Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Adolesc. 2007 Jul 24;31(3):307–321. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.06.002

Short-term Changes in Plans to Drink and Importance of Positive and Negative Alcohol Consequences

Megan E Patrick a, Jennifer L Maggs a
PMCID: PMC2484119  NIHMSID: NIHMS53365  PMID: 17651796

Clinicians and prevention scientists who implement indicated interventions seek to intervene in the midst of an ongoing process (Weissberg, Kumpfer, & Seligman, 2003). For example, those working with high risk drinkers encounter individuals who have initiated the behavior (i.e., heavy alcohol use) and who therefore already hold specific motivations and attitudes regarding their behavior and its consequences (Dunn & Goldman, 1998; Schulenberg & Maggs, 2002). These past experiences and present beliefs about alcohol may perpetuate and sustain emerging trajectories of use (Greenbaum, Del Boca, Darkes, Wang, & Goldman, 2005), which in turn have significant implications for adolescent health and development (Hawkins, Catalano, & Miller, 1992). The reciprocal relations between anticipatory cognitions and behavior should be recognized and articulated to fully capture the dynamic feedback of these constructs (Cooper, Frone, Russell, & Mudar, 1995), and their effects on adolescent risk behaviors.

Intervention programs that are designed to reduce alcohol use hypothesize about specific mediating constructs, such as alcohol expectancies, but these are often left unmeasured or empirically untested (Botvin & Griffin, 2004; Del Boca, Darkes, Goldman, & Smith, 2002; Jensen, Weersing, Hoagwood, & Goldman, 2005; Petraitis, Flay, & Miller, 1995). However, a defining feature of developmental science, clinical psychology, and prevention science alike is their focus on the importance of understanding and evaluating proximal predictors and processes (Catalano, Hawkins, Berglund, Pollard, & Arthur, 2002; Clingempeel & Henggeler, 2002). Proximal mediators that operate within a short time span, such as alcohol expectancies and motivations, may be both more amenable to the effects of intervention and more influential in creating behavioral changes (e.g., Goldman, 1999) than more distal mediators such as a family history of alcoholism (Hawkins et al., 1992). The current study examines whether anticipatory cognitions about alcohol evidence short-term changes (i.e., across weeks) following the experience of positive and negative consequences of drinking using diary data from a sample of first-year university students.

Alcohol Use Among Post-Secondary Students

Understanding the ways in which adolescents and emerging adults respond to their experiences and create expectations about future drinking is important because of the associated effects on physical, social, and emotional health and development. Among American college students, severe consequences resulting from alcohol use have a high prevalence, including damage to self, others, and institutions (Perkins, 2002). Hingson, Heeren, Winter, and Wechsler (2005) reported that over 500 000 students in the United States are unintentionally injured each year as a result of their own drinking and over 600 000 are hit or assaulted by other drinking students. College-bound students tend to use less alcohol than their non-college-bound peers during high school, but surpass their age mates in consumption during their university years (O’Malley & Johnston, 2002). As a result, the transition to college is an opportune time to investigate how an individual’s personal characteristics and experiences shape development and behavioral choices (Maggs et al., 2006).

Theories about substance use, including social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 2000), and expectancy-value theory (Hays, 1985; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000) highlight the theoretical importance of perceptions of the costs and benefits of substance use and substance-specific cognitions (see Petraitis et al., 1995 for a review). These theoretical traditions propose that individuals’ evaluations of the rewards and costs of substance use behavior determine the level and frequency of use. Empirical evidence also illustrates the link, as planned and actual drinking and binge drinking are highly correlated (.69 and .67, respectively) among college students (Maggs, 1997), and intentions to drink before a party are significantly correlated with subsequent blood alcohol concentration (Glindemann, Geller, & Ludwig, 1996). Several interventions built on these ideas have targeted drinking among college students. Two popular strategies for intervention are motivational interviewing (e.g., Baer, Kivlihan, Blume, McKnight, & Marlatt, 2001; Borsari & Carey, 2000; Murphy et al., 2001) and public health social marketing campaigns, which highlight risks and aim to correct inflated perceived norms of campus drinking (e.g., Graham, Tatterson, Roberts, & Johnston, 2004; Neighbors, Larimer, & Lewis, 2004; Wechsler et al., 2003). Both types of programs aim to alter how students think about their behavior and its consequences, such that plans for behavior and perceptions of consequences are targeted as the mediators through which behavior change is hypothesized to be achieved (e.g., Borsari & Carey, 2000). Less research attention, however, has been focused on short-term influences on and variation of such anticipatory cognitions. The present paper focuses specifically on the role played by personal experiences with alcohol in predicting short-term changes in anticipatory cognitions, including plans to drink and the subjective importance of experiencing positive consequences and avoiding negative consequences of alcohol use.

