Skip to main content
Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health logoLink to Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health
. 2007 Jan;61(1):34–39. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.048389

The population effect of crime and neighbourhood on physical activity: an analysis of 15 461 adults

Roger A Harrison 1,2, Islay Gemmell 1,2, Richard F Heller 1,2
PMCID: PMC2465585  PMID: 17183012

Abstract

Area‐based interventions offer the potential to increase physical activity for many sedentary people in countries such as the UK. Evidence on the effect of individual and area/neighbourhood influences on physical activity is in its infancy, and despite its value to policy makers a population focus is rarely used. Data from a population‐based health and lifestyle survey of adults in northwest England were used to analyse associations between individual and neighbourhood perceptions and physical activity. The population effect of eliminating a risk factor was expressed as a likely effect on population levels of physical activity. Of the 15 461 responders, 21 923 (27.1%) were physically active. Neighbourhood perceptions of leisure facilities were associated with physical activity, but no association was found for sense of belonging, public transport or shopping facilities. People who felt safe in their neighbourhood were more likely to be physically active, but no associations were found for vandalism, assaults, muggings or experience of crime. The number of physically active people would increase by 3290 if feelings of “unsafe” during the day were removed, and by 11 237 if feelings of “unsafe” during the night were removed. An additional 8342 people would be physically active if everyone believed that they were “very well placed for leisure facilities”. Feeling safe had the potential largest effect on population levels of physical activity. Strategies to increase physical activity in the population need to consider the wider determinants of health‐related behaviour, including fear of crime and safety.


In the UK, as many as two thirds of adults live sedentary lives,1,2 representing one of the least physically active nations of 15 European member states.3 Lack of regular physical activity is associated with marked preventable mortality and morbidity4 and is a public health priority. Although efforts to increase physical activity among individuals have had some small effect,5,6,7 modifying social, economic and environmental factors may be more successful at the population level.6 Indeed, evidence is emerging that contextual or area‐level factors, including transport systems, land use mix, population density and leisure opportunities, are related to population levels of physical activity.7,8,9,10 However, few studies have examined this in the UK8 despite sedentary behaviour being a major public health concern. Increasing our understanding of the relationship between physical activity, “who you are” and “where you live”,9 although challenging,6 is essential to inform the development of interventions to seriously reduce the number of people living mainly sedentary lives.

In the general population, regular physical activity is more likely among men, younger adults, people with other healthy lifestyle behaviours (eg, non‐smoking, greater intake of fruit and vegetables), those reporting good general health and no history of chronic disease.2 The aim of the current study was to examine neighbourhood influences on physical activity and to quantify this in terms of the population effect using population impact measures (PIMs).10 PIMs provide a population perspective by adding incidence information to traditional measures of risk, such as the population attribu

risk, thus, providing information on the actual numbers of people who are at risk from specific exposures in a particular population to assist local policy decisions.11

Methods

The study was based in two districts in northwest England, which is divided into 44 administrative electoral wards. Data from the 2001 national census calculated a resident population of 567 600 adults: 94% were white people and the population density was 1700 people per square kilometre.12 Methods for data collection have been described previously.2,13,14 In brief, data were collected using a postal self‐completion questionnaire as part of a population‐based health and lifestyle survey in 2001. The sampling frame was all resident adults on the general practitioner register and systematic sampling was used to select a 5% sample. The postal questionnaire was sent with a covering letter and a business pre‐paid return envelope. Non‐responders were sent a reminder postcard 10 days later. After another 10 days, persistent non‐responders were sent a reminder letter with another copy of the survey and a return envelope. The questionnaire included an introduction in Gujarati and Urdu, the main second languages spoken in the area, with information on the local health translation services. A favourable opinion was received from the local research ethics committees before starting the study.

The 50‐item questionnaire sought information on general and specific health, health behaviours and perceptions of neighbourhood. Question constructs were taken from previous national surveys.1,14 The questions specific to the neighbourhood asked respondents about the following: the extent that they felt they belonged to that area (strongly agree to strongly disagree); how well placed their home was for public transport, general shopping and leisure facilities (very well placed to badly placed); in their neighbourhood how much of a problem was vandalism; assaults and muggings; speeding traffic; and whether they had been the subject of personal crime in the past year. They were also asked whether they felt safe “out and about” in their neighbourhood during the day and during the night. Multiple deprivation was measured using the Townsend Index, which is constructed on four census variables (unemployment, overcrowding, non‐car ownership and non‐home ownership).15 Townsend Scores from 1142 census enumeration districts for the two electoral districts in the study were assigned using the participants' postal code.16

Physical activity was assessed using the Godin and Shephard instrument.17 This is valid for use in epidemiological studies and discriminates between adults participating in different amounts and types of physical activity. Participants were asked to record how many times in the past week they had engaged in light, moderate or vigorous activity for a session lasting at least 15 min. Examples of moderate physical activity included brisk walking, table tennis, easy cycling, golf, dancing and cleaning windows; vigorous activity included running, football, cardiovascular gym workouts and aerobics. In the current analysis, physically active was defined as participating in at least five sessions per week of moderate or vigorous physical activity, with each session lasting at least 15 min.17

Analysis

Individual associations with physical activity and neighbourhood factors were expressed as relative differences (prevalence rate ratios) using a modified Poisson regression approach.18 This involves fitting a generalised linear model to the data with a log link and a Poisson error term. The outcome variable in these models was being physically active, and the predictor variables were the health and lifestyle behaviours. The robust variance estimator was used to adjust for misspecification of the error term. The analyses controlled for the potential confounding effects of age, sex, ethnicity and deprivation. Data were analysed Stata V.8.2 (StataCorp, College Station, Texas, USA).

The population effect of eliminating a risk factor was calculated when the relative risk was statistically significant. The calculation excluded a time element, given the cross‐sectional nature of our data. Its formula is10:

PIN‐ER=n×Ip×PAR

where n is the population size; Ip is the incidence of sedentary behaviour (physical inactivity) in the whole population; PAR is the population attributable risk (Pe(RR−1)/1+Pe(RR−1)); Pe is proportion of the population who is physically inactive; RR is relative risk.

Calculations of the population attributable risk for variables with multiple strata were adjusted according to the methods of Hanley.19

Results

In June 2001, 70.1% of the sample returned a useable questionnaire (15 461/21 923). Their mean age was 49.8 (standard deviation (SD) 17.6) years, 45.2% (6986) were men and 95.5% (14 765) described themselves as Caucasians. The mean age of responders was 8.3 years more than that of non‐responders. No other information on non‐responders was available for comparisons. In all, 27.1% (4193/15 461) of responders defined themselves as being physically active. The mean age of physically active respondents was 10 years lesser than those not defined as being physically active (42.5 v 52.5 years, p = 0.001).

We found no differences in the proportion of men and women who were defined as physically active (27.6% v 26.7%), but those described as Caucasians compared with non‐Caucasians had a higher relative prevalence of physical activity (1.32, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.16 to 1.52). For deprivation, a graded relationship was observed, with the prevalence of physical activity reducing across each of the deprivation quintiles (table 1).

Table 1 Prevalence of physical activity by baseline characteristics.

Number of respondents* Physically active, % (n) Unadjusted relative prevalence (95% CI) Adjusted relative prevalence (95% CI)†
Everyone 15 461 27.1 (4193)
Sex
 Male 6 984 27.6 (1927) 1.00 1.00
 Female 8 477 26.7 (2266) 0.97 (0.92 to 1.02) 0.97 (0.92 to 1.01)
Ethnicity
 Non‐white 689 75.9 (523) 1.00 1.00
 White 14 559 27.5 (4004) 1.14 (1.00 to 1.30) 1.32 (1.16 to 1.52)
Deprivation quintiles‡
 1 (least deprived) 3 512 30.4 (1066) 1.00 1.00
 2 3 037 28.1 (853) 0.93 (0.86 to 1.0) 0.92 (0.85 to 0.99)
 3 2 726 27.8 (759) 0.92 (0.85 to 0.99) 0.90 (0.84 to 0.97)
 4 2 812 26.1 (734) 0.86 (0.79 to 0.93) 0.85 (0.78 to 0.91)
 5 (most deprived) 3 279 22.9 (752) 0.76 (0.70 to 0.82) 0.77 (0.72 to 0.84)

*Not all respondents answered every question.

†Adjusted for all variables in the table.

‡Townsend Score at enumeration level as a proxy for individual deprivation.

Looking at neighbourhood factors, a graded relationship was observed between how well people thought their neighbourhood was for leisure facilities and the prevalence of being physically active (table 2).

Table 2 Association of physical activity with individual perceptions of neighbourhood facilities.

Variable Number of respondents* Physically active, % (n) Unadjusted relative prevalence (95% CI) Adjusted relative prevalence (95% CI)†
How well placed for leisure facilities?
 Very well 2926 30.3 ( 886) 1.00 1.00
 Fairly well 3792 29.4 (1115) 0.97 (0.90 to 1.05) 0.95 (0.88 to 1.02)
 Average 4212 28.6 (1203) 0.94 (0.88 to 1.01) 0.96 (0.89 to 1.03)
 Not very well 2192 26.3 (576) 0.87 (0.79 to 0.95) 0.90 (0.82 to 0.98)
 Badly 1493 23.0 (344) 0.76 (0.68 to 0.85) 0.86 (0.77 to 0.94)
Feel of belonging to the area?
 Strongly agree 3300 26.2 (864) 1.00 1.00
 Agree 5928 26.9 (1594) 1.03 (0.96 to 1.10) 0.98 (0.92 to 1.05)
 Neither agree nor disagree 4069 30.3 (1232) 1.16 (1.07 to 1.25) 0.99 (0.92 to 1.06)
 Disagree 1042 28.7 (299) 1.10 (0.98 to 1.23) 0.93 (0.84 to 1.04)
 Strongly disagree 451 29.1 (131) 1.11 (0.95 to 1.30) 0.90 (0.77 to 1.04)
How well placed for transport?
 Very well 6728 27.9 (1878) 1.00 1.00
 Fairly well 4380 28.4 (1244) 1.02 (0.96 to 1.08) 0.98 (0.92 to 1.04)
 Average 2668 26.9 (718) 0.96 (0.90 to 1.04) 0.95 (0.88 to 1.01)
 Not very well 780 28.1 (219) 1.01 (0.89 to 1.13) 1.01 (0.90 to 1.13)
 Badly 335 24.2 (81) 0.87 (0.71 to 1.05) 0.89 (0.73 to 1.08)
How well placed for general shopping?
 Very well 5059 28.1 (1423) 1.00 1.00
 Fairly well 4626 29.1 (1344) 1.03 (0.97 to 1.10) 1.00 (0.94 to 1.06)
 Average 3633 27.3 (991) 0.97 (0.90 to 1.04) 0.95 (0.90 to 1.01)
 Not very well 1183 23.9 (283) 0.85 (0.76 to 0.95) 0.93 (0.83 to 1.03)
 Badly 504 24.2 (122) 0.86 (0.73 to 1.01) 1.00 (0.86 to 1.17)

*Not all respondents answered every question.

†Adjusted for age, sex, ethnicity and deprivation (Townsend Score at enumeration district).

We found no association between physical activity and sense of “belonging” to their neighbourhood, how well placed they believed their neighbourhood was for public transport and for general shopping (table 2).

People who felt unsafe out and about in their neighbourhood during the day (relative prevalence 0.70, 95% CI 0.59 to 0.82) and during the night (relative prevalence 0.82, 95% CI 0.78 to 0.88) were significantly less likely to be defined as physically active compared with those who felt safe during these times (table 3).

Table 3 Association of individual perceptions of crime and safety with physical activity.

Number of respondents* Physically active, % (n) Unadjusted relative prevalence (95% CI) Adjusted relative prevalence (95% CI)†
Feel safe out in neighbourhood
 During the day?
  Yes 14 155 28.3 (4009) 1.00 1.00
  No 758 15.8 (120) 0.55 (0.47 to 0.66) 0.70 (0.59 to 0.82)
 During the night?
  Yes 9 601 31.1 (2982) 1.00 1.00
  No 5 305 21.6 (1148) 0.70 (0.66 to 0.74) 0.82 (0.78 to 0.88)
How much of a problem to you are any of the following
 Vandalism?
  Not a problem 5 424 28.0 (1518) 1.00 1.00
  Some problem 7 799 28.5 (2221) 1.01 (0.96 to 1.08) 1.05 (1.00 to 1.11)
  Serious problem 1 525 25.6 (391) 0.92 (0.83 to 1.01) 1.01 (0.92 to 1.12)
 Assaults or muggings?
  Not a problem 10 161 28.8 (2921) 1.00 1.00
  Some problem 3 817 28.3 (1079) 0.98 (0.93 to 1.04) 1.01 (0.95 to 1.07)
  Serious problem 372 25.3 (94) 0.88 (0.74 to 1.05) 0.91 (0.77 to 1.08)
 Speeding traffic?
  Not a problem 5 491 26.8 (1474) 1.00 1.00
  Some problem 6 255 29.4 (18.7) 1.09 (1.03 to 1.16) 1.08 (1.10 to 1.14)
  Serious problem 2 925 28.2 (824) 1.05 (0.98 to 1.13) 1.04 (0.97 to 1.11)
Personal experience of crime in the past year?
 Yes 2 649 29.6 (786) 1.09 (1.02 to 1.17) 0.97 (0.91 to 1.03)
 No 12 404 27.2 (3370) 1.00 1.00

*Not all respondents answered every question.

†Adjusted for age, sex, ethnicity and deprivation (Townsend Score at enumeration district).

We observed no association for physical activity and people stating that vandalism, and assaults or muggings were a problem in their neighbourhood, also not among people who had or not been victims of personal crime during the past year. People who thought that there was some problem with speeding traffic in their neighbourhood were more likely to be physically active, but this was not consistent to this being a serious problem.

Table 4 shows the population effect of eliminating statistically significant risk factors for sedentary behaviour.

Table 4 Estimated population effect on physical activity from changing neighbourhood perceptions.

Number Number of respondents answering this question RR of being sedentary Pe PAR Number of adults in this population Proportion physically active overall PIN‐ER*
Feel unsafe
 During the day 758 14 913 1.43 0.05 0.021 567 600 0.27 3 290
 During the night 5 305 14 606 1.23 0.34 0.073 567 600 0.27 11 237
How well placed for leisure facilities
 Very well 2 926 14 615 Ref Ref Ref
 Fairly well 3 792 14 615 1.05 0.26 0.01
 Average 4 212 14 615 1.04 0.29 0.01
 Not very well 2 192 14 615 1.11 0.15 0.02
 Badly 1 493 14 615 1.16 0.10 0.02
Overall PAR 0.05 567 600 0.27 8 342

PAR, population attributable risk; Pe, proportion of the population who is physically inactive, PIN‐ER, population effect of eliminating a risk factor.

*The number of people expected to become physically active if everyone in this population felt safe during the day, or felt safe during the night, or thought their neighbourhood was very well placed for leisure facilities, calculated as PIN‐ER = n×Ip×PAR, where n is the population size; Ip, the incidence of sedentary behaviour (physical inactivity) in the whole population; PAR = (Pe(RR−1)/1+Pe(RR−1)).

The data suggest that the number of physically active people would increase by 3290 if feelings of being unsafe during the day were removed, and by 11 237 if feelings of being unsafe during the night were removed. An additional 8342 people would be physically active if everyone believed that they were “very well placed for leisure facilities”. In absolute terms, this would be expected to increase the current level of physical activity in the population by 0.6%, 2.0% and 1.5%, respectively (table 5).

Table 5 Number of people in the total population expected to become physically active if neighbourhood perceptions improved.

Number of adults in this population Currently physically active (%)* Currently physically active (n)† PIN‐ER‡ Expected to be physically active (n)§ Expected to be physically active (%)¶ Absolute increase in people expected to be physically active (%)**
Feel safe
 During the day 567 600 27.0 153 252 3 290 156 542 27.6 0.6
 During the night 567 600 27.0 153 252 11 237 164 489 29.0 2.0
How well placed for leisure facilities
 Very well 567 600 27.0 153 252 8 342 161 594 28.5 1.5

PIN‐ER, population effect of eliminating a risk factor.

*Observed in the survey.

†Calculated as number of adults in the population×(percentage of adults physically active/100).

‡From table 4.

§Number of adults physically active+PIN‐ER.

¶Expected number in the population to be physically active as a proportion of total population.

**Difference between expected percentage of the population to be physically active and percentage of the population currently physically active.

Discussion

Our work represents one of the most comprehensive assessments of individual and contextual associations with physical activity among adults in the UK general population. We have previously confirmed low levels of physical activity among several adults, which decreased with advancing age and by socioeconomic deprivation.2 The focus of the current investigation was to examine the association of physical activity with contextual factors, based on the notion that both individual and contextual factors can influence physical activity. We found that individual perceptions of how well placed their neighbourhood was for leisure facilities were considerably associated with physical activity. The fact that this increased across each response category adds strength to this dose–response association. We also found that feeling safe in the neighbourhood during the day or during the night was positively associated with physical activity. Our approach of applying population effect measures suggested that the greatest increase in physical activity would be achieved in the population if everyone was made to feel safe during the night, with only a small effect if everyone was made to feel that their neighbourhood was well placed for leisure facilities. Therefore, if we are to increase population levels of physical activity, increasing feelings of safety seems to be a greater priority than improving perceptions regarding the provision of leisure facilities.

In our study, we failed to find any consistent association between physical activity and sense of belonging to the neighbourhood or perceptions about transport or shopping facilities, or problems in the neighbourhood from unsociable and criminal behaviours. Perhaps these did not differ sufficiently across the study setting to influence physical activity or among this population these factors may have had little effect on this behaviour.

The strengths of our study are its population focus, a large sample size with good response rates and data on a wide range of possible effects on physical activity. The survey included validated questions and reflected those used in national surveys and surveillance systems. We also adjusted for the potential confounding effects of area deprivation, using the participants' postcode linked to deprivation data at an enumeration level. Although this method has been found to be an effective method to examine the effect of individual deprivation on health,16 some misclassification may have taken place.

PIMs are a recently described addition to other measures of population effect, such as population attributable risk.10 PIMs add information on incidence to estimate the number of people in a total population who may benefit (or be at risk) from an intervention. As such, they provide a population perspective to inform local policy decisions.16,20 In the current study, this method has been used to estimate the effect of neighbourhood and neighbourhood perceptions on sedentary behaviour in adults.

Our study relied on self‐reported measures, which may be subject to measurement error, and our control for confounders was limited to the data originally collected. Simple methods for assessing physical activity have been found to reliably predict outcomes such as mortality,21 supporting their wide application in epidemiological studies. Response bias is a known problem in population studies and just <30% of those in the original sample did not return a useable questionnaire. A previous study found that non‐responders were less likely to be physically active compared with responders.22 Therefore, the true prevalence of sedentary behaviour in the population studied might be more than what we observed.

The main weakness of our cross‐sectional study is that a cause–effect relationship between the factors we examined and their effect on physical activity cannot be assumed. We have been careful to use the term “association” rather than “relationship”. Therefore, our calculation of the population effect of eliminating a risk factor, which assumes a cause–effect relationship, needs to be interpreted with caution. We make no claim here that making people feel safe in their neighbourhood would, in itself, increase the number of people who would be physically active. Rather, we have applied PIMs to highlight the potential effect of changes in particular neighbourhood factors on physical activity, and state that intervention studies are the only sure way to examine their effect. However, in practice, given the paucity of community‐based evaluations, policy makers often rely on cause–effect relationships to be assumed to some degree. We have merely applied a population perspective to such interpretation.

Few studies have previously examined the influence of feelings of safety on physical activity, particularly in the UK. A small cross‐sectional study in England23 found that women were more likely to walk at least 15 min a week if they felt safe during the day. In the US, perceptions of safety for walking were associated with actual walking,24 and crime was perceived as more of a problem in socially deprived areas that also had low levels of physical activity.25 Similarly, Americans who perceived their neighbourhood as less than extremely safe were more than twice as likely to have no leisuretime physical activity, and those who considered it to be not at all safe were nearly three times as likely to have no leisuretime physical activity.26 However, in a Danish study, although participating in sports activities was inversely related to perceptions about the amount of police attention their neighbourhood received,27 it was not found to influence walking and cycling activities.

What this paper adds

  • Few studies have considered the wider determinants of health on levels of physical activity in the population.

  • Feeling safe in the home and out and about in the neighbourhood may have as large an effect on population levels of physical activity as factors such as access to leisure facilities.

Policy implications

  • Making people feel safer in their neighbourhood is a key priority to increase population levels of physical activity.

Evidence on the possible role of perceptions relating to the location of leisure facilities and physical activity is conflicting. Our own findings support an independent association of perceived access to recreational facilities and physical activity, although its population effect was much less than for feelings of safety. This differs to the earlier study in England,23 but supports findings in Australia28 and the US.29,30 Consequently, we argue the urgent need to carry out prospective studies in the UK, which, wherever possible, will make full use of the many “natural experiments” around the country to obtain reliable evidence on the effect of contextual changes on population levels of physical activity.

Conclusion

Our study suggests that feeling unsafe in the neighbourhood is as much of a barrier to physical activity as how well people thought their home was for access to leisure facilities. As such, strategies to increase physical activity need to emphasise the perceived effect of feeling safe among the local population. Encouraging people to spend more time walking for leisure and commuting purposes seems to be a sensible approach to incorporate physical activity within activities of daily living. For this to become a reality, we need to start by ensuring that people feel safe out and about in their neighbourhood.

Abbreviations

PIM - population impact measure

Footnotes

Competing interests: None declared.

References

  • 1. Department of Health Health survey for England 2004. Updating of trends data to include 2004 data London, DoH, 2005. http://www.ic.nhs.uk/pubs/hlthsvyeng2004upd (accessed 28 Apr 2006)
  • 2.Harrison R A, McElduff P, Edwards R. Planning to win: health and lifestyles associated with physical activity amongst 15423 adults. Public Health 2006120206–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Vaz de Almeida M D, Grace P, Afonso C.et al Physical activity levels and body weight in a nationally representative sample in the European Union. Public Health Nutr 19992105–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.World Health Organization World health report. Geneva: WHO, 2002
  • 5.Cavill N, Buxton K, Bull F.et alPromotion of activity among adults: evidence into practice briefing. London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence, 2006
  • 6.Lawlor D A, Ness A R, Cope A M.et al The challenges of evaluating environmental interventions to increase population levels of physical activity: the case of the UK National Cycle Network. J Epidemiol Community Health 20035796–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Berrigan D, Troiano R P. The association between urban form and physical activity in U.S. adults. Am J Prev Med 200223(2 Suppl)74–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hillsdon M, Foster C, Naidoo B.et al The effectiveness of public health interventions for increasing physical activity among adults: a review of reviews. In: Evidence briefing. London: Health Development Agency, 2004
  • 9.Boslaugh S E, Luke D A, Brownson R C.et al Perceptions of neighbourhood environment for physical activity: is it “Who you are” or “Where you live”? J Urban Health 200481671–681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Heller R.Evidence for population health. Oxford: Oxford Medical Publications, 2005
  • 11.Heller R F, Buchan I, Edwards R.et al Communicating risks at the population level: application of population impact numbers. BMJ 20033271162–1165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Office for National Statistics 2005. http://neighbourhood.statistics.gov.uk (accessed 27 Jun 2005)
  • 13.Harrison R A, Holt D, Elton P J. Do postage stamps increase response rates to postal health surveys? A randomised controlled trial. Int J Epidemiol 200231872–874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Office for National Statistics Census 2001. General report for England and Wales. London: Palgrave Macmillan, 2005
  • 15.Townsend P, Phillimore P, Beattie A.Health and deprivation: inequality and the North. London: Croom Helm, 1988
  • 16.Adams J, Ryan V, White M. How accurate are Townsend Deprivation Scores as predictors of self‐reported health? A comparison with individual level data. J Public Health 200427101–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Godin G, Shephard R. A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community. Can J Appl Sports Sci 198510141–146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zou G. A modified Poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. Am J Epidemiol 2004159702–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hanley J A. A heuristic approach to the formulas for population attributable fraction. J Epidemiol Community Health 200155508–514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Gemmell I, Heller R F, McElduff P.et al Population impact of stricter adherence to recommendations for pharmacological and lifestyle interventions over one year in patients with coronary heart disease. J Epidemiol Community Health 2005591041–1046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hillsdon M, Thorogood M, Murphy M.et al Can a simple measure of vigorous physical activity predict future mortality? Results from the OXCHECK study. Public Health Nutr 20037557–562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hill A, Roberts J, Ewings P.et al Non‐response bias in a lifestyle survey. J Public Health 199719203–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Foster C, Hillsdon M, Thorogood M. Environmental perceptions and walking in English adults. J Epidemiol Community Health 200458924–928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Li F, Fisher J, Brownson R C.et al Multilevel modelling of build environment characteristics related to neighbourhood walking activity in older adults. J Epidemiol Community Health 200559558–564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wilson D K, Kirtland K A, Ainswort B E.et al Socioeconomic status and perceptions of access and safety for physical activity. Ann Behav Med 20042820–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Morbidity Mortality Weekly Reports Perceptions of neighbourhood characteristics and leisure‐time physical inactivity—Austin/Travis County, Texas, 2004. MMWR 200554926–928. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Van Lenthe F J, Brug J, Mackenback J P. Neighbourhood inequalities in physical inactivity: the role of neighbourhood attractiveness, proximity to local facilities and safety in the Netherlands. Soc Sci Med 200560763–775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ball K, Bauman A, Leslie E.et al Perceived environmental aesthetics and convenience and company are associated with walking for exercise among Australian adults. Prev Med 200133434–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Booth M L, Owen N, Bauman A.et al Social‐cognitive and perceived environmental influences associated with physical activity in older Australians. Prev Med 20003115–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Parks S E, Housemann R A, Brownson R C. Differential correlates of physical activity in urban and rural adults of various socio‐economic backgrounds in the United States. J Epidemiol Community Health 20035729–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES