Skip to main content
Biology Letters logoLink to Biology Letters
. 2007 Nov 27;4(1):119–122. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0513

Aggressive and non-aggressive personalities differ in oxidative status in selected lines of mice (Mus musculus)

David Costantini 1,2,*, Claudio Carere 3, Doretta Caramaschi 4, Jaap M Koolhaas 4
PMCID: PMC2412938  PMID: 18042511

Abstract

Mice selected for aggression and coping (long attack latency (LAL), reactive coping strategy; short attack latency (SAL), pro-active coping strategy) are a useful model for studying the physiological background of animal personalities. These mice also show a differential stress responsiveness, especially in terms of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis reactivity, to various challenges. Since the stress response can increase the production of reactive oxygen species, we predicted that the basic oxidative status of the lines could differ. We found that LAL showed higher serum antioxidant capacity (OXY) than SAL, while no differences emerged for reactive oxygen metabolites (ROMs) or the balance between ROMs and OXY, reflecting oxidative stress. Moreover, the lines showed inverse relationships between ROMs or OXY and body mass corrected for age. The results indicate that variation in oxidative status is heritable and linked to personality. This suggests that different animal personalities may be accompanied by differences in oxidative status, which may predict differences in longevity.

Keywords: oxidative stress, free radicals, antioxidants, personality, glucocorticoids, aggression

1. Introduction

Metabolism produces pro-oxidant compounds that damage biomolecules. To cope with pro-oxidants, the body uses antioxidants and mechanisms that are able to repair or remove damaged molecules. When the redox status, i.e. the balance between pro-oxidants and antioxidants, is shifted towards more oxidative conditions, oxidative stress (OS) arises (Finkel & Holbrook 2000). The accumulation of degenerative changes caused by OS to biomolecules may lead to pathologies, cell senescence and cell death (Beckman & Ames 1998; Finkel & Holbrook 2000).

One of the mechanisms by which organisms cope with stressful challenges is the secretion of glucocorticoid steroid hormones. Several studies show that stress response mediated by glucocorticoids may increase OS in birds or mammals (e.g. Lin et al. 2004; Sahin & Gümüslu 2007). Individuals show consistent and non-random differences in how they cope behaviourally and physiologically with challenges, referred to as behavioural syndromes, coping styles or personalities (Koolhaas et al. 1999; Sih et al. 2004; Groothuis & Carere 2005; Bell 2007; Wolf et al. 2007). One established model is provided by mouse lines selected for short and long attack latencies (SAL and LAL; Van Oortmerssen & Bakker 1981; Benus et al. 1991; Koolhaas et al. 1999; Veenema et al. 2003b). SAL mice show shorter attack latencies and higher attack counts than LAL mice. Furthermore, the lines display different coping strategies with non-social environmental challenges (Benus et al. 1991; Koolhaas et al. 1999). More importantly for the present work, the glucocorticoid response to ACTH, novelty and forced swim is significantly higher in LAL than in SAL mice (Veenema et al. 2004, 2005b). Chronic psychosocial stress induces a long-lasting increase in glucocorticoid production in LAL mice, but not in SAL mice (Veenema et al. 2003a, 2005a). These data show that LAL mice have a higher stress responsiveness in terms of glucocorticoid production than SAL mice (Veenema et al. 2004).

In line with the characterization of personality in other taxa (e.g. birds, Groothuis & Carere 2005), LAL can be described as reactive and SAL as pro-active (Koolhaas et al. 1999). So far, there is scant information on the relationship between personality and oxidative status. Evidence from humans shows that the activity of the glutathione peroxidase 1 gene is lower in shy than in bold individuals (Matsuzawa et al. 2005). There are no studies on other animals explicitly relating personality traits to oxidative status, but a few studies suggest a link. One longitudinal study found that neophobic, shy rats had increased basal corticosterone levels throughout life and a 60% higher chance of death compared with neophilic, bold individuals (Cavigelli & McClintock 2003). These rats, although not tested for aggression, may be comparable to the SAL–LAL mice, at least in terms of glucocorticoid production and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis reactivity. One potential explanation is that the higher glucocorticoid responsiveness of shy individuals could be associated with or induce OS (Behl et al. 1997).

On the hypothesis that differential stress responsiveness is related to different oxidative profiles, we compared the serum of adolescent male SAL (generation 78) and LAL (generation 51) mice for two physiological measures: (i) the level of reactive oxygen metabolites (ROMs), which is a marker of oxidative damage and (ii) the serum antioxidant capacity (OXY; e.g. Ballerini et al. 2003; Costantini & Dell'Omo 2006a,b; Costantini et al. 2006, 2007). In this study, the mice of the two lines were not exposed to any stressor to amplify their behavioural and neuroendocrine differences; however, we expected to find different basal oxidative profiles, given the evident behavioural and physiological differences that the lines display also under non-stressful conditions.

2. Material and methods

Blood was collected from 38 to 64 days old male mice (Mus musculus domesticus; n=14 for each line).

ROMs and OXY were measured by the d-ROMs test and the OXY-Adsorbent test, respectively, according to previous studies (Costantini & Dell'Omo 2006a,b; Costantini et al. 2006).

Generalized linear models (GLMs) were performed using the Statistica package (v. 7.0, StatSoft, Inc., 2004, Tulsa, OK, USA).

Details of the methods are reported in the electronic supplementary material.

3. Results

The lines did not differ in the levels of ROMs (table 1; figure 1). However, they showed inverse relationships between ROMs and body mass corrected for age (significant interaction term; table 1; figure 2).

Table 1.

Outcomes of GLMs. (Significant p-values are shown in italics.)

variable source of variation F d.f. p-value
ROMs line 1.30 1,24 0.264
body mass corrected for age 2.84 1,24 0.105
line×body mass corrected for age 5.50 1,24 0.028
OXY line 9.15 1,24 0.006
body mass corrected for age 1.97 1,24 0.173
line×body mass corrected for age 4.34 1,24 0.048
oxidative stress: ROMs/OXY×1000 line 2.98 1,24 0.097
body mass corrected for age 3.49 1,24 0.074
line×body mass corrected for age 1.07 1,24 0.312
oxidative stress: ROMs with OXY as a covariate line 0.10 1,21 0.755
OXY 0.00 1,21 0.948
body mass corrected for age 0.00 1,21 0.983
line×OXY 0.18 1,21 0.677
line×body mass corrected for age 3.51 1,21 0.075
OXY×body mass corrected for age 0.03 1,21 0.867

Figure 1.

Figure 1

SAL mice show lower OXY than LAL mice. No differences emerged for ROMs or OS. Means+s.e.m. are shown. (a) p=0.26, (b) p=0.006 and (c) p=0.10.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

SAL (open circles, dashed line) and LAL (filled circles, solid line) mice show inverse relationships between body mass corrected for age and ROMs or OXY. Trend lines are shown when significant (SAL: (a) ROMs, r=−0.65, p=0.011; (c) OS, r=−0.56, p=0.037; LAL: (b) OXY, r=0.69, p=0.006). Body mass corrected for age is shown as residuals from a linear regression of body mass on age.

The OXY was higher in LAL than in SAL mice (table 1; figure 1) and the lines showed inverse relationships between OXY and body mass corrected for age (significant interaction term; table 1; figure 2).

The ratio of OS did not differ between the lines (table 1; figure 1), nor did the relationship between this ratio and age-corrected body mass (no significant interaction term; table 1; figure 2). The difference in OS between the lines approached significance (p=0.052) after dropping the interaction term from the model. Similar results were found using ROMs as the dependent variable and OXY as a covariate (table 1). The line effect approached significance (p=0.069) after dropping all the interaction terms from the model.

SAL and LAL mice did not differ in age (t=−1.21, p=0.23; Levene's test, p<0.001; mean±s.e.: SAL, 49.71±2.40 days; LAL, 52.71±0.61 days). A significant difference was found for body mass (t=2.32, p=0.03; Levene's test, p<0.001; mean±s.e.: SAL, 18.59±0.82 g; LAL, 16.54±0.32 g) and body mass corrected for age (t=4.00, p<0.001; Levene's test, p=0.19; mean±s.e.: SAL, +1.32±0.55; LAL, −1.32±0.37).

4. Discussion

We found that non-aggressive mice (LAL) showed higher OXY than aggressive mice (SAL). Moreover, the lines showed different relationships between ROMs or OXY and body mass corrected for age. This is the first evidence in non-human animals that oxidative status differs between personalities with a known genetic background.

Our findings indicate that oxidative status has a genetic basis and is linked with personality and stress coping, probably having been co-selected with the behavioural phenotype. However, the effect could be indirect, as different behavioural styles could themselves induce different oxidative status profiles. This latter hypothesis is unlikely, since the subjects were adolescent and experimentally naive concerning social challenges. A genetic basis for oxidative status is known for many species (Martin et al. 1996), thus our finding is not surprising. However, a link with personality has been shown for humans only (Matsuzawa et al. 2005).

Less aggressive male mice have shorter lifespans, and it has been suggested that the age-related decline in the concentrations of catecholamines and testosterone might be responsible for their reduced longevity (Ewalds-Kwist & Selander 1996). Given the present results and the fact that OS may modulate ageing (Harman 1956; Beckman & Ames 1998), the redox status could explain the differences in lifespan that different personalities experience (Cavigelli & McClintock 2003). In our study, the less aggressive LAL showed higher OXY. This suggests that LAL may overexpress OXY to cope with higher free-radical production or to prepare the organism for the high stress levels they will experience throughout adulthood. Whether glucocorticoid production, which is higher in shy, neophobic and less aggressive individuals, mediates such differences needs to be tested experimentally, but the evidence from this and other studies and species supports this view (e.g. Lin et al. 2004; Sahin & Gümüslu 2007).

The patterns of covariance between both markers of OS and body mass corrected for age showed that in SAL mice, ROMs decreased with age-corrected body mass, while in LAL mice OXY increased with it. In contrast, the relationship between age-corrected body mass and the ratio between ROMs and OXY did not differ. These results suggest that the lines have different maturation times for the mechanisms that regulate redox status, or that they undergo different metabolic costs of body mass.

In conclusion, our results shed light on the personality/oxidative status nexus and call for further investigations to evaluate how the seemingly different redox statuses characterizing different personalities respond to stress challenges. A potential problem is that the selection lines are not replicated and this implies that this study does not prove causality (see Falconer & Mackay 1996, p. 318). However, studies comparing all three genetic selection lines for high and low aggression available in the world show striking similarities in behaviour, neuroendocrinology and neurochemistry (e.g. Caramaschi et al. 2007). Moreover, these similarities also hold for the extremes in aggressive behaviour in an unselected strain of feral rats (Koolhaas et al. 2007).

Future work on the two lines should compare oxidative status between basal and acute stress conditions, as well as examine its relationship to ageing. Studies should also consider the levels of oxidative damage and antioxidant capacity in other tissues to build a more comprehensive view of the individual OS of SAL–LAL lines.

Acknowledgments

Animal care and sampling procedures were performed according to the Animal Experiment Committee (DEC) of the University of Groningen.

We thank the editor and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions and constructive criticisms, Gianfranco Brambilla and Edoardo Vignolo for their technical and logistical support at the Istituto Superiore di Sanità, Rome and Auke Meinema for the animal breeding and caretaking. Tim Fawcett kindly revised the English of the final version. David Costantini was supported by a fellowship at the University of Zurich issued from the University La Sapienza.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary methods

Animals and blood collection; biochemical analyses; statistical analyses

rsbl20070513s25.doc (28.5KB, doc)