Alcohol Expectancies and Consequences

Research on individual factors for alcohol use has addressed the role of alcohol expectancies and motivations in predicting alcohol use cross-sectionally and longitudinally over months and years (e.g., Baer, 2002; Goldman, Del Boca, & Darkes, 1999; Komro et al., 2001; Leigh, 1989). Among adolescents and young adults, positive expectancies, more than negative expectancies, are particularly predictive of levels of between-person differences in alcohol use and heavy drinking (Leigh & Stacy, 2004) and perceived benefits are important determinants of behavioral intentions (Parsons, Siegel, & Cousins, 1997). Positive expectancies are also strongly associated with experiencing positive alcohol consequences and with alcohol use behavior among college students (Park & Grant, 2005). Men and women both experience positive consequences of alcohol more frequently than negative consequences (Park & Grant, 2005), although men experience more overall positive and negative consequences of alcohol than do women (Park, 2004), which is consistent with the fact that men consume alcohol more frequently and in greater quantities per occasion (Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2005; Wilsnack & Wilsnack, 2002).

In contrast to an extensive body of research documenting that alcohol expectancies predict use and consequences (Goldman, 1994; Goldman, Brown, & Christiansen, 1987; Goldman, Darkes, & Del Boca, 1999), in the current study we focus on whether alcohol expectancies evidence short-term changes in response to experienced consequences of drinking. Recent research suggests that experienced consequences of alcohol use may affect motivations (Blume, Schmaling, & Marlatt, 2006), although there is some evidence that the heaviest drinkers fail to change their behavior in response to negative alcohol effects (Mallett, Lee, Neighbors, Larimer, & Turrisi, 2006; McCarthy, Pedersen, & Leuty, 2005). Therefore, the relative importance of experienced positive and negative consequences in predicting both behavior and cognitions provides a unique and important perspective for research on the determinants of short-term changes in alcohol use. Questions surrounding how experiencing consequences may affect short-term changes in anticipatory cognitions largely remain to be answered, although they are theoretically proximal causes of alcohol use behavior.

The Current Study

The current analyses address several factors that may influence an individual’s anticipatory cognitions, which were operationalized as plans to drink and the subjective importance of avoiding negative drinking consequences and of attaining positive drinking consequences. Using plans and subjective importance of alcohol consequences as outcome variables, rather than as predictors, is unusual. The present study focuses on plans to drink, and on the conscious and intended consumption of alcohol, as opposed to prior alcohol use which may be more heavily influenced by situational pressures and environmental influences (Baer, 2002). The variance associated with this planned aspect of behavior may be especially important because of its centrality as a targeted mediator in many intervention programs designed to reduce alcohol use among adolescents and college students (Botvin & Griffin, 2004; Del Boca et al., 2002; Jensen, Weersing, Hoagwood, & Goldman, 2005; Petraitis et al., 1995). Therefore, modeling short-term changes in these constructs in response to experienced consequences may provide insights into the etiology of proximal predictors of behavioral intentions as well as valuable information toward the development of more effective interventions.

Research Questions

This study investigates the associations of three predictors with plans to drink and the subjective importance of potential positive and negative alcohol consequences over 10 weeks in one semester. Experiencing more positive and more negative consequences across weeks is hypothesized to be associated with anticipatory cognitions that support drinking behavior (i.e., plans to drink more, greater importance of experiencing positive consequences, less importance of avoiding negative consequences) because individuals who consume more alcohol experience more positive and negative consequences (Park, 2004). Pilot data from first-year university students showed that experiencing positive consequences during the previous three weeks led to a reported greater importance of attaining positive consequences and lesser importance of avoiding negative consequences (Maggs, 1993). Experiencing negative consequences between occasions of measurement was not associated with changes in anticipatory cognitions in this pilot sample. Other work has shown that experiencing negative consequences is positively correlated with positive alcohol expectancies (Park & Grant, 2005). Although counter-intuitive, experiencing negative consequences may fail to lead to a decrease in alcohol use because of the correlation between experiencing positive consequences and experiencing negative consequences, and the relatively stronger influence of positive consequences on students’ intentions for future drinking (Park, 2004).

Two central research questions are examined, the first focusing on between-person differences and the second on within-person short-term changes or fluctuations.

  1. Do males and students who experience more positive and negative alcohol-related consequences on average across weeks (a) plan to drink more, (b) report greater importance of potential positive consequences, and (c) report lesser importance of potential negative consequences of alcohol use?

  2. Week to week, do individuals’ plans to drink and subjective importance of potential drinking consequences change systematically in response to experiencing positive and negative consequences of alcohol use the week prior?

Method

Procedures

The University Life Transitions project was conducted at a large state university in the Southwest U.S. (see Lee, Maggs, & Rankin, 2006; Rankin & Maggs, in press). Students (N = 943) completed initial paper surveys during first-year orientation sessions the summer prior to college entry (98% response rate). Incentives were a t-shirt and entry into a raffle for §20 at each data collection session. From these orientation surveys, students were recruited for participation in a 10-week telephone diary study. Participants were telephoned once each week for 10 weeks, and compensated with a nominal payment (§20) at the end of the study. Brief telephone interviews regarding alcohol use and related behaviors and attitudes were conducted once per week, for a total of 10 interviews per participant. All procedures were approved by the institutional review board at the university where the data were gathered, and procedures for the ethical treatment of human subjects were followed.

Participants

Eligible students for the diary study (a) were in their first year (96% of Orientation sample); (b) were under 21 years of age, the local legal age for alcohol consumption (99.8%); (c) were living in on-campus housing (86.3%); (d) agreed to be contacted (64.6%); and (e) had not abstained from alcohol in their final year of high school (79.3%). As a result of the focus on within-person variation, alcohol abstainers were not included in the diary sample. Telephone numbers were identified for 342 of the 390 students who met the eligibility criteria; 87 were not reached. Of the 255 contacted, 90% agreed to participate in the diary study and 69% provided sufficient data to be included in the present analyses. The telephone diary participants were 63% women, 84% white, and 32% sorority/fraternity members, with a mean of 18.8 years (SD = 0.4). The dataset used for these analyses included 176 individuals and 1742 weeks (of 1760 possible [176 × 10] weeks) of data on each of the between-person measures and at least partial data on the within-person measures of interest.

Measures

Data were collected weekly for a period of 10 weeks, to provide both between-person and within-person variance. Participants reported their experienced consequences of drinking by indicating whether they had experienced any of 23 alcohol use consequences in the previous 7 days. Positive consequences (13 items, α = .90; e.g., became more social, had fun) and negative consequences (10 items, α = .79; e.g., had a hangover, did/said something embarrassing) were computed separately as the mean of responses within positive and negative domains. Consequence scores are used as between-person predictors (person mean across weeks) and within-person predictors (individual deviation from person mean on a given week).

Two variables were also assessed as outcomes in the model. Plans to drink were reported by participants at each telephone interview. The number of standard drinks individuals expected to consume during the following seven days was reported. The intra-class correlation (ICC) indicated that 65% of the variance in plans to drink over 10 weeks was between-persons (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002; Snijders & Bosker, 1999). In other words, although people differ from one another on average as expected, 35% of the variance resulted from within-person fluctuations in plans to drink from week-to-week from how people differ from themselves across weeks. The presence of meaningful within-person variation allows for the modeling of variation in behavior across weeks.

Importance of Alcohol Consequences was assessed using the Importance of Consequences of Drinking (ICOD) measure (Maggs, Vesterdal, & Galambos, 2004). This 21-item scale measures the importance of achieving potential positive alcohol consequences (e.g., have more fun, unwind, maintain your reputation) and avoiding potential negative alcohol consequences (e.g., avoid passing out, avoid getting in a car accident) as a result of drinking alcohol on scale of 1 = not important to 5 = very important. These scales capture an individual’s perspective on the value of gains or losses that may result from future drinking behavior, rather than evaluating consequences that were already experienced. The importance of avoiding potential negative consequences (Neg-ICOD) was computed as the mean of responses from negative items (10 items, α = .89). The importance of attaining potential positive consequences (Pos-ICOD) was indicated by the mean of responses from positive items (13 items, α = .91). Each week’s importance ratings referred to the subjective importance of experiencing consequences in the upcoming week (Level 1, within-person fluctuations) and a person’s mean across weeks reflected the subjective importance of experiencing consequences overall (Level 2, between-person differences). Variance in Neg-ICOD indicated by ICC was 74% between-persons and 26% within-persons over the 10 weeks; 78% of variance in Pos-ICOD was between-persons, and 22% was within-persons. Although there are differences between people on average, about a quarter of variation is within-people over time.

Results

Plan of Analysis

Multi-level models estimated within- and between-person variation using hierarchical linear modeling software (HLM; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). The multi-level approach models both between-person variance in the outcomes (e.g., by gender) and within-person variance in the outcomes (e.g., by weekly consequences) so that we can test whether individuals differ from each other on average and from themselves across multiple occasions (Singer & Willett, 2003). Ten occasions of each Level 1 predictor (i.e., positive and negative consequences experienced) and 10 occasions of each outcome (i.e., plans to drink, Neg-ICOD, Pos-ICOD) were nested within persons. Outcomes of interest were plans to drink, Neg-ICOD, and Pos-ICOD (see equations in Table 1). Although the dependent variables were positively skewed, Level 1 residuals for the final models of all three outcomes were symmetrically distributed. Between-person (Level 2) predictors were gender and the person means of experienced positive and negative consequences over the 10 weeks. Within-person (Level 1) variables were centered within persons; the resulting deviation scores represent week-to-week variation in experienced negative and positive consequences (Kreft, de Leeuw, & Aiken, 1995). For example, weekly experienced consequences indicated the extent to which individuals experienced more or fewer consequences than they usually did on a given week (i.e., deviated from their own means).

Table 1.

