Abstract
Objectives
Social cognition plays an important role in the functioning of individuals with psychosis. In this study, we explored two areas of social cognition not previously investigated early in the course of psychosis.
Method
Eighty-eight clinical high risk participants, 26 participants diagnosed with schizophrenia for less than 5 years, and 41 non-clinical control participants completed two measures of social cognition.
Results
Clinical high risk participants demonstrated biased responses to untrustworthy faces compared to both of the other groups. Early schizophrenia participants performed more poorly on an advanced theory of mind task compared to the clinical high risk and control groups.
Conclusions
There are different patterns of performance on social cognitive tasks in these groups, which require further examination in longitudinal studies.
Keywords: schizophrenia, social cognition, prodrome, social judgment, theory of mind
1. Introduction
Social cognition has been identified as an important contributing factor to social dysfunction in psychosis, particularly in chronic schizophrenia (Couture et al., 2006). There is also clear evidence of social difficulties in individuals recently diagnosed with psychosis and in those who are identified as clinically “high risk” (i.e., those who meet Criteria of Prodromal States, see Yung et al., 1998; Miller et al., 1999) for developing psychosis (e.g., Ballon et al., 2007; Pinkham et al., 2007). However, despite these social problems, less is known about the nature of social cognitive deficits in the early phases of psychosis.
The few studies that have examined social cognition in first-episode psychosis support the notion of a deficit in facial affect perception and vocal affect perception relative to controls (Addington et al., in press; Addington et al., 2006a; Edwards et al., 2001; Herbener et al., 2005; Kucharska-Pietura et al., 2005; Pinkham et al., 2007). The evidence is currently mixed on comparisons between first-episode and chronic participants, with some studies finding evidence for a comparable deficit (Addington et al., in press; Addington et al., 2006a; Pinkham et al., 2007), and others suggesting that participants with chronic schizophrenia perform most poorly (Kucharska-Pietura et al., 2005). One study also found evidence for impaired social perception (Addington et al., 2006b), and others for theory of mind deficits (Bertrand et al., 2007; Inoue et al., 2006) in individuals with a first episode of psychosis compared to non-clinical controls (NCC).
In studies examining individuals at clinical high risk (CHR) for psychosis, findings typically support a subtle deficit in CHR participants, with their performance on affect perception and theory of mind tasks falling in between first episode and NCC participants, without differing from either group (Addington et al., in press; Pinkham et al., 2007).
The purpose of the current study was to expand upon previous research by examining new areas of social cognition (i.e., advanced theory of mind and complex social judgments) in those at CHR and in individuals early in the time course of psychosis (i.e., ill less than 5 years).
2. Methods
2.1 Participants
The sample consisted of 88 CHR individuals, 26 individuals early in schizophrenia-spectrum illness (ES), and 41 NCC. Participants provided informed consent and the protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at each university.
2.1.1 CHR Participants
All CHR participants were recruited from the three sites in the PREDICT study: the University of Toronto (n=36), the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC) (n=33), and Yale University (n=19). CHR participants met the Criteria of Prodromal States (COPS) based on the Structured Interview for Prodromal Symptoms (SIPS; Miller et al., 2003). All participants met Attenuated Positive Symptom State Criteria, which included the emergence or worsening of non-psychotic disturbance of thought content, thought process, or perceptual abnormality over the past year. All relevant study staff completed a rater training program developed at Yale University. On the SIPS, the kappa was greater than 0.80 at all sites and the overall kappa was 0.90. In addition, all sites participated in weekly conference calls chaired by JA to achieve consensus diagnosis for every CHR case admitted to the study.
2.1.2 ES Participants
ES participants were recruited from a broader study examining neurocognition and social cognition in schizophrenia and autism at UNC. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Diagnosis – Patient Version (SCID-P; First et al., 1995) was used to confirm diagnosis. Research assistants were trained on the SCID-P to high reliability by one of the authors (DLP; ICC>0.80). Diagnosis was also verified from documented medical records. Participants were excluded if they met current criteria for substance abuse or dependence, had a prior head injury, or an IQ<70 as assessed by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale for Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999).
2.1.3 NCC Participants
NCC participants took part in the same study as ES participants. They were recruited from the community via mailings, mass emails, and postings. Participants in this group could not meet criteria for any current Axis I disorder, nor have relatives with autism, schizophrenia, Down’s syndrome, or Fragile X Syndrome.
2.1.4 Demographic Characteristics
Demographic characteristics for the 3 groups are displayed in Table 1. CHR participants were younger, and had a lower percentage of men and high school completers than the other two groups. The low percentage of women in the ES and NCC groups is reflective of recruiting these participants for matched comparison to an autism (i.e., high percentage of males) sample. The two clinical groups differed only on negative symptoms, with the ES group having more negative symptoms.
Table 1.
CHR | ES | NCC | Significance | |
---|---|---|---|---|
N=88 | N=26 | N=41 | Test | |
Demographic Characteristics | ||||
Age: mean (SD) | 18.9 (4.6)a | 24.9 (5.1) | 23.0 (5.9) | F=18.57** |
% male | 57%a | 88% | 93% | χ2=22.1** |
% completed high school | 56% | 85% | 73% | χ2=9.0* |
Race: | ||||
Caucasian | 88% | 69% | 89% | χ2=8.8, NS |
African-American | 7% | 27% | 8% | |
Other | 5% | 4% | 3% | |
Symptoms | ||||
PANSS positive symptoms | 12.4 (2.67) | 13.5 (3.49) | N/A | t=1.55, NS |
PANSS negative symptoms | 11.7 (4.82) | 14.4 (4.48) | N/A | t=2.42* |
PANSS general symptoms | 26.1 (5.25) | 26.8 (5.97) | N/A | t=.474, NS |
Social Cognition | Test for Group | |||
Trustworthy Faces | 1.28 (.77) | 1.46 (.82) | 1.34 (.66) | F=.050, NS |
Untrustworthy Faces | −.70 (.84)b | −.86 (.87) | −1.21(.54)b | F=4.03* |
Eyes percent correct | 69.6 (13.8) | 62.7 (13.7)a | 68.8(14.0) | F=6.31** |
Note. NS=Not significant;
p<.05;
p<.01.
significantly different from all other groups;
significantly different from each other; Trustworthy and untrustworthy faces are mean ratings in response to faces based on a −3 (very untrustworthy) to +3 (very trustworthy) scale, Eyes Test is expressed in percentage correct.
2.2 Measures and Procedures
2.2.1 Social Judgments
The Abbreviated Trustworthiness Task assesses complex social judgments (Adolphs et al., 1998). Participants were shown 42 faces of unfamiliar people and were asked to imagine they had to trust the pictured person with their money or with their life. They rated how much they would trust the person on a 7-point scale, ranging from −3 (very untrustworthy) to +3 (very trustworthy). The most trustworthy (top third/a score greater than +1) and least trustworthy faces (bottom third/a score less than −1) according to Adolphs’ normative scores were used to form 2 scales: the average rating on “trustworthy” faces and the average rating on “untrustworthy” faces. Adolphs and colleagues have scored the measure in this manner and found different biases between the two ends of the continuum in individuals with bilateral amygdala damage and in those with high-functioning autism (both groups provide more positive ratings to untrustworthy faces; Adolphs et al., 1998; Adolphs et al., 2001).
2.2.2. Complex Mental States
The Eyes Task was designed to assess adult theory of mind abilities (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). Participants were shown a pair of eyes and asked to choose among 4 words the one that best describes what the person is thinking or feeling. The percentage of correct responses was used as a summary score for this measure, consistent with previous research.
2.2.3. Symptoms
The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al., 1987) was used with CHR and ES participants to assess the severity of symptoms.
2.2.4. Statistical Analysis
Given differences in age, education, and a marginal difference in ethnic composition of the 3 groups, they were included as covariates in the main analyses. Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was performed for each social cognitive task, and, if significant, followed-up by Bonferroni comparisons. These analyses were then repeated controlling for the effect of gender, and then again with only males, given the gender differences across samples. Finally, Pearson’s correlations were computed for the symptom and social cognitive measures to assess any potential relationships. All variables met assumptions required for parametric tests.
3. Results
All results are presented in Table 1. For the Trustworthy Faces, there were no significant differences among the groups on trustworthy ratings. In contrast, there was a statistically significant group effect on Untrustworthy Faces. Post-hoc Bonferroni comparisons indicated the CHR group rated untrustworthy faces significantly more positively than the control group (p<.05), with the ES group falling in between the CHR and control groups but not significantly different from either one. Finally, for the Eyes Test, the effect of group was also significant. Post-hoc comparisons revealed the CHR and NCC groups did not differ from each other (p=.634), but the ES group performed worse on the Eyes Test than both groups (p<.05 for both contrasts). These results were unchanged when the analyses were repeated controlling for gender or with males only.
The correlations among the 3 symptom subscales of the PANSS and the social cognition tasks were examined both within group (ES and CHR separately) and aggregating across group; none of the correlations were statistically significant.
4. Discussion
The aim of this study was to extend previous research investigating social cognition in early psychosis and CHR groups. Neither theory of mind nor social judgments have received much attention in these groups. It is important to investigate these constructs as improving understanding of social cognitive abilities early in illness course can highlight potential treatment targets and illness vulnerability markers. On a test of advanced ToM (the Eyes Test), only ES participants demonstrated impaired performance. These results suggest that perception of complex mental states is not a vulnerability marker for schizophrenia. However, it is important to note that not all individuals at CHR will develop a psychotic disorder. Thus, these results need to be supplemented with longitudinal analyses.
On a task requiring participants to make social judgments about unfamiliar faces, those at CHR showed a bias whereby they rated untrustworthy faces as more trustworthy relative to NCC participants, but neither group differed from the ES participants. This finding is consistent with limited evidence suggesting subtle social cognitive deficits in those at CHR (Addington et al., in press). Although it may seem counterintuitive that those at CHR provide more biased ratings of trustworthiness compared to ES participants, it should be noted that it is not necessary for vulnerability or risk for psychosis (or any condition) to be stable and immutable. Indeed, Just and colleagues (2001) note in regard to cognitive vulnerability for depression, one might expect some variability to occur across illness course due to a variety of factors such as stress, previous experiences being ill, and life experience which occur both dependently and independently of illness. Judgments of trustworthiness in particular involve analysis of multiple sources of information, including facial expression and comparison of the presented face with those in prior experiences with similar-looking individuals. Thus, these complexities may result in ratings of trustworthiness to be vulnerable to change. Longitudinal studies of this construct aimed at predicting illness course and severity would shed further light on this issue.
In contrast, neither clinical group was biased in their ratings of trustworthy faces. This is consistent with previous findings showing that positive affect, particularly happiness, is easier to detect than negative emotions (e.g., Gosselin et al., 1995).
There was also a notable lack of a relationship between symptoms and social cognitive performance. There has been mixed evidence whether social cognition and symptoms are related, with some studies finding support for a relationship with negative or disorganized symptoms (Edwards et al., 2001; Greig et al., 2004; Herbener et al., 2005; Sprong et al., 2007) whereas others fail to find a relationship with any symptoms (Bertrand et al., 2007; Bora et al., 2006; Inoue et al., 2006; Kucharska-Pietura et al., 2005). One possible explanation for these discrepancies is the different symptom rating scales and social cognitive measures used across studies as well as varying methods of assessing this relationship (i.e., correlational analysis versus symptom subtyping). Efforts such as the MATRICS initiative (see Green et al., 2005) may help elucidate this subject further.
One major limitation of the current study concerns the low number of women in the ES and control samples due to the unavoidable recruiting needs in the larger study from which the data were collected. However, there has been no evidence for the influence for gender on trustworthiness ratings in the normative sample used for its development (Adolphs et al., 1998). In addition, although there is some evidence for gender differences on the Eyes Task in individuals with high-functioning autism or their relatives (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Losh & Piven, 2007), these differences do not exist in bipolar patients (Bora et al., 2005), or in relatives of schizophrenia patients (Kelemen et al., 2004). Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis of theory of mind showed that gender did not alter the effect size of patient-control differences in these abilities (Sprong et al., 2007). Finally, in the current study results were not altered by controlling for the effect of gender or removing women from the analysis.
In sum, these cross-sectional results suggest that understanding social cognition prior to illness onset, and early in the course of the disorder, may vary depending on the content of the task as well as the nature of task demands. This underscores that social cognition is not a unilateral construct and that vulnerability may be a reflection of tendencies to respond to social stimuli in a certain way, rather than getting them “right or wrong”. Larger social cognitive batteries as well as longitudinal designs will help to address this issue.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to acknowledge the research participants who participated in these studies. We would also like to thank Dr. Joseph Piven for his assistance in study design involving schizophrenia and non-clinical control participants.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Addington J, Penn D, Woods SW, Addington D, Perkins DO. Facial affect recognition in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. 2007. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Addington J, Saeedi H, Addington D. Facial affect recognition: A mediator between cognitive and social functioning in psychosis? Schizophr Res. 2006a;85(1–3):142–150. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.03.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Addington J, Saeedi H, Addington D. Influence of social perception and social knowledge on cognitive and social functioning in early psychosis. Br J Psychiatr. 2006b;189(4):373–378. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.021022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adolphs R, Sears L, Piven J. Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. J Cognit Neurosci. 2001;13(2):232–240. doi: 10.1162/089892901564289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio AR. The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature. 1998;393:470–474. doi: 10.1038/30982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballon JS, Kaur T, Marks II, Cadenhead KS. Social functioning in young people at risk for schizophrenia. Psychiatr Res. 2007;151:29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The "reading the mind in the eyes" test revised version: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger Syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2001;42(2):241–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertrand MC, Sutton H, Achim AM, Malla AK, Lepage M. Social cognitive impairments in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2007;95:124–133. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.05.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bora E, Eryavuz A, Kayahan B, Sungu G, Veznedaroglu B. Social functioning, theory of mind, and neurocognition in outpatients with schizophrenia; mental state decoding may be a better predictor of social functioning than mental state reasoning. Psychiatr Res. 2005;145:95–103. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bora E, Vahip S, Gonul AS, Akdeniz F, Alkan M, Ogut M, Eryavuz A. Evidence for theory of mind deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006;112:110–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00570.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couture SM, Penn DL, Roberts DL. The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: A Review. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:S44–S63. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards J, Pattison PE, Jackson HJ. Facial affect and affective prosody recognition in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;48(2–3):235–253. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00099-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders - Patient edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0) New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Gosselin P, Kirouac G, Doré FY. Components and recognition of facial expression in the communication of emotion by actors. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;68:83–96. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.68.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, Olivier B, Crawley JN, Penn DL, Silverstein SM. Social cognition in schizophrenia: Recommendations from the measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia new approaches conference. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31(4):882–887. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greig TC, Bryson G, Bell MD. Theory of mind performance in schizophrenia: Diagnostic, symptom, and neuropsychological correlates. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004;192:12–18. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000105995.67947.fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbener ES, Hill SK, Marvin RW, Sweeney JA. Effects of antipsychotic treatment on emotion perception deficits in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatr. 2005;162(9):1746–1748. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inoue Y, Yamada K, Hirano M, Shinohara M, Tamaoki T, Iguchi H, Tanooka Y, Kanba S. Impairment in theory of mind in patients in remission following first episode schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci. 2006;256(5):326–328. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-0608-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Just N, Abramson LY, Alloy LB. Remitted depression studies as tests of the cognitive vulnerability hypotheses of depression onset: A critique and conceptual analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2001;21(1):63–83. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00035-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13(2):261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelemen O, Keri S, Must A, Benedeck G, Janka Z. No evidence for impaired ‘theory of mind’ in unaffected first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2004;110:146–149. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0047.2004.00357.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucharska-Pietura K, David AS, Masiak M, Phillips ML. Perception of facial and vocal affect by people with schizophrenia in early and late stages of illness. Br J Psychiatr. 2005;187(6):523–528. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.6.523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Losh M, Piven J. Social-cognition and the broad autism phenotype: Identifying genetically meaningful phenotypes. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2007;48(1):105–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01594.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, Cadenhead K, Ventura J, McFarlane W, Perkins DO, Pearlson GD, Woods SW. Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: Predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(4):703–715. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Woods SW, Stein K, Driessen N, Corcoran CM, Hoffman R, Davidson L. Symptom assessment in schizophrenic prodromal states. Psychiatr Q. 1999;70(4):273–287. doi: 10.1023/a:1022034115078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Graham KA, Siegel M. Emotion perception and social skill over the course of psychosis: A comparison of individuals “at-risk” for psychosis and individuals with early and chronic schizophrenia spectrum illness. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. 2007;12(3):198–212. doi: 10.1080/13546800600985557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, Van Engeland H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatr. 2007;191:5–13. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. San Antonio, TX.: Psychological Corporation; Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) Manual. 1999
- Yung AR, Phillips L, McGorry PD, McFarlane CA, Francey S, Harrigan S, Patton G, Jackson HJ. Prediction of psychosis: A step towards indicated prevention of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatr. 1998;172 suppl 33:14–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]