased recall in children with self-reported symptoms of depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1990;18:15–27. doi: 10.1007/BF00919453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zupan BA, Hammen C, Jaenicke C. The effects of current mood and prior depressive history on self-schematic processing in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1987;43:149–158. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(87)90056-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]