Abstract
Muscimol is a GABAA-agonist that causes rapid and reversible suppression of neurophysiological activity. Interpretations of the effects of muscimol infusions into the brain have been limited because of uncertainty about spread of the drug around the injection site. To solve this problem, the present study explored the use of a fluorophore-conjugated muscimol molecule (FCM). Whole-cell recordings from horizontal brain slices demonstrated that bath-applied FCM acts like muscimol in reversibly suppressing excitatory synaptic transmission. Two types of in vivo experiments demonstrated that the behavioral effects of FCM infusion are similar to the behavioral effects of muscimol infusion. FCM infusion into the rat amygdala before fear conditioning impaired both cued and contextual freezing, which were tested 24 or 48 hr later. Normal fear conditioning occurred when these same rats were subsequently given phosphate buffered saline infusions. FCM infusion into the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex impaired accuracy during a delayed-response task. Histological analysis showed that the region of fluorescence was restricted to 0.5 to 1 mm from the injection site. Myelinated fiber tracts acted as diffusional barriers, thereby shaping the overall spread of fluorescence. The results suggest that FCM is indeed useful for exploring the function of small brain regions.
Keywords: GABAA, Agonist, lesions, fear conditioning, amygdala, prefrontal cortex
1.0 Introduction
Reversible inactivations of small volumes of brain tissue have been used to explore fundamental questions in behavioral and systems neuroscience (Majchrzak and Di Scala, 2000). Reversible inactivations are an important alternative to permanent brain lesions (Jarrard, 2002) in assessing structure-function relationships. The obvious advantage is that reversible brain inactivations allow within-subjects designs. One common disadvantage is that this method typically leaves considerable uncertainty about the size and shape of the volume of the drug-infused tissue.
Muscimol, a GABAA-agonist (Beaumont et al., 1978), is commonly and productively used for reversible inactivations (Lomber, 1999; Martin and Ghez, 1999; Krupa and Thompson, 1997). However, the spatial extent of a muscimol infusion is indeterminate without the use a radioactive tracer ([3H]muscimol; Martin, 1991; Edeline et al., 2002). Indirect estimates about the spread of muscimol have been obtained by measuring its anticipated functional consequences on glucose uptake (Martin, 1991), evoked field potentials (Wilensky et al., 2006), and multiunit activity levels (Arikan et al., 2002; Edeline et al., 2002). Practical considerations have precluded the routine use of either radioactive or functional techniques (but see Krupa and Thompson, 1997).
Here we evaluated the use of a fluorophore-conjugated muscimol molecule (FCM) for producing local and reversible brain inactivations. FCM was first used in conjunction with muscimol to assess the role of dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) in a delayed-response task (Narayanan and Laubach, 2006; Narayanan et al., 2006). Here, the evaluation of FCM made use of whole-cell recordings, well-characterized brain-behavior relationships, and optical quantification methods. The results show that FCM can serve as a stand-alone and practical substitute for muscimol.
2.0 Materials and Methods
2.1 Subjects
This study used a total of 15 rats, described for each experiment below. Procedures were approved by the Yale University Animal Care and Use Committees (whole-cell, acute infusion, and fear conditioning) and the John B. Pierce Laboratory (delayed-response task).
2.2 Drugs
Muscimol was acquired from Sigma-Aldrich. FCM (commercially designated as Muscimol-TMR-X) was acquired from Molecular Probes (product #M23400, www.molecularprobes.com). FCM is a conjugate of muscimol and the Bodipy® TMR-X fluorophore (covalent amide bonding). FCM is stable and will not dissociate into its constituent parts in solution. The Bodipy® TMR-X portion of FCM is highly lipophilic (personal communication, Molecular Probes, Technical Support, Carlsbad, CA). FCM has excitation and emission peaks, respectively, at 543 nm and 572 nm. Figure 1 shows the structures, formulas, and molecular weights of FCM and muscimol.