Predicting Short-term Changes in Plans to Drink and Anticipatory Cognitions Regarding Alcohol Use

Anticipatory Cognitions for the Subsequent Week
Planned Drinks B (SE) Neg-ICOD B (SE) Pos-ICOD B (SE)
Average Outcome over 10 Weeks, β0
   Intercept, γ0 −0.35(0.76) 4.56(0.08)*** 1.36(0.07)***
   Male Gender, γ01 2.53(0.81)** −0.21(0.09)* 0.09(0.07)
   Negative Consequences Mean, γ02 33.07(5.15)*** −1.76(0.55)** −0.12(0.44)
   Positive Consequences Mean, γ03 14.37(2.53)*** −0.55(0.27)* 2.99(0.22)***
   SB Trip (Control), γ04 0.60(0.81) 0.08(0.09) −0.13(0.07)
Average Fluctuations in Weekly Negative Consequences, β1
   Intercept, γ10 0.09(1.32) −0.09(0.10) −0.17(0.09)
Average Fluctuations in Weekly Positive Consequences, β2
   Intercept, γ20 1.80(0.69)* −0.05(0.06) 0.50(0.06)***
Average Effect of SB Week (Control), β3
   Intercept, γ30 5.04(1.41)** −0.12(0.07) 0.05(0.07)
   SB Trip (Control), γ31 3.89(1.81)* −0.06(0.09) 0.06(0.08)
*

p < .05

**

p <. 01

***

p < .001.

Note. Level 1 N = 1742 person weeks, Level 2 N = 176 people. β coefficients (Level 1) are estimated for each person. γ coefficients (Level 2) are aggregate estimates across the sample and are presented in the table.

Level 1: Outcome (Anticipatory Cognitions) = β0 + β1 (Neg Consequences) + β2 (Pos Consequences) + β3 (SB Week) + rit

Level 2: β0 = γ00 + γ01 (Gender) + γ02(Neg Cons Mean) + γ03(Pos Cons Mean) + γ04(SB Trip) + U0

β1 = γ10

β2 = γ20

β3 = γ30 + γ31(SB Trip) + U3

Although it was not the focus of the present study, because Spring Break occurred during the data collection period, controls were added to the model to account for associated variation. Lee, Rankin, and Maggs (2006) demonstrated that actual alcohol use and heavy drinking increased during this week, particularly among students who went on Spring Break trips. These controls were dummy-coded variables indicating whether a given week was Spring Break (Level 1), and whether the individual went on a Spring Break trip (Level 2). Spring Break week was associated with an increase in planned drinks across the sample, as well as additional increases in planned drinks for individuals who went on trips.

Description of the Variables

Means, standard deviations, and ranges are shown for the Level 1 variables in Table 2. On average across weeks, male students planned to consume 10.34 (SD = 12.57) drinks and female students planned to consume 6.44 (SD = 7.53) drinks during the subsequent week. On average, participants reported that it was less important to attain positive consequences (M = 1.98, SD = 0.79 on a scale of 1 = not important to 5 = very important) and more important to them to avoid negative consequences (M = 4.25, SD = 0.70), paired-samples t (175) = 76.22, p <.001. However, students reported experiencing more positive consequences (M = 0.23, SD = 0.27 over 13 items, or 2.99 positive consequences per week) than negative consequences (M = 0.08, SD = 0.15 over 10 items, or 0.8 negative consequences per week), paired-samples t (175) = −12.26, p < .001. More than 3 out of 4 students (77.8%) reported binge drinking at least once during the study (defined as 4 or more drinks for women and 5 or more drinks for men). Four out of five students (80.1%) reported negative consequences at least once during the 10 week period; 91.5% reported positive consequences.

Table 2.

Descriptive Statistics for Level 1 Constructs

M SD Range
Predictors
   Positive Consequences 0.23 0.27 0–1
   Negative Consequences 0.08 0.15 0–0.9
Anticipatory Cognition Outcomes
   Planned Drinks 7.49 9.42 0–50
   Pos-ICODa 1.98 0.79 1–5
   Neg-ICODb 4.25 0.70 1–5

Note. Level 1 N = 1742 person weeks, Level 2 N = 176 weeks.

a

Pos-ICOD = Importance of Positive Alcohol Consequences.

b

Neg-ICOD = Importance of Negative Alcohol Consequences.

Between-Person Differences in Anticipatory Cognitions

Between-person differences in the three outcome variables were assessed with the Level 2 portion of the multi-level model (Table 1). On average, men (γ01) planned to drink more and had lower Neg-ICOD ratings than did females. Individuals who experienced more negative consequences on average over the 10 weeks (γ02) planned to drink more and had lower Neg-ICOD ratings. Students who experienced more positive alcohol consequences on average over the 10 weeks (γ03) planned to drink more and reported lower Neg-ICOD and higher Pos-ICOD scores.

Short-Term Changes in Anticipatory Cognitions

Within-person fluctuations, or short-term changes, in the outcome variables were modeled using positive and negative consequences as Level 1 predictors. Across the 10 weeks, individuals planned to drink more and had higher Pos-ICOD scores following weeks in which they reported having experienced a greater number of positive consequences (γ20). Although many students experienced negative consequences (as noted previously), weekly variations in experienced negative consequences were not significantly associated with fluctuations in plans to drink or Neg-ICOD ratings for the following week (γ10). The pattern of within-person associations did not differ between males and females in this sample, as indicated by non-significant interactions between experienced consequences and gender (not shown).1

Discussion

Between-Person Differences

Subjectively rewarding consequences of alcohol use, such as having fun and feeling good, are important to college students and therefore may affect their drinking patterns. Between-person analyses allow us to investigate the differences between individuals on average across the period of 10 weeks. Differences between individuals on gender and average number of positive and negative alcohol consequences experienced were associated with anticipatory cognitions regarding alcohol. Individuals who experienced more positive and negative consequences of alcohol had anticipatory cognitions that were supportive of future use, which is consistent with the overall importance of positive consequences in reinforcing alcohol behavior (Park, 2004; Park & Grant, 2005). Men planned to drink more and reported that negative alcohol consequences were less important to them than women. These findings are expected given that men evidence heavier alcohol use than women (Johnston et al., 2005).

Within-Person Fluctuations

The Level 1, or within-person, component of multi-level models enables researchers to assess how the experiences of individuals affect their own fluctuations using data reported on a short-term basis. In this study, we investigated whether college students planned to drink more in weeks after they had experienced more positive and negative consequences of alcohol use, compared to weeks that had experienced fewer consequences. Similarly, we examined whether the importance of experiencing positive consequences and avoiding negative consequences rose and fell in tandem with the previous week’s experienced consequences. The results suggested that experiencing more positive consequences appeared to reinforce continued use, as evidenced by short-term changes in anticipatory cognitions surrounding alcohol use; conversely, experiencing fewer positive consequences predicted planning to drink less and subjectively evaluating positive consequences as less important for the subsequent week. For instance, if people have relatively more fun than usual, that is particularly reinforcing; if they have less fun than usual, they tend to plan to drink less during the subsequent week. Experiencing more or fewer negative consequences than usual in the past week was not associated with plans to drink or the subjective importance of experiencing positive and negative consequences the following week.

Between- and within-person effects provide different types of information for intervention. For example, negative consequences were associated with plans to drink more on average (between-person), but experiencing more negative consequences than usual during the previous week was not associated with planning to drink more the following week (within-person). Positive consequences, however, were associated with plans to drink on average (between-persons) and on a weekly basis (within-person). Therefore, brief interventions that seek to change the ways individuals plan for and think about their alcohol use experiences would likely be more successful by acknowledging the positive effects of use, rather than the negative consequences.

The Development of Drinking

The current findings suggest that individuals who drank most problematically, evidenced by experiencing the most negative consequences of use across weeks (Level 2 effect), also reported that the negative consequences of alcohol use were less important to avoid compared to individuals who experienced fewer negative consequences. Individuals who experienced more positive consequences also reported that negative consequences were less important and positive consequences were more important, compared to individuals who experienced fewer consequences. Since positive expectancies are most strongly associated with alcohol use behavior (Goldberg, Halpern-Felsher, & Millstein, 2002; Leigh & Stacy, 2004; Park & Grant, 2005), this finding has important public health implications. Furthermore, these associations may be increasingly reinforced with time, evidenced by the short-term increases in reported importance of future positive consequences after experiencing rewarding alcohol effects during the previous seven days (Level 1 effect). Experiencing positive consequences is powerful, and the desire to attain them in the future may cause drinking patterns to escalate. These results inform models, such as social learning theory (Bandura, 1977) and the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 2000), that focus on expectations of a behavior’s costs and benefits. Specifically, positive consequences of alcohol use are more prevalent than negative consequences and therefore more likely to affect future behavior motivation (see Leigh, 1989).

Intervention Challenges

Intervening in the midst of ongoing alcohol use involves encountering existing expectations and values regarding drinking (Cooper et al., 1995). Developmental scientists are well aware of the ongoing nature of development and change across the life span (e.g., Baltes, 1987; Elder, 1998). Regardless of the timing of an intervention, individuals already have behaviors and predispositions that may affect how they respond. For instance, even children and early adolescents who have yet to initiate alcohol use have beliefs and attitudes about drinking (Johnson & Johnson, 1996; Leigh, 1989; Leigh & Stacy, 2004; Miller, Smith, & Goldman, 1990) that may affect how they respond to intervention programs. However, investigating the proximal processes that operate in the midst of ongoing development may provide interesting and informative results for intervention, particularly because they may be more susceptible to intervention changes than would more cumulative and distal influences. For college students, in particular, the importance attached to achieving positive consequences (Maggs, 1997), which are aspects of motivation (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002), are likely among the most important proximal processes influencing use. Therefore, interventionists must be aware of the ways these positive beliefs may alter plans, behaviors, and responses to intervention programs targeting alcohol use.

Students are likely aware of both positive and negative alcohol consequences, and choose to drink in order to experience these effects based on what is rewarding to them both physiologically and socially (e.g, Furby & Beyth-Marom, 1992; Goldman et al., 1999; Spear, 2000). Especially because many intervention programs that are designed to reduce alcohol use attempt to change anticipatory cognitions (e.g., Borsari & Carey, 2000), the fact that these cognitions respond so quickly to experiences adds further complexity. Innovative intervention approaches that acknowledge positive consequences experienced from alcohol use (e.g., fun) and provide safer alternatives for experiencing them (e.g., alcohol-free programs; see Morritz, Seehafer, & Maatz-Majestic, 1993) are needed. Darkes and Goldman (1993) suggested using expectancy challenge procedures to encourage reflection about the veracity of alcohol expectancies. Realizing how expectations affect behavior (even in the absence of alcohol’s physiological effects) may decrease the reinforcing value of alcohol (Goldman, 1999), although tests of this approach have yielded mixed results (e.g., Corbin, McNair, & Carter, 2001; Wiers & Kummeling, 2004).

Limitations

Three limitations regarding the timing of the current study are worthy of mention. First, the models presented here focus on anticipatory cognitions at the weekly level. However, within a given week, there is also variation in alcohol use across days and types of days (e.g., weekday vs. weekend). Finer-grained analyses linking experiences with subsequent plans and expectancies should be conducted with data collected daily or across shorter intervals, using for example experience sampling methodologies (e.g., Muraven, Collins, Morsheimer, Shiffman, & Paty, 2005). Second, we focused exclusively on the impact of proximal factors on anticipatory cognitions regarding alcohol use. However, more distal factors are likely also important, including family background, previous alcohol consumption, and peer alcohol norms (e.g., Hawkins et al., 1992). Our focus on proximal factors represents an effort to explore the fluctuations in expectations regarding alcohol in order to understand more fully potential mediational processes. As the short-term impacts of anticipatory alcohol cognitions are more clearly articulated, intervention science will be better equipped to respond to the challenges of intervening in the midst of these processes. Third, the current sample is relatively homogenous in terms of ethnicity, age, and residence (living on campus at a single university.) Therefore, generalizing results to other populations and age ranges may not be appropriate.

Future Directions

Future research should address the limitation of data collection timing highlighted above. For example, measurement burst designs that utilize daily data collection techniques will enable researchers to deepen our understanding of what influences alcohol use behavior among adolescents and emerging adults, both students and those in other social roles, on an event level. Measuring multiple events known to be associated with increases in alcohol use (e.g., Halloween, St. Patrick’s Day, sports tailgating; Greenbaum et al., 2005) will allow for within-person prediction of spikes in plans to drink. Such heavy drinking episodes are particularly risky for acute consequences that may have lifelong implications. Moreover, the inclusion of daily covariates will enable researchers to answer interesting questions about associations among behaviors and cognitions. For example, event-level associations between sexual behavior and alcohol use among college students are a matter of campus and public health concern (e.g., Cooper, 2002). Identifying the specific situational and interpersonal determinants of risky sexual behaviors in the context of heavy alcohol use is of public health importance (e.g., Cooper, 2002). Innovative data collection strategies could be used to describe more fully the manner in which such behaviors co-vary over time.

In addition, whether and how strongly students subjectively value specific alcohol-related consequences will need to be better understood to inform intervention efforts. Specific consequences vary in their magnitude, such that one severe consequence (e.g., receiving a citation for drinking and driving) may over-shadow several more minor consequences (e.g., having a headache) in terms of the effects on behavior. In addition, positive effects may override negative effects in motivating behavior (Park & Grant, 2005) and consequences labeled as positive or negative by researchers (e.g., hangovers) may, in fact, not be subjectively evaluated as such by college students themselves (Leigh, 1989; Perkins, 2002). Future investigations may more fully capture the effects of alcohol consequences by focusing on which specific positive and negative consequences are most important to adolescents, and how these associations change over time. Questions regarding perceptions of the consequences of drinking, and how these perceptions change with experience, are important avenues for adolescent research.

Acknowledgements

Support for this research was provided by grants to J. Maggs from NIAAA (AA 013763) and the Alcoholic Beverage Medical Research Foundation and by a training fellowship from NIDA awarded to M. Patrick (DA 017629; M. Greenberg, PI).

Footnotes

1

The expectancy literature (e.g., Wigfield & Eccles, 2000) has suggested that experiencing consequences may particularly affect plans to drink if the consequences are highly valued. Therefore, we also tested whether experiencing a positive consequence on a given week interacted with a person’s mean level of valuing positive consequences in the prediction of drinking the following week. The same interaction was tested for negative consequences and expectancies. These interactions were non-significant.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational behavior and human decision processes. Special issue: Theories of cognitive self-regulation. 1991;50:179–211. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ajzen I, Fishbein M. The prediction of behavior from attitudinal and normative variables. In: Higgins ET, Kruglanski AW, editors. Motivational science: Social and personality perspectives. Key readings in social psychology. New York: Psychology Press; 2000. pp. 177–190. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baer JS. Student factors: Understanding individual variation in college drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002 Supplement No. 14:40–53. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baer JS, Kivlihan DR, Blume AW, McKnight P, Marlatt GA. Brief intervention for heavy-drinking college students: 4-year follow-up and natural history. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91:1310–1316. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.8.1310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baltes PB. Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology. 1987;23:611–626. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bandura A. Social learning theory. Oxford: Prentice Hall; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  7. Blume AW, Schmaling KB, Marlatt GA. Recent drinking consequences, motivation to change, and changes in alcohol consumption over a three month period. Addictive Behaviors. 2006;31:331–338. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Borsari B, Carey KB. Effects of a brief motivational intervention with college student drinkers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:728–733. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Botvin GJ, Griffin KW. Life Skills Training: Empirical findings and future directions. The Journal of Primary Prevention. 2004;25:221–232. [Google Scholar]
  10. Catalano RF, Hawkins JD, Berglund L, Pollard JA, Arthur MW. Prevention science and positive youth development: Competitive or cooperative frameworks? Journal of Adolescent Health. 2002;31:230–239. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00496-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Clingempeel WG, Henggeler SW. Randomized clinical trials, developmental theory, and antisocial youth: Guidelines for research. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14:695–711. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402004030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cooper ML. Alcohol use and risky sexual behavior among college students and youth: Evaluating the evidence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002 Supplement No. 14:101–117. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P. Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:990–1005. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Corbin WR, McNair LD, Carter JA. Evaluation of a treatment-appropriate cognitive intervention for challenging alcohol outcome expectancies. Addictive Behaviors. 2001;26:475–488. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(00)00138-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Darkes J, Goldman MS. Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: Experimental evidence for a mediational process. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:344–353. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.2.344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Goldman MS, Smith GT. Advancing the expectancy concept via the interplay between theory and research. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2002;26:926–935. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dunn ME, Goldman MS. Age and drinking-related differences in the memory organization of alcohol expectancies in 3rd-, 6th-, 9th-, and 12th-grade children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1998;66:579–585. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.66.3.579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Eccles JS, Wigfield A. Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:109–132. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Elder GH., Jr The life course as developmental theory. Child Development. 1998;69:1–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Furby L, Beyth-Marom R. Risk-taking in adolescence: A decision-making perspective. Developmental Review. 1992;12:1–44. [Google Scholar]
  21. Glindemann KE, Geller ES, Ludwig TD. Behavioral intentions and blood alcohol concentration: A relationship for prevention intervention. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 1996;41:120–134. [Google Scholar]
  22. Goldberg JH, Halpern-Felsher BL, Millstein SG. Beyond invulnerability: The importance of benefits in adolescents’ decision to drink alcohol. Health Psychology. 2002;21:477–484. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.21.5.477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Goldman MS. The alcohol expectancy concept: Applications to assessment, prevention, and treatment of alcohol abuse. Applied & Preventive Psychology. 1994;3:131–144. [Google Scholar]
  24. Goldman MS. Risk for substance abuse: Memory as a common etiological pathway. Psychological Science. 1999;10:196–198. [Google Scholar]
  25. Goldman MS, Brown SA, Christiansen BA. Expectancy theory: Thinking about drinking. In: Blane HT, Leonard KE, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. New York: Guildford; 1987. pp. 181–226. [Google Scholar]
  26. Goldman MS, Darkes J, Del Boca FK. Expectancy mediation of biopsychosocial risk for alcohol use and alcoholism. In: Kirsch I, editor. How expectancies shape experience. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999. pp. 233–262. [Google Scholar]
  27. Goldman MS, Del Boca FK, Darkes J. Alcohol expectancy theory: The application of cognitive neuroscience. In: Blane HT, Leonard KE, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 203–246. [Google Scholar]
  28. Greenbaum PE, Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Wang CP, Goldman MS. Variation in the drinking trajectories of freshman college students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:229–238. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Graham JW, Tatterson JW, Roberts MM, Johnston SE. Preventing alcohol-related harm in college students: Alcohol-related harm prevention program effects on hypothesized mediating variables. Health Education Research. 2004;19:71–84. doi: 10.1093/her/cyg006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Miller JY. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:64–105. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hays R. An integrated value-expectancy theory of alcohol and other drug use. British Journal of Addiction. 1985;80:379–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1985.tb03009.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hingson R, Heeren T, Winter M, Wechsler H. Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24: Changes from 1998 to 2001. Annual Review of Public Health. 2005;26:259–279. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jensen PS, Weersing R, Hoagwood KE, Goldman E. What is the evidence for evidence-based treatments? A hard look at our soft underbelly. Mental Health Services Research. 2005;7:53–74. doi: 10.1007/s11020-005-1965-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Johnson PB, Johnson HL. Children's beliefs about the social consequences of drinking and refusing to drink alcohol. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 1996;41:34–43. [Google Scholar]
  35. Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2004. Volume II: College students and adults ages 19–45. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2005. NIH Publication No. 05-5728. [Google Scholar]
  36. Komro KA, Perry CL, Williams CL, Stigler MH, Farbakhsh K, Veblen-Mortenson S. How did Project Northland reduce alcohol use among adolescents? Analysis of mediating variables. Health Education Research. 2001;16:59–70. doi: 10.1093/her/16.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kreft IGG, de Leeuw J, Aiken LS. The effect of different forms of centering in hierarchical linear models. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1995;30:1–21. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3001_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lee CM, Maggs JL, Rankin LA. Spring Break trips as a risk factor for heavy alcohol use among first-year college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67:911–916. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Leigh BC. In search of the seven dwarves: Issues of measurement and meaning in alcohol expectancy research. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1989;105:361–373. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Leigh BC, Stacy AW. Alcohol expectancies and drinking in different age groups. Addiction. 2004;99:215–227. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00641.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Maggs JL. Adolescent alcohol use as a goal-directed behavior. British Columbia: University of Victoria; 1993. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  42. Maggs JL. Alcohol use and binge drinking as goal-directed action during the transition to postsecondary education. In: Schulenberg J, Maggs JL, Hurrelmann K, editors. Health risks and developmental transitions during adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1997. pp. 345–371. [Google Scholar]
  43. Maggs JL, Vesterdal WJ, Galambos NL. The Importance of Consequences of Drinking (ICOD) measure: Motivations for alcohol use in college students. 2004 Manuscript in review. [Google Scholar]
  44. Maggs JL, Vesterdal WJ, Lee CM, Korn M. Niche selection among emerging adults at university: Do personal characteristics predict residence hall and activities? 2006 Manuscript in review. [Google Scholar]
  45. Mallett KA, Lee CM, Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Turrisi R. Do we learn from our mistakes? An examination of the impact of negative alcohol-related consequences on college students' drinking patterns and perceptions. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67:269–276. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Morritz T, Seehafer RW, Maatz-Majestic E. A student competition to develop and innovative alcohol education strategy. Journal of American College Health. 1993;41:283–286. doi: 10.1080/07448481.1993.9936346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. McCarthy DM, Pedersen SL, Leuty ME. Negative consequences and cognitions about drinking and driving. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2005;66:567–570. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Miller PM, Smith GT, Goldman MS. Emergence of alcohol expectancies in childhood: A possible critical period. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1990;51:343–349. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1990.51.343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Muraven M, Collins RL, Morsheimer ET, Shiffman S, Paty JA. The morning after: Limit violations and the self-regulation of alcohol consumption. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19:253–262. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.3.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Murphy JG, Duchnick JJ, Vuchinich RE, Davison JW, Karg RS, Olson AM, et al. Relative efficacy of a brief motivational intervention for college student drinkers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2001;15:373–379. doi: 10.1037//0893-164x.15.4.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Lewis MA. Targeting misperceptions of descriptive drinking norms: Efficacy of a computer-delivered personalized normative feedback intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:434–447. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. O'Malley PM, Johnston LD. Epidemiology of alcohol and other drug use among American college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002 Supplement No. 14:23–39. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Park CL. Positive and negative consequences of alcohol consumption in college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2004;29:311–321. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Park CL, Grant C. Determinants of positive and negative consequences of alcohol consumption in college students: Alcohol use, gender, and psychological characteristics. Addictive Behaviors. 2005;30:755–765. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Parsons JT, Siegel AW, Cousins JH. Late adolescent risk-taking: Effects of perceived benefits and perceived risks on behavioral intentions and behavioral change. Journal of Adolescence. 1997;20:381–392. doi: 10.1006/jado.1997.0094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Perkins HW. Surveying the damage: A review of research on consequences of alcohol misuse in college populations. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002 Supplement No. 14:91–100. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Petraitis J, Flay BR, Miller TQ. Reviewing theories of adolescent substance use: Organizing pieces of the puzzle. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117:67–86. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rankin LA, Maggs JL. First-year college student affect and alcohol use: Paradoxical within- and between-person associations. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. (in press) [Google Scholar]
  59. Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical Linear Models (Second Edition) Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  60. Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002 Supplement No. 14:54–70. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Singer JD, Willett JB. Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  62. Snijders T, Bosker R. Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  63. Spear L. Modeling adolescent development and alcohol use in animals. Alcohol Research and Health. 2000;24:115–123. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wechsler H, Nelson TF, Lee JE, Seibring M, Lewis C, Keeling RP. Perception and reality: A national evaluation of social norms marketing interventions to reduce college students' heavy alcohol use. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2003;64:484–494. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Weissberg RP, Kumpfer KL, Seligman MEP. Prevention that works for children and youth: An introduction. American Psychologist. 2003;58:425–432. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.6-7.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wier RW, Kummeling RHC. An experimental test of an alcohol expectancy challenge in mixed gender groups of young heavy drinkers. Addictive Behaviors. 2004;29:215–220. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(03)00081-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wigfield A, Eccles JS. Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2000;25:68–81. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Wilsnack SC, Wilsnack RW. International gender and alcohol research: Recent findings and future directions. Alcohol Research & Health. 2002;26:245–250. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES