Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008 Feb 7;33(4):455–461. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.01.002

Plasma c-peptide levels and rates of cognitive decline in older, community-dwelling women without diabetes

Olivia I Okereke 1,2,*, Michael N Pollak 3, Frank B Hu 2,4, Susan E Hankinson 2,5, Dennis J Selkoe 6, Francine Grodstein 1,2,5
PMCID: PMC2396343  NIHMSID: NIHMS48553  PMID: 18261857

SUMMARY

Background

Both type 2 diabetes and hyperinsulinemia have been related to diminished cognition. To address independent effects of increasing mid-life insulin secretion on late-life cognition, we prospectively examined the relation of plasma c-peptide levels to cognitive decline in a large sample of older women without diabetes or stroke.

Methods

Plasma c-peptide levels were measured in 1,187 “young-old” women (mean age=64 years) without diabetes in the Nurses’ Health Study. Cognitive decline was assessed approximately 10 years later. Three repeated cognitive batteries were administered over an average of 4.4 years using telephone-based tests of general cognition, verbal memory, category fluency, and attention. Primary outcomes were general cognition (measured by the Telephone interview for Cognitive Status [TICS], as well as a global score averaging all tests) and a verbal memory score averaging 4 tests of word-list and paragraph recall. Linear mixed effects models were used to compute associations between c-peptide levels and rates of cognitive decline.

Results

Higher c-peptide levels were associated with faster decline in global cognition and verbal memory. Compared to those in the lowest c-peptide quartile, multivariable-adjusted mean differences (95% CI) in rates of decline for women in the highest quartile were −0.03 (−0.06, − 0.00) units/year for the global score, and −0.05 (−0.09, −0.02) units/year for verbal memory. Each one standard-deviation increase in c-peptide was associated with significantly faster decline on the TICS (p-trend=0.05), global score (p-trend=0.04) and verbal memory (p-trend=0.006).

Conclusions

Higher levels of insulin secretion in those without diabetes may be related to decline in general cognition and verbal memory.

Keywords: insulin, c-peptide, diabetes, cognitive decline, aging

Introduction

Large-scale epidemiologic studies (Ott et al., 1999; Peila et al., 2002) have identified strong associations between type 2 diabetes and increased risk of dementia. The early stages of type 2 diabetes are represented by insulin resistance and compensatory increases in insulin production and secretion. Recent attention has turned to the question of whether higher levels of insulin may directly increase risk of cognitive decline, possibly in addition to vascular damage often associated with insulin resistance – even in the absence of clinical diabetes. Indeed, growing biologic and epidemiologic evidence suggests a more direct contribution of high insulin levels to development of Alzheimer disease (AD) brain pathology (Watson et al., 2003; Ferris et al., 2003) and risk of dementia (Peila et al., 2004; Luchsinger et al., 2004).

However, few studies to date have examined the effect of elevated mid-life insulin secretion on cognitive decline – independent of the presence of diabetes mellitus. Because cognitive impairment typically develops gradually (Linn et al., 1995), mid-life risk factors may be most relevant to identifying targets for prevention of late-life cognitive decline. In an earlier publication, we presented preliminary data indicating an association between higher mid-life levels of c-peptide, a measure of insulin secretion, and lower late-life cognitive performance in a sub-sample of 718 Nurses Health’ Study (NHS) participants without diabetes (Okereke et al., 2005). However, in the previous publication, participants had only one to two assessments of cognition, with less than 2 years of follow-up; this was a very short period for detecting meaningful cognitive decline, and the trajectory of decline is not well-measured with only two datapoints (Morris et al., 1999). Thus, in the present study, we substantially extend our previous work by increasing the follow-up to 4.4 years, including three repeated measures of cognition, and substantially increasing the sample size to approximately 1,200 women without diabetes.

Methods

Study population

The Nurses’ Health Study included 121,700 U.S., female registered nurses, aged 30 to 55 years at the study’s inception in 1976. Since then, participants have completed biennial mailed questionnaires updating information on lifestyle factors and health outcomes. Between 1989 and 1990, 32,826 NHS participants provided blood samples; health and lifestyle characteristics were very similar between the whole cohort and those who returned blood samples (Okereke et al., 2005). Total follow-up for women who provided blood samples exceeds 98%.

From 1995–2001, participants aged 70 years and over, and free of diagnosed stroke, were invited to participate in a telephone-based study of cognitive function, and 19,395 (93.3% of those eligible) completed an initial cognitive assessment. Since then, two follow-up cognitive assessments have been performed an average of 2 years apart. Follow-up remains over 90% for cognitive study participants.

Using stored blood samples from 1989–90, we performed c-peptide measurements in a random sample of 1,195 cognitive study participants who had provided a fasting blood sample, did not have diagnosed diabetes at blood draw, and had completed at least one follow-up cognitive assessment. Participation rates in the cognitive study were similar in those who had and had not provided blood, suggesting little possibility for bias in examining associations in those providing blood samples (Okereke et al., 2005). We excluded 8 women who reported high levels of alcohol intake (≥60 grams daily), as this potentially has a strong influence on both c-peptide levels as well as cognitive performance. Thus, the population for analysis was 1,187 women.

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA.

Measurement of fasting plasma c-peptide

C-peptide is cleaved in a 1:1 ratio in the conversion of proinsulin to insulin and provides an accurate representation of insulin secretion (Polonsky and Rubenstein, 1984; Wahren et al., 2000). Blood samples from the 1989–90 blood collection had been stored at −130°C or colder; plasma c-peptide was measured using antiserum M1230 in an alcohol precipitation non-equilibrium assay (Faber et al., 1978) with reagents provided by Diagnostic Systems Laboratory (Webster, Texas). Assays were run in a single batch, and in blinded quality control tests, the mean intra-assay coefficient of variation was 5.1%. In addition, a previous study demonstrated a within-person correlation coefficient of 0.57 between c-peptide measurements taken 4 years apart from a sample of health professionals (Ma et al., 2004), suggesting that one-time measures of c-peptide provide a reasonably stable representation of participants’ mid-life c-peptide levels.

Assessment of cognitive function

Participants were administered: the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS) (Brandt et al., 1988), a test of general cognition similar to the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975); immediate and delayed paragraph recalls of the East Boston Memory Test (EBMT) (Albert et al., 1991); a test of category fluency, in which women named as many different animals as possible during 1 minute; a delayed recall of the TICS 10-word list; and a digit span task, in which women repeated backward increasingly long series of digits, to evaluate attention and working memory.

Primary outcomes were general cognition and verbal memory – verbal memory, in particular, is a strong predictor of early AD (Small et al., 2000; Chen et al., 2001). To assess general cognition, we considered the TICS, as well as a global cognitive score, calculated by averaging the z scores of all six cognitive tests. For the verbal memory score, we combined the results of the immediate and delayed recalls of both the East Boston Memory Test and the TICS 10-word list, by averaging z scores of those four tests (Okereke et al., 2005). Global and verbal memory scores were only calculated for participants who had completed all component tests.

Reliability and validity of telephone-based cognitive assessments

In a test of instrument reliability, we administered the TICS twice to a sample of women at an interval of one month and found a Pearson correlation of 0.70 (p<0.001). Examining inter-rater reliability (interviewers all scored the same cognitive assessment), we found intraclass correlations >0.95 on each test.

In a validity study, 61 women who had completed an extensive in-person interview were administered our brief telephone-administered battery; we found a correlation of 0.81 comparing overall performance on our telephone-based tests to overall performance measured from the inperson interview. Furthermore, among 88 older health professionals, cognitive impairment as determined by our telephone method was strongly associated with clinically-diagnosed dementia three years later: poor performance on the TICS and in verbal memory was associated with significant 8- and 12-fold increased risks, respectively, of dementia – demonstrating strong clinical validity of our telephone assessment (Kang et al., 2006).

Data analysis

To examine the association of c-peptide levels with cognitive decline, quartiles of c-peptide were constructed, and the lowest quartile was used as the reference category. We also examined c-peptide as a continuous variable with the unit of analysis as a one standard deviation increment in c-peptide level (standard deviation=0.32 nmol/L).

We used linear mixed-effects models (Laird and Ware, 1982) to examine the relation of c-peptide levels to change in cognitive test scores across three assessments. In the basic mixed models, time since baseline interview (in years), age, highest attained education, c-peptide quartile, as well as interaction terms for time by age, and time by c-peptide quartile were included as fixed effects. Multivariable-adjusted mixed models also included the following potential confounders as fixed effects: body mass index (BMI, in kg/m2) categories (normal/overweight/obese), current smoking (yes/no), history of hypertension (yes/no), history of elevated cholesterol (yes/no), alcohol use (grams/day), physical activity (metabolic equivalents/week), current postmenopausal hormone use (yes/no) and history of antidepressant use (yes/no); all covariates were determined as of the time of blood draw, except age and antidepressant use, which were determined as of the baseline cognitive interview. Because depression is an important potential confounder of cognition, we evaluated multivariable models that further adjusted for depression using Medical Outcomes Short Form-36 (Stewart et al., 1988) mental health index scores, which were available in most participants; results were identical, and thus we did not include these scores in the final models. In addition to the fixed effects, we included two person-specific random effects in all models: baseline cognitive level (random intercept) and rate of change (random slope). In models using c-peptide as a categorical variable, the interaction terms for time and c-peptide quartile represent the annual rate of cognitive decline associated with that quartile, compared to the first quartile, across the three assessments (mean interval from first to third assessment=4.4 years, range=2.9–7.7 years). Similarly, in models treating c-peptide as a continuous variable, the interaction terms for time and c-peptide represent annual rates of cognitive decline associated with each one-SD increment in plasma c-peptide.

Three a priori, secondary analyses were conducted. First, due to the strong correlation between c-peptide level and BMI (Harris et al., 2002), we created multivariable models including and excluding BMI, to assess the impact of this variable on results. Second, to evaluate whether incipient diabetes, or “pre-diabetes,” at the time of blood draw might explain any observed association between c-peptide and cognitive decline approximately 10 years later, we excluded all women who developed type 2 diabetes between blood draw and the initial cognitive assessment (n=41). Finally, given the strong association between insulin resistance and cardiovascular risk (Hsueh and Quinones, 2003; Kernan and Inzucchi, 2004) and its potential influence on any observed relation between c-peptide and cognitive decline, we performed a secondary analysis excluding women who had any history of heart disease before or stroke as of the final cognitive assessment (n=62).

Results

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the study population at blood draw, across quartiles of c-peptide. Women in the highest c-peptide quartile were generally less healthy than those in the lowest quartile: higher c-peptide was associated with higher BMI, prevalence of hypertension and elevated cholesterol, and with lower physical activity. Also, women in the fourth c-peptide quartile were less likely to be using postmenopausal hormones than those in the first quartile.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the Study Population at Blood Draw, By Quartiles of Fasting C-peptide*

CHARACTERISTIC 1st quartile 2nd quartile 3rd quartile 4th quartile

Mean c-peptide (nmol/L) 0.36 0.52 0.69 1.11
Mean age at interview (years) 73.7 73.7 73.7 74.0
Mean age at blood draw (years) 64.1 64.0 64.2 64.4
Mean body mass index (kg/m2) 22.6 24.2 25.4 27.7
Master’s degree or higher education 8.4 7.8 7.1 5.7
History of hypertension 22.2 25.0 33.3 49.2
History of elevated cholesterol 26.9 34.8 34.3 40.4
Current smoking 6.4 9.1 7.1 10.4
Past smoking 36.4 39.9 41.6 46.1
Current hormone use 43.0 37.4 31.4 26.2
Past hormone use 22.4 26.3 28.6 33.3
History of antidepressant use 6.7 3.7 7.4 7.7
Mean exercise level (METS/week) 18.2 17.4 17.4 14.2
Mean alcohol intake (grams/day) 4.9 4.8 5.4 4.1
*

Figures are expressed as percentages, unless otherwise indicated.

Antidepressant use is as of baseline cognitive testing

Results from the linear mixed-effects models (Table 2) demonstrated that higher c-peptide levels were associated with faster cognitive decline across three assessments (over an average of 4.4 years). Compared to the first quartile, the multivariable-adjusted annual rates of decline (95% CI) for women in the fourth quartile were lower by −0.03 (−0.06, −0.00; p=0.03) standard units/year on the global score and −0.05 (−0.09, −0.02; p=0.004) standard units/year on verbal memory; in addition, there were significant trends of increasing levels of c-peptide and greater rates of cognitive decline for TICS (p-trend=0.05), global score (p-trend=0.04), and verbal memory (p-trend=0.006). To help interpret these effect estimates, we compared the relation of c-peptide with cognitive decline to the relation of age with rates of cognitive decline in our cohort. We found that each year of age was associated with a greater annual rate of decline by −0.01 units on the global score; thus, our finding of greater annual decline of −0.03 units comparing the highest to lowest c-peptide quartile would indicate that higher levels of c-peptide are cognitively equivalent to 3 years of aging.

Table 2.

Mean Differences in Annual Rates of Decline over Four Years, by Fasting C-peptide Levels (n=1,187)

Cognitive test 1st quartile 2nd quartile 3rd quartile 4th quartile Per SD* Increase in C-peptide
Age- and education-adjusted
TICS 0.00 0.03 (−0.08, 0.14) −0.03 (−0.14, 0.08) −0.03 (−0.14, 0.07) −0.03 (−0.07, 0.01)
p = 0.58 p = 0.54 p = 0.53 p-trend = 0.11
Global score 0.00 −0.02 (−0.05, 0.01) −0.03 (−0.06, 0.00) −0.03 (−0.06, 0.00) −0.01 (−0.02, 0.00)
p = 0.17 p = 0.05 p = 0.05 p-trend = 0.07
Verbal score 0.00 −0.04 (−0.07, −0.01) −0.05 (−0.08, −0.01) −0.05 (−0.08, −0.01) −0.02 (−0.03, −0.00)
p = 0.01 p = 0.01 p = 0.01 p-trend = 0.02
Multivariable-adjusted
TICS 0.00 0.02 (−0.09, 0.13) −0.04 (−0.15, 0.08) −0.06 (−0.17, 0.05) −0.04 (−0.08, −0.00)
p = 0.70 p = 0.53 p = 0.31 p-trend = 0.05
Global score 0.00 −0.02 (−0.05, 0.01) −0.03 (−0.06, −0.00) −0.03 (−0.06, −0.00) −0.01 (−0.02, −0.00)
p = 0.13 p = 0.03 p = 0.03 p-trend = 0.04
Verbal score 0.00 −0.05 (−0.08, −0.01) −0.05 (−0.08, −0.01) −0.05 (−0.09, −0.02) −0.02 (−0.03, −0.00)
p = 0.007 p = 0.008 p = 0.004 p-trend = 0.006
*

SD = standard deviation (0.32 nmol/L)

Adjusted for age at baseline interview and education.

Adjusted for age at baseline interview and education; cigarette smoking, postmenopausal hormone use, hypertension, elevated cholesterol, body mass index, alcohol intake and physical activity level as of blood draw; and antidepressant use as of cognitive testing.

In separate analyses, we constructed multivariable models that removed BMI as a covariate; results were unchanged compared to those presented above, where BMI was included in the model: e.g., the mean differences in annual rates of decline associated with being in the fourth c-peptide quartile, compared to the first quartile, were −0.03 (−0.06, −0.00; p=0.03) units/year on the global score and −0.05 (−0.09, −0.02; p=0.004) units/year on verbal memory. Findings were nearly identical in secondary analyses that excluded women who developed type 2 diabetes after blood draw (n=41): for example, the mean differences in the multivariable-adjusted annual rates of decline for each one-SD increase in c-peptide were −0.04 points (−0.09, −0.00; p-trend=0.03) on the TICS, −0.01 units (−0.02, −0.00; p-trend=0.03) on global score, and −0.02 units (−0.03, −0.01; p-trend=0.005) on verbal memory. Finally, results were also similar after excluding the 62 women with any history of coronary disease or who were diagnosed with coronary disease or stroke as of the final cognitive assessment (data for secondary analyses not shown in tables).

Discussion

In this study of community-dwelling women without type 2 diabetes mellitus, we found that higher mid-life levels of plasma c-peptide – a marker of insulin secretion – were associated with significantly greater late-life decline in general cognition and verbal memory over an average of four years. Specifically, being in the highest c-peptide quartile appeared to be cognitively equivalent to aging by three years. Associations remained strong after adjustment for numerous potential confounding factors – including vascular factors, such as hypertension and dyslipidemia – and after exclusion of women who were diagnosed either with diabetes at any time after c-peptide was measured, with any history of coronary disease, or with coronary disease or stroke as of the final cognitive assessment.

Our findings are consistent with previous epidemiologic studies linking higher insulin levels to cognitive impairment (Kalmijn et al., 1995; Okereke et al., 2005; Okereke et al., 2006) and cognitive decline (Luchsinger et al., 2004) in older adults. We previously reported significant cross-sectional relations between higher c-peptide levels and greater general cognitive and verbal memory impairment among 718 Nurses’ Health Study participants (Okereke et al., 2005); similarly, in a cross-sectional study involving 386 elderly men without diabetes, Kalmijn and colleagues (1995) reported that those in the highest quartile of fasting insulin had 25% more errors on the MMSE compared with those in the lowest quartile, with a trend of increasing errors with increasing fasting insulin (p-trend=0.02). Other studies (Vanhanen et al., 1998; Yaffe et al., 2004) have suggested associations between indicators of abnormal insulin (e.g., impaired glucose tolerance) and cognitive impairment: for example, Yaffe and co-workers (2004) reported significantly higher risk of developing cognitive impairment among older women with impaired fasting glucose but without diabetes, compared to those with normal glucose. Finally, Luchsinger et al. (2004) reported an association between hyperinsulinemia and significantly greater decline in memory scores, but not in other cognitive domains, among 683 older, community-dwelling adults; however, this sample did include people with diabetes, and participants were older when insulin levels were obtained (mean age=76.2 years). While consistent with these previous reports, the current study represents an important advance in the literature, as it is a large-scale prospective study, involves only participants without diabetes, and relates mid-life insulin secretion levels to later-life cognitive decline over repeated assessments.

Higher insulin levels may impact cognitive function indirectly through many mechanisms, including vascular damage associated with insulin resistance even in the absence of diabetes (Hsueh and Quinones, 2003; Geroldi et al., 2005); thus, despite our ability to account for major vascular factors such as coronary disease, stroke and hypertension in our analyses, the impact on cognition of the microvascular changes associated with high insulin levels cannot be excluded in the current study, or in the others mentioned above. Nevertheless, there is growing biological evidence that more direct effects of elevated insulin may exist. For example, a leading hypothesis suggests that high levels of insulin in the brain could interfere with metabolism of amyloid beta (Aβ, the primary component of neuritic plaques which are central to AD pathology) (Hardy and Selkoe, 2002) through their impact on the insulin degrading enzyme (IDE, the major enzyme responsible for insulin degradation and also an Aβ-degrading protease) (Farris et al., 2003). Because IDE binds more readily to insulin relative to other substrates (Duckworth et al., 1998), insulin acts as a competitive inhibitor of Aβ degradation; thus, higher insulin levels may interfere with Aβ clearance, resulting in higher Aβ concentrations in the brain (Watson et al., 2003).

Strengths of the current study include the large sample size, high rate of follow-up, and abundant information on potential confounding factors. In addition, participants had three repeated assessments of cognitive function over an average of more than 4 years, allowing us to examine the relation of insulin secretion levels to paths of cognitive change over time. Finally, c-peptide was measured from blood collected in mid-life – an average of 10 years prior to the start of cognitive testing; since cognitive impairment appears to take many years to develop (Linn et al., 1995), the level of exposure at these younger ages may be most relevant to dementia risk and ultimately to targeting dementia prevention.

Potential limitations of our study should also be considered. First, we had a single measurement of c-peptide, which may have increased measurement error; however, such random error would lead, if anything, to underestimation of the association between c-peptide and cognitive decline. Second, we utilized a telephone-based instrument rather than in-person testing; however, this cognitive battery has been demonstrated to have excellent reliability and validity. Third, we relied on self-report to exclude diabetic women; however, high validity of these participants' reports of diabetes has been established (Manson et al., 1991). Nevertheless, our sample may have included women with undiagnosed diabetes; thus, a strong association between diabetes and cognitive decline may partly explain our findings. This is unlikely, however, as our results remained similar when we excluded all women who were diagnosed with diabetes during approximately 10 years of follow-up after blood draw, likely excluding any women with undiagnosed diabetes at time of blood draw. Furthermore, in a validation sample of 200 randomly-selected NHS participants who never reported diabetes, only 1 woman had a plasma fasting glucose or fructosamine level in the diabetic range. Fourth, the generalizability of our findings is a potential concern: although basic biological relations observed in this largely Caucasian sample of well-educated women are likely comparable to those seen among women in the general population, further research involving ethnic minorities would be necessary to address potential differences in other populations. Finally, although we were able to adjust for numerous potential confounders, residual confounding is still possible, as may occur in any observational study. However, the relative homogeneity of our cohort reduces potential influences of many unmeasured confounders, such as health knowledge and access to care.

In conclusion, our study provides evidence for an association between elevated mid-life insulin secretion – in the absence of diabetes mellitus or clinical stroke – and late-life cognitive decline measured over several years. This association clearly requires further investigation, as addressing insulin regulation and secretion levels could lead to exciting preventive tools to combat cognitive decline and potentially Alzheimer disease.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants AG24215, CA49449 and CA87969 from the National Institutes of Health. Dr. Okereke is supported by an R01 Minority Supplement to grant AG24215. Dr. Okereke had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Footnotes

The authors have reported no conflicts of interest.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Albert M, Smith LA, Scherr PA, Taylor JO, Evans DA, Funkenstein HH. Use of brief cognitive tests to identify individuals in the community with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer's disease. Int J Neurosci. 1991;57:167–178. doi: 10.3109/00207459109150691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brandt J, Spencer M, Folstein MF. The telephone interview for cognitive status. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1988;1:111–117. [Google Scholar]
  3. Chen P, Ratcliff G, Belle SH, Cauley JA, DeKosky ST, Ganguli M. Patterns of cognitive decline in presymptomatic Alzheimer disease: a prospective community study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:853–858. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.9.853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Duckworth WC, Bennett RG, Hamel FG. Insulin acts intracellularly on proteasomes through insulin-degrading enzyme. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 1998;244:390–394. doi: 10.1006/bbrc.1998.8276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Faber OK, Binder C, Markussen J, Heding LG, Naithani VK, Kuzuya H, Blix P, Horwitz DL, Rubenstein AH. Characterization of seven C-peptide antisera. Diabetes. 1978;27 Suppl 1:170–177. doi: 10.2337/diab.27.1.s170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Farris W, Mansourian S, Chang Y, Lindsley L, Eckman EA, Frosch MP, Eckman CB, Tanzi RE, Selkoe DJ, Guenette S. Insulin-degrading enzyme regulates the levels of insulin, amyloid beta-protein, and the beta-amyloid precursor protein intracellular domain in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:4162–4167. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0230450100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Geroldi C, Frisoni GB, Paolisso G, Bandinelli S, Lamponi M, Abbatecola AM, Zanetti O, Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L. Insulin resistance in cognitive impairment: the InCHIANTI study. Arch Neurol. 2005;62:1067–1072. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.7.1067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Hardy J, Selkoe DJ. The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer's disease: progress and problems on the road to therapeutics. Science. 2002;297:353–356. doi: 10.1126/science.1072994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Harris MI, Cowie CC, Gu K, Francis ME, Flegal K, Eberhardt MS. Higher fasting insulin but lower fasting C-peptide levels in African Americans in the US population. Diabetes Metab Res Rev. 2002;18:149–155. doi: 10.1002/dmrr.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hsueh WA, Quinones MJ. Role of endothelial dysfunction in insulin resistance. Am J Cardiol. 2003;92 Suppl:10J–17J. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9149(03)00611-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Kalmijn S, Feskens EJ, Launer LJ, Stijnen T, Kromhout D. Glucose intolerance, hyperinsulinaemia and cognitive function in a general population of elderly men. Diabetologia. 1995;38:1096–1102. doi: 10.1007/BF00402181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kang JH, Cook N, Manson J, Buring JE, Grodstein F. A randomized trial of vitamin E supplementation and cognitive function in women. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:2462–2468. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.22.2462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Kernan WN, Inzucchi SE. Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus and Insulin Resistance: Stroke Prevention and Management. Curr Treat Options Neurol. 2004;6:443–450. doi: 10.1007/s11940-004-0002-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Laird NM, Ware JH. Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics. 1982;38:963–974. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Linn RT, Wolf PA, Bachman DL, Knoefel JE, Cobb JL, Belanger AJ, Kaplan EF, D'Agostino RB. The "preclinical phase" of probable Alzheimer's disease. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:485–490. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1995.00540290075020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Luchsinger JA, Tang MX, Shea S, Mayeux R. Hyperinsulinemia and risk of Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2004;63:1187–1192. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000140292.04932.87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ma J, Giovannucci E, Pollak M, Leavitt A, Tao Y, Gaziano JM, Stampfer MJ. A prospective study of plasma c-peptide and colorectal cancer risk in men. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2004;96:546–553. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djh082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Colditz GA, Willett WC, Rosner B, Hennekens CH, Speizer FE, Rimm EB, Krolewski AS. Physical activity and incidence of noninsulin-dependent diabetes mellitus in women. Lancet. 1991;338:774–778. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)90664-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Morris MC, Evans DA, Hebert LE, Bienias JL. Methodological issues in the study of cognitive decline. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;149:789–793. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Okereke O, Hankinson SE, Hu FB, Grodstein F. Plasma C peptide level and cognitive function among older women without diabetes mellitus. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165:1651–1656. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.14.1651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Okereke O, Kang JH, Gaziano JM, Ma J, Stampfer MJ, Grodstein F. Plasma c-peptide and cognitive performance in older men without diabetes. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:1041–1050. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000240983.25359.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ott A, Stolk RP, van Harskamp F, Pols HA, Hofman A, Breteler MM. Diabetes mellitus and the risk of dementia: The Rotterdam Study. Neurology. 1999;53:1937–1942. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.9.1937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Peila R, Rodriguez BL, Launer LJ. Type 2 diabetes, APOE gene, and the risk for dementia and related pathologies: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Diabetes. 2002;51:1256–1262. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.51.4.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Peila R, Rodriguez BL, White LR, Launer LJ. Fasting insulin and incident dementia in an elderly population of Japanese-American men. Neurology. 2004;63:228–233. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000129989.28404.9b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Polonsky KS, Rubenstein AH. C-peptide as a measure of the secretion and hepatic extraction of insulin: pitfalls and limitations. Diabetes. 1984;33:486–494. doi: 10.2337/diab.33.5.486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Small BJ, Fratiglioni L, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Backman L. The course of cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer disease: three- and 6-year follow-up of a population-based sample. Arch Neurol. 2000;57:839–844. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.6.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Stewart AL, Hays RD, Ware JE., Jr The MOS short-form General Health Survey: reliability and validity in a patient population. Med Care. 1988;26:724–735. doi: 10.1097/00005650-198807000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Vanhanen M, Koivisto K, Kuusisto J, Mykkanen L, Helkala EL, Hanninen T, Riekkinen P, Sr, Soininen H, Laakso M. Cognitive function in an elderly population with persistent impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes Care. 1998;21:398–402. doi: 10.2337/diacare.21.3.398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Wahren J, Ekberg K, Johansson J, Henriksson M, Pramanik A, Johansson BL, Rigler R, Jornvall H. Role of C-peptide in human physiology. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2000;278:E759–E768. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.2000.278.5.E759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Watson GS, Peskind ER, Asthana S, Purganan K, Wait C, Chapman D, Schwartz MW, Plymate S, Craft S. Insulin increases CSF Abeta42 levels in normal older adults. Neurology. 2003;60:1899–1903. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000065916.25128.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Kanaya AM, Davidowitz N, Barrett-Connor E, Krueger K. Diabetes, impaired fasting glucose, and development of cognitive impairment in older women. Neurology. 2004;63:658–663. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000134666.64593.ba. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES