Abstract
We previously reported that auditory nerve projections from the cochlear spiral ganglion (SG) to the cochlear nucleus (CN) exhibit clear cochleotopic organization in adult cats deafened as neonates before hearing onset. However, the topographic specificity of these CN projections in deafened animals is proportionately broader than normal (less precise relative to the CN frequency gradient). This study examined SG-to-CN projections in adult cats that were deafened as neonates and received a unilateral cochlear implant at ∼7 weeks of age. Following several months of electrical stimulation, SG projections from the stimulated cochleae were compared to projections from contralateral, non-implanted ears. The fundamental organization of SG projections into frequency band laminae was clearly evident, and discrete projections were always observed following double SG injections in deafened cochleae, despite severe auditory deprivation and/or broad electrical activation of the SG. However, when normalized for the smaller CN size after deafness, AVCN, PVCN, and DCN projections on the stimulated side were broader by 32%, 34%, and 53%, respectively, than projections in normal animals (although absolute projection widths were comparable to normal). Further, there was no significant difference between projections from