For both acute infusions and fear conditioning experiments, 1 mg of FCM was dissolved into 2 mL of 0.01 M phosphate buffered 0.9% saline (PBS), resulting in a 0.8 mM concentration. For the brain-slice experiments, this stock solution was then diluted in artificial cerebral spinal fluid (aCFS) to a final bath concentration of 10 μM. For the delayed-response experiments, 1 mg FCM was dissolved in 1 mL of 0.9% saline, resulting in a 1.6 mM concentration. FCM concentrations higher than 1 mg/ml are possible, using dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO) as a solvent, but were not explored here.
2.3 Whole-Cell Recordings
Whole-cell recordings were performed in horizontal brain slices that included the medial temporal lobe. The methods have been described in extensive detail elsewhere (Faulkner and Brown, 1999; Moyer and Brown, 1998; 2002; 2007). Briefly, male Sprague-Dawley rats (n = 3; 22 – 32 days) were deeply anesthetized with halothane and decapitated. The brain was quickly removed and placed in ice-cold oxygenated (95% O2/5%CO2) sucrose-aCSF containing (in mM): 206 sucrose, 2.8 KCl, 1 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 2 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3 and 10 D-glucose for about 3 min. Horizontal slices (300 – 400 μm) containing perirhinal cortex (PR; approx. −7.8 D/V) were cut at ~1° C using a temperature-controlled vibrotome (Vibrotome 3000, Vibrotome Company Inc., MO).
Slices were moved to an incubation chamber at room temperature (22 – 26° C) for at least an hour. The oxygenated incubation aCSF contained (in mM): 124 NaCl, 2.8 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 2 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3 and 10 D-glucose, pH 7.4, 295 mOsmol. Individual slices were transferred to a submerged-type recording chamber and perfused with oxygenated aCSF. Recordings were done at room temperature. PR was visualized using an upright microscope (Zeiss Axioskop) equipped with infrared-filtered light and differential interference contrast optics. A bipolar stimulating electrode was positioned into layer I of PR. The whole-cell recording pipette (3 – 5 M Ω) was directed to layer V of PR. The pipette solution contained (in mM): 110 K-gluconate, 10 HEPES, 1.0 EGTA, 20 KCL, 2.0 MgCl2, 2.0 Na2ATP, 0.25 Na3GTP and 10 Dphosphocreatine (d-Tris), pH 7.4, 290 mOsmol (Moyer and Brown, 2007).
2.4 Surgery
Rats (described for specific experiments below) were anesthetized with injections (i.p.) of either a cocktail of ketamine (100 mg/kg) and diazepam (10 mg/kg), or a cocktail of ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg). Anesthesia was maintained at surgical levels throughout surgeries with supplements of ketamine (30 mg/kg), approximately every hour. Under aseptic conditions, the scalp was retracted, and the skull was leveled between bregma and lambda. Craniotomies were made above the target cannula sites. A microsyringe or guide cannula (26-guage; Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) was lowered into the region of interest: basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA; coordinates from bregma: −3.0 A/P, ± 5.0 M/L, −7.5, D/V); PR (coordinates from bregma: −4.5 A/P, − 6.8 M/L −7 D/V at 45° from the medial plane); or dmPFC (coordinates from bregma: −3.2 A/P, ±1.4 M/L, −3.7 D/V at 12° in the frontal plane). After guide cannula were positioned, craniotomies were sealed with cyanoacrylate (SloZap, Pacer Technologies, Rancho Cucamonga, CA) accelerated by ZipKicker (Pacer Technologies), and/or methyl methacrylate (i.e., dental cement; AM Systems, Port Angeles, WA). Rats were given buprenorphine subcutaneously (0.05 mg/kg) every 12 hours after surgery for analgesia for 36–48 hours and allowed to recover in their home cages.
2.5 Microsyringe infusions
Four male rats (Sprague-Dawley) weighing approximately 300 – 400g were used in the acute infusion experiments. A micromanipulator (Sutter Instrument Company, Novato, CA) was used to position the tip of a hypodermic microsyringe (26 gauge, non-coring, Hamilton Co., Reno, NV) into BLA, PR or dmPFC. Rats received one BLA and one PR injection, each in a different hemisphere. One of the four rats received an additional dmPFC infusion in the same hemisphere as the PR infusion. A motorized infusing apparatus (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL), attached to the micromanipulator, delivered fluid at a rate of 0.05 μL/min. The injected volumes were 0.5 μL for BLA and dmPFC, and 1 μL for PR. The holes in the skull were covered with a thin layer of bone wax and the overlying skin was sutured. Rats were monitored and given analgesics as stated above. Subjects were allowed to recover for 24 hr and perfused.
2.6 Cannula infusions
Chronic Infusions of FCM were made with a 33-gauge injector cannula (Plastics One). The injector cannula extended 1 mm from the tip of the cannula guide when the BLA was targeted and extended 0.2 mm from the cannula tip when the dmPFC was targeted. Rats were restrained by hand or lightly anesthetized using halothane. Injector cannula were inserted into the guide cannula. The infusion fluid (0.5 μl) was delivered into each site at a rate of 15 μpl/hr (0.25 μL/min; Martin and Ghez 1999) via a syringe infusion pump (PHD 2000 Infusion, Harvard Apparatus, Holliston, MA). The infusion fluid was PBS, saline (0.9%; Phoenix Scientific, St. Joseph, MO), muscimol (1 μg/μl), or FCM (0.5 –1 μg/μl), specified for each experiment below. The fluid was infused via polyethylene tubing (0.38 mm diameter; Intramedic, New York, NY) that was attached to the infusion cannula on one end and to a 10 μl Hamilton syringe (Hamilton) on the other end. Injections were confirmed by monitoring movement of fluid in the tubing via a small bubble. After infusions were complete, the infusion cannula was left in place for 2 –3 min. Behavioral studies began 30 –45 minutes after infusion. Previous physiological studies have suggested that the effects of muscimol begin almost immediately and are stable for many hours (Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1985; Krupa et al., 2004).
2.7 Histology
Histological analysis identified the locations of the cannula tips and quantified the spread of FCM. Subjects were given an overdose of sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, i.p.) and perfused with 50 mL of 0.01 M phosphate buffered, 0.9% saline followed by 100 mL of 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains were removed and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose. Coronal sections (50 μm thickness) were acquired with a freezing microtome (AO860; Rankin Biomedical). FCM was visualized using a Zeiss M2 Bio Fluorescence Microscope (Carl Zeiss, Thornwood, NY) that was equipped with a high-intensity lamp (Zeiss) and FITC and Texas Red filter sets (Zeiss). Images were acquired with a computer-assisted CCD camera (AxioCam HRc; Zeiss) imaging system (AxioVision CD 28; Zeiss). Exposure times were adjusted to maximize image quality between 2 and 8 seconds and did not affect the visualized area of fluorescence. Dark-field images of PBS-soaked slices allowed the visualization of fibers for gross histological localizations, in acute infusions and fear conditioning experiments. Alternatively, in the delayed-response task, slices were counterstained with a fluorescent-green Nissl (Neurotrace®; Molecular Probes). Images were reassembled (by direct insertion without manipulation into red and green color channels) using Adobe Photoshop (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA). Measurements of drug spread were estimated from equally-spaced 50 μm sections. Red channels were loaded in MATLAB and image averages were computed based on custom scripts (delayed-response task).
2.8 Fear Conditioning
Four male Sprague-Dawley rats weighing approximately 300 –400g were used in the fear conditioning experiment. Rats were singly housed with ad libitum access to food and water. Rats were handled prior to and after surgical implantation of the guide cannula. The delay fear conditioning procedures used here were similar to those described in detail previously (Lindquist et al., 2004). Briefly, the conditional stimulus (CS) was a 22 kHz ultrasonic vocalization (USV; 7.712 s duration) pre-recorded from a conspecific (Anderson, 1954; Brudzynski, 2007; Allen et al., 2007; Furtak et al., 2007). The unconditional stimulus (US) was a 0.4 s, 0.8 mA foot shock delivered through a standard grid floor (Coulbourn Instruments). Rats were placed in the conditioning chamber and given 2 min to habituate to the experimental context. Next they were given 5 conditioning trials (ITI = 180 ± 30 s) in which the CS always coterminated the US.
Freezing served as the conditional response (CR; Blanchard and Blanchard, 1969). Behavior was recorded for offline analysis with a miniature IR-CCD camera (Circuit Specialists, Mesa, AZ) digitized and encoded in MPEG-2 format (WinTV-PVR, Hauppauge, NY). Video files were stored and viewed on a personal computer. Twenty-four and 48 hours after conditioning subjects were tested for CS-elicited freezing in a shifted context or context-elicited freezing in the original context. The order of testing sessions was counterbalanced among animals. Freezing was scored manually by a research assistant blind to all experimental conditions.
The fear conditioning procedures were performed once with pre-training FCM infusions (0.5 μL) into BLA and then were repeated 72 hr later with PBS infusions (vehicle) into BLA (always in that order). See Table I for the behavioral and infusions schedule used in the fear conditioning experiments. A day after completion of all behavioral experiments, rats were given second infusion of FCM and perfused 40 –45 min later.
Table I.
DAY | PROCEDURE |
---|---|
1 | Surgery: Bilateral BLA cannula implantation |
2 – 5 | Recovery |
6 – 10 | Handling |
11 | Pre-training FCM infusions and 1st fear conditioning session |
12 | Testing Day 1 (Cue or Context) |
13 | Testing Day 2 (Cue or Context) |
14 | Pre-training PBS infusions and 2nd fear conditioning session |
15 | Testing Day 3 (Cue or Context) |
16 | Testing Day 4 (Cue or Context) |
17 – 20 | FCM Infusion, Perfusion, Histology and Imaging |
2.9 Delayed-Response Task
Four male Brown-Norway rats (5 –10 months old) were trained to perform a delayed-response task. Rats were motivated by water restriction, while food was available ad libitum. Rats received 10 –15 ml of water during each behavioral session as reward. Additional water (5–10 ml) was provided 1 –3 hours after each behavioral session in the home cage. Rats were maintained at ~90% of their free-access body weights during the course of these experiments and received one day of free access to water per week.
The apparatus and methods for the delayed-response task have been described extensively elsewhere (Naranayan et al., 2006; Naranayan and Laubach, 2006). Briefly, when the chamber light turned, a trial of the delayed-response task was initiated by the rat depressing a lever and ended when the lever was released. The delay period was 1.2 s (Figure 5A). A trigger stimulus was given on 50% of trials (8 kHz tone). Correct responses occurred when the rats successfully maintained a lever press for the full foreperiod duration and released the lever within a pre-defined response latency (within 600 ms after the end of the foreperiod). Each correct response in the task activated the water pump at 0.03 ml/s for 1–2 s, depending on the stage of training. Premature responses occurred when rats did not maintain a lever press for the full foreperiod, and late responses occurred when the lever was released after the response window. These latter two types of trials were followed by a timeout period, in which all experimental stimuli were extinguished for 4–8 s. Behavior was monitored throughout via a closed-circuit video camera. See Table II for the daily procedures in the delayed-response task infusion experiments.
Table II.
DAY | PROCEDURE |
---|---|
1–3 | Handling |
4 | Autoshaping |
~5 – 7 | Lever-press training |
~7–14 | Hold and release training in delayed-response task |
15 | Surgery: Bilateral dmPFC cannula implantation |
16–22 | Recovery |
~22 | Post-surgical reintroduction to delayed-response task |
23 | Pre-testing saline infusion in delayed-response performance |
24 | Pre-testing muscimol infusion in delayed-response performance |
25 | No infusion (recovery) in delayed-response performance |
26 | Pre-testing FCM infusion in delayed-response performance and perfusion |
27–28 | Histology and imaging |
Forty-five minutes prior to the last testing session, FCM (0.5 μl at a concentration 1 μg/μl) was bilaterally infused into dmPFC. Rats performed the reaction time task for 30 minutes, after which they were deeply anesthetized and transcardially perfused.
3.0 Results
3.1 Effects of FCM on Excitatory Postsynaptic Potentials and Input Resistance
Horizontal rat brain slices were used to verify that FCM, like unlabeled muscimol, does in fact suppress neuronal and synaptic function. Whole-cell recordings were made from neurons in layer V of PR. Synaptic responses were evoked in these neurons by stimulating layer I of PR. The complete experimental protocol was conducted on eleven healthy cells recorded from 3 rat pups. In each case, bath application FCM (10 μM in aCSF) caused an immediate and rapid suppression of excitatory synaptic transmission and a parallel reduction in the input resistance(RN) of the postsynaptic neuron (Figure 2). In each of the eleven cells, the maximum effect occurred 15 –25 min after the start of the infusion (the 1st and 2nd time points that were measured). Excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) amplitudes were reduced by 45 – 64% of their baseline levels. Recovery of RN and EPSP amplitudes required 2 – 3 hours. Thus the results show that FCM, like muscimol, reversibly depresses neural function.
3.2 Asymmetrical spread of FCM
The spread of FCM was analyzed in four rats that received acute infusions of FCM into BLA, PR and/or dmPFC. Infusions were performed using a microsyringe. Rats recovered for 24 hr after the surgery, were deeply anesthetized and then given transcardiac perfusions. Brains were removed and incubated in 4% paraformaldehyde for 24 hr. The brains were cryoprotected in 30% sucrose for 72 hr. Slicing and imaging occurred immediately after the incubation periods.
FCM was visualized in coronal brain slices (50 μm thickness; Figure 3). Notably, infusions into different brain regions resulted in different diffusion gradients. Figure 3 highlights one such infusion in PR in which the spread of the infusion was impeded medial to the infusion site. This caused asymmetrical fluorescence in the medial-lateral axis. The deep cerebral white matter (dcw) appears to act as a diffusion barrier (Figure 3C). Restricted diffusion through white matter may be related to the lipophilic properties of the fluorophore portion of FCM. An asymmetrical spread was also observable in BLA and dmPFC infusions. Thus the shape of the affected tissue depends on the injection site.
3.3 Infusion of FCM into BLA impairs Fear Conditioning
The functional effect of FCM infusion was tested using a well-characterized brain-behavior relationship. Fear conditioning gives robust results that are known to depend upon normal functioning in the BLA (for review LeDoux; 2000; Maren, 2001). Histological analysis showed FCM to be well-localized within BLA in 3 of 4 rats (Figure 4C). One rat showed no evidence of fluorescence in brain slices and was thus excluded from the behavioral analysis. This animal showed robust cue and context conditioning. In the remaining rats, pre-training infusions of FCM resulted in little freezing to the 22 kHz USV (7%) or the conditioning context (9%). The FCM effect size (d; Cohen, 1988) on conditioning was very large for both cue conditioning (d = 4.7) and context conditioning (d = 3.8).
The inhibition of BLA was reversible. Re-conditioning 3 days later, with pre-training PBS (vehicle) infusions, resulted in robust levels of freezing to both the 22 kHz USV (60%) and the conditioning context (91%). A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a main effect of Infusion Type (PBS versus FCM), F(1,2) = 36.288, p < 0.02. The mean levels of freezing to the cue and context were not significantly different, F(1,2) = 0.966, p = 0.43. There was no interaction between the Infusion and Stimulus Type, F(1,2) = 1.230, p = 0.38. These results show that pre-training infusions of FCM into BLA reversibly block normal fear conditioning to both a cue and a context (Figure 4B), as is true for pre-training muscimol infusions into BLA (Helmstetter and Bellgowan, 1994; Muller et al., 1997; Wilensky et al., 2002; Wilensky et al., 2006).
3.4 Infusions of FCM into dmPFC in a Delayed-Response Task
We also assessed the behavioral effect of FCM infusions into dmPFC. This assessment used an instrumental task on which performance is known to be impaired by muscimol infusion into dmPFC (Narayanan et al., 2006; Narayanan and Laubach, 2006). Rats were trained to asymptotic performance over days. After asymptotic performance was achieved, saline infusions were made bilaterally into dmPFC. Following saline infusions, rats made correct responses on 53 ± 3% (mean ± SD) of trials. Twenty-four hours later, muscimol was infused into dmPFC. Following muscimol infusions, rats made correct responses on 18 ± 12% of trials. The performance decrease was significant (paired t(3) = 6.78, p < 0.01). A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that rats made more premature responses in muscimol sessions relative to saline sessions (F(1,1143) = 110.43, p <0.001). After recovery, FCM was infused into dmPFC. Rats made correct responses on 29 ±11%, significantly less than in saline sessions (paired t(3) = 4.76, p < 0.02; Figure 5B). Repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that rats made significantly more premature response following FCM infusions than following saline sessions (F(1,1190)= 21.88, p < 0.001).
Successful performance was not significantly different between muscimol infusion sessions and FCM-infusion sessions (paired t(3) = 2.72, p > 0.05). A repeated-measures ANOVAs revealed that rats made significantly more premature responses following muscimol infusions than after FCM infusions (F(1,780)= 26.74, p < 0.001). This effect might reflect the fact that the drug concentration was higher for muscimol (8.8 mM) than for FCM (1.6 mM). These sizable concentration differences could affect the amount of inhibition at the infusion site as well as the spatial gradient of the inhibition. Monitoring of behavior prior to and during delayed-response performance revealed that dmPFC inactivations with FCM caused no gross changes in alertness, locomotion, stereotyped behavior, paw movements or activity levels.
Histology showed that all cannula placements were located in dmPFC. FCM was well-restricted to dmPFC, with the exception of a small amount of fluorescence that was sometimes evident around the guide cannula, near anterior cingulate cortex (Figure 5C). Further histological analysis quantified the average spread of FCM. An average fluorescence was computed from 8 slices taken from 4 animals (Figure 5D). Fluorescence was consistently observed ~0.5 mm from the cannula tip in the medial-lateral axis and ~0.7 mm in the dorsal-ventral axis.
4.0 Discussion
4.1 FCM reversibly depresses synaptic transmission
Whole-cell recordings in PR showed that bath application of FCM decreased the mean amplitude of EPSPs and also decreased RN (Figure 2). These results show that the desired neuropharmacological action persists after conjugating muscimol with a large fluorophore. Following washout of FCM from the bath, the recovery of neural function required hours (Figure 2). The slow reversal may result from the lipophilic properties of the Bodipy-TMR-X fluorophore®, as discussed below.
4.2 FCM spread is not always uniform
The spread of FCM can be irregularly shaped, depending on the microanatomy surrounding the infusion site. Figure 3 shows an example in which a 1 μL infusion of FCM into PR cortex resulted in asymmetrical spread. In this case, the diffusion of FCM was impeded by a large fiber bundle that runs along the medial border of PR. Such diffusion barriers can be productively used to restrict the drug to a region of interest. For two reasons, the spread of FCM from the injection site may be much more limited than the spread of muscimol. First, the molecular weight of FCM is approximately 6 times larger than the molecular weight of muscimol. Second, FCM may dissolve in lipid-rich myelinated fibers and cell membranes, which could also slow diffusion.
4.3 FCM infusion into BLA reversibly impairs fear conditioning
Fear conditioning is an ideal task for assessing FCM because much is known about the neurobiology of fear conditioning. In particular, the BLA is essential for normal fear conditioning. This has been shown using both permanent lesions and reversible inactivations (LeDoux, 2000; Maren, 2001). Pretraining infusions of BLA with FCM significantly impaired fear conditioning (Figure 4).
Infusions of FCM into BLA prior to fear conditioning resulted in almost no cued or contextual freezing during testing sessions, which occurred 24 and 48 hr later. Thus fear conditioning to a 22 kHz USV depends on the functioning of BLA at the time of acquisition. Three days later, re-conditioning trials were given with pre-training vehicle infusions. Reconditioning resulted in normal levels of freezing to the cue and context (24 or 48 hr later), showing that the FCM inhibition is temporary. Similarly, previous experiments have shown that muscimol infusions into BLA temporarily impair fear conditioning (Helmstetter and Bellgowan, 1994; Muller et al., 1997; Wilensky et al., 2002; Wilensky et al., 2006). Thus both FCM and muscimol infusions into BLA reversibly impair fear conditioning.
4.4 dmPFC infusions of FCM and muscimol have similar effects
The use of FCM for reversible inactivation was further accessed during performance on a delayed-response task. In each subject, the effects of FCM were directly compared to the effects of muscimol. Both compounds impaired performance to nearly the same extent (Figure 5B). Performance recovered 24 hr later. Muscimol infusions resulted in greater number of premature responses compared to FCM infusions. This difference might be due to a smaller diffusion radius of FCM compared to muscimol.
4.5 Conclusions
A fluorescently-conjugated muscimol molecule was assessed for use in visualizing the spread of reversible brain inactivations. FCM was evaluated physiologically, behaviorally, and optically. We conclude that FCM is valuable for visualizing the spatial gradient of reversible brain inactivations. FCM is likely to find many productive uses in the fields of systems and behavioral neuroscience. By combining FCM infusions with single-unit recordings, it should be possible get insights into the relative contributions of one brain area to the firing patterns in another (see Krupa et al., 2004; Naranayan and Laubach, 2006).
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by a National Institute of Health grant (MH58405) and Yale University to Thomas H. Brown; and supported by a National Science Foundation grant (0642951) and the Kavli foundation awarded to Mark Laubach.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Allen TA, Furtak SC, Brown TH. Single-unit responses to 22 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rat perirhinal cortex. Behav Brain Res. 2007;182(2):327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arikan R, Blake NMJ, Erinjeri JP, Woolsey TA, Giraud L, Highstein SM. A method to measure the effective spread of focally injected muscimol into the central nervous system with electrophysiology and light microscopy J. Neurosci Methods. 2002;118:51–57. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(02)00143-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JW. The production of ultrasonic sounds by laboratory rats and other mammals. Science. 1954;19:808–809. doi: 10.1126/science.119.3101.808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beaumont K, Chilton WS, Yamamura HI, Enna SJ. Muscimol binding in rat brain: Association with synaptic GABA receptors. Brain Res. 1978;148:153–162. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90385-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. Crouching as an index of fear. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1969;67:370–375. doi: 10.1037/h0026779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brudzynski SM. Ultrasonic calls of rats as indicator variables of negative or positive states. Acetylcholine-dopamine interaction and acoustic coding. Behav Brain Res. 2007;182(2):261–273. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic Press; New York: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Edeline J-M, Hars B, Hennevin E, Cotillon N. Muscimol diffusion after intracerebral microinjections: A reevaluation based on electrophysiological and autoradiographic quantifications. Neurobio Learn Mem. 2002;78:100–124. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2001.4035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faulkner B, Brown TH. Morphology and physiology of neurons in the rat perirhinal-lateral amygdala area. J Comp Neurol. 1999;411:613–642. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furtak SC, Allen TA, Brown TH. Single-unit firing in rat perirhinal cortex caused by fear conditioning to arbitrary and ecological stimuli. J Neurosci. 2007;27(45):12277–12291. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1653-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmstetter FJ, Bellgowan PS. Effects of muscimol applied to the basolateral amygdala on acquisition and expression of contextual fear conditioning in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1994;108:1005–1009. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.5.1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka O, Wurtz RH. Modification of saccadic eye movements by GABA-related substances. I. Effect of muscimol and bicuculline in monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol. 1985;53:266–291. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.1.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrard LE. Use of excitotoxic lesion to the hippocampus: update. Hippocampus. 2002;12(3):405–414. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krupa DJ, Thompson R. Reversible inactivation of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus completely prevents acquisition of the classically conditioned eye-blink response. Learn Mem. 1997;3(6):545–556. doi: 10.1101/lm.3.6.545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krupa DJ, Ghazanfar AA, Nicolelis M. Immediate thalamic sensory plasticity depends on corticothalamic feedback. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2004;96:8200–8205. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.8200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindquist DH, Jarrard LE, Brown TH. Perirhinal cortex supports delay fear conditioning to rat ultrasonic social signals. J Neurosci. 2004;24(14):3610–3617. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4839-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomber SG. The advantages and limitations of permanent or reversible deactivation techniques in the assessment of neural function. J Neurosci Methods. 1999;86:109–117. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00160-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Majchrzak M, Di Scala G. GABA and muscimol as reversible inactivation tools in learning and memory. Neural Plast. 2000;7:19–29. doi: 10.1155/NP.2000.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:897–931. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin JH. Autoradiographic estimation of the extent of reversible inactivation produced by microinjection of lidocaine and muscimol in the rat. Neurosci Lett. 1991;127:160–164. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90784-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin JH, Ghez C. Pharmacological inactivation in the analysis of the central control of movement. J Neurosci Methods. 1999;86:145–159. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00163-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moyer JR, Brown TH. Methods for whole-cell recording from visually preselected neurons of perirhinal cortex in brain slices from young and aging rats. J Neurosci Methods. 1998;86(1):35–54. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00143-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moyer JR, Brown TH. Patch-clamp techniques applied to brain slices. In: Walz W, Baker GB, editors. Patch-Clamp Analysis: Advanced Techniques. Vol. 35. Humana Press Inc.; Totowa NJ: 2002. pp. 135–193. [Google Scholar]
- Moyer JR, Brown TH. Visually guided patch-clamp recordings in brain slices. In: Walz W, Baker GB, editors. Patch-Clamp Analysis: Advanced Techniques. Vol. 3. Vol. 38. Humana Press Inc.; Totowa NJ: 2007. pp. 169–227. [Google Scholar]
- Muller J, Corodimas KP, Fridel Z, LeDoux JE. Functional inactivation of the lateral and basal nuclei of the amygdala by muscimol infusion prevents fear conditioning to an explicit conditioned stimulus and to contextual stimuli. Behav Neurosci. 1997;111:683–691. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.111.4.683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narayanan NS, Laubach M. Top-down control of motor cortex ensembles by dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2006;52:921–931. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narayanan NS, Horst NK, Laubach M. Reversible inactivations of rat medial prefrontal cortex impair the ability to wait for a stimulus. Neurosci. 2006;139(3):865–876. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.11.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 3. Academic Press; San Diego: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Wilensky AE, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Functional inactivation of the amygdala before but not after auditory fear conditioning prevents memory formation. J Neurosci. 1999;19(24):RC48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-j0006.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilensky AE, Schafe GE, Kristensen MP, LeDoux JE. Rethinking the fear circuit: The central nucleus of the amygdala is required for the acquisition, consolidation and expression of Pavlovian fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 2006;26(48):12387–12396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4316-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]