References

  1. Ballerini A, Civitareale C, Fiori M, Regini M, Betti M, Brambilla G. Traceability of inbred and crossbred cinta senese pigs by evaluating the oxidative stress. J. Vet. Med. A. 2003;50:113–116. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0442.2003.00508.x. doi:10.1046/j.1439-0442.2003.00508.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Beckman K.B, Ames B.N. The free radical theory of aging matures. Physiol. Rev. 1998;78:547–581. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1998.78.2.547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Behl C, Lezoualc'h F, Trapp T, Widmann M, Scutella T, Holsboer F. Glucocorticoids enhance oxidative stress-induced cell death in hippocampal neurons in vitro. Endocrinology. 1997;138:101–106. doi: 10.1210/endo.138.1.4835. doi:10.1210/en.138.1.101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bell A.M. Future directions in behavioural syndrome research. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2007;274:755–761. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.0199. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Benus R.F, Bohus B, Koolhaas J.M, Van Oortmerssen G.A. Heritable variation for aggression as a reflection of individual coping strategies. Experientia. 1991;47:1008–1019. doi: 10.1007/BF01923336. doi:10.1007/BF01923336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Caramaschi D, de Boer S.F, Koolhaas J.M. Differential role of the 5-HT1A receptor in aggressive and non-aggressive mice: an across-strain comparison. Physiol. Behav. 2007;90:590–601. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.11.010. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.11.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cavigelli S.A, McClintock M.K. Fear of novelty in infant rats predicts adult corticosterone dynamics and an early death. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2003;100:16 131–16 136. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2535721100. doi:10.1073/pnas.2535721100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Costantini D, Dell'Omo G. Effects of T-cell-mediated immune response on avian oxidative stress. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A. 2006a;145:137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpa.2006.06.002. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2006.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Costantini D, Dell'Omo G. Environmental and genetic components of oxidative stress in wild kestrel nestlings (Falco tinnunculus) J. Comp. Physiol. B. 2006b;176:575–579. doi: 10.1007/s00360-006-0080-0. doi:10.1007/s00360-006-0080-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Costantini D, Casagrande S, De Filippis S, Brambilla G, Fanfani A, Tagliavini J, Dell'Omo G. Correlates of oxidative stress in wild kestrel nestlings (Falco tinnunculus) J. Comp. Physiol. B. 2006;176:329–337. doi: 10.1007/s00360-005-0055-6. doi:10.1007/s00360-005-0055-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Costantini D, Cardinale M, Carere C. Oxidative damage and anti-oxidant capacity in two migratory bird species at a stop-over site. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. C. 2007;144:363–371. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpc.2006.11.005. doi:10.1016/j.cbpc.2006.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ewalds-Kwist S.B.N, Selander R.-K. Lifespans in mice from strains selected for high or low aggression. Aggress. Behav. 1996;22:457–464. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:6<457::AID-AB6>3.0.CO;2-E [Google Scholar]
  13. Falconer D.S, Mackay T.F.C. 4th edn. Longman; London, UK: 1996. Introduction to quantitative genetics. [Google Scholar]
  14. Finkel T, Holbrook N.J. Oxidants, oxidative stress and the biology of ageing. Nature. 2000;408:239–247. doi: 10.1038/35041687. doi:10.1038/35041687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Groothuis T.G.G, Carere C. Avian personalities: characterization and epigenesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005;29:137–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.010. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Harman D. Aging: a theory based on free radical and radiation chemistry. J. Gerontol. 1956;11:298–300. doi: 10.1093/geronj/11.3.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Koolhaas J.M, Korte S.M, de Boer S.F, van der Vegt B.J, van Reenen C.G, Hopster H, de Jong I.C, Ruis M.A, Blokhuis H.J. Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1999;23:925–935. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00026-3. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00026-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Koolhaas J.M, de Boer S.F, Buwalda B, van Reenen C.G. Individual variation in coping with stress: a multidimensional approach of ultimate and proximate mechanisms. Brain Behav. Evol. 2007;70:218–226. doi: 10.1159/000105485. doi:10.1159/000105485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lin H, Decuypere E, Buyse J. Oxidative stress induced by corticosterone administration in broiler chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) 1. Chronic exposure. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. B. 2004;139:737–744. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpc.2004.09.013. doi:10.1016/j.cbpc.2004.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Martin G.M, Austad S.N, Johnson T.E. Genetic analysis of ageing: role of oxidative damage and environmental stresses. Nat. Genet. 1996;13:25–34. doi: 10.1038/ng0596-25. doi:10.1038/ng0596-25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Matsuzawa D, Hashimoto K, Shimizu E, Fujisaki M, Iyo M. Functional polymorphism of the glutathione peroxidase 1 gene is associated with personality traits in healthy subjects. Neuropsychobiology. 2005;52:68–70. doi: 10.1159/000086607. doi:10.1159/000086607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Sahin E, Gümüslu S. Stress-dependent induction of protein oxidation, lipid peroxidation and anti-oxidants in peripheral tissues of rats: comparison of three stress models (immobilization, cold and immobilization-cold) Clin. Exp. Pharm. Physiol. 2007;34:425–431. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1681.2007.04584.x. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1681.2007.04584.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Sih A, Bell A.M, Johnson J.C, Ziemba R.E. Behavioral syndromes: an integrative overview. Q. Rev. Biol. 2004;79:341–377. doi: 10.1086/422893. doi:10.1086/422893 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Van Oortmerssen G.A, Bakker T.C. Artificial selection for short and long attack latencies in wild Mus musculus domesticus. Behav. Genet. 1981;11:115–126. doi: 10.1007/BF01065622. doi:10.1007/BF01065622 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Veenema A.H, Meijer O.C, de Kloet E.R, Koolhaas J.M. Genetic selection for coping style predicts stressor susceptibility. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2003a;15:256–267. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.00986.x. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.00986.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Veenema A.H, Meijer O.C, de Kloet E.R, Koolhaas J.M, Bohus B.G. Differences in basal and stress-induced HPA regulation of wild house mice selected for high and low aggression. Horm. Behav. 2003b;43:197–204. doi: 10.1016/s0018-506x(02)00013-2. doi:10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00013-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Veenema A.H, Koolhaas J.M, de Kloet E.R. Basal and stress-induced differences in HPA-axis, 5-HT responsiveness, and hippocampal cell proliferation in two mouse lines. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2004;1018:255–265. doi: 10.1196/annals.1296.030. doi:10.1196/annals.1296.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Veenema A.H, Sijtsma B, Koolhaas J.M, de Kloet E.R. The stress response to sensory contact in mice: genotype effect of the stimulus animal. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005a;30:550–557. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.01.003. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Veenema A.H, Cremers T.I, Jongsma M.E, Steenbergen P.J, de Boer S.F, Koolhaas J.M. Differences in the effects of 5-HT (1A) receptor agonists on forced swimming behavior and brain 5-HT metabolism between low and high aggressive mice. Psychopharmacology. 2005b;178:151–160. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2005-5. doi:10.1007/s00213-004-2005-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Wolf M, van Doorn G.S, Leimar O, Weissing F.J. Life-history trade-offs favour the evolution of animal personalities. Nature. 2007;447:581–585. doi: 10.1038/nature05835. doi:10.1038/nature05835 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary methods

Animals and blood collection; biochemical analyses; statistical analyses

rsbl20070513s25.doc (28.5KB, doc)

Articles from Biology Letters are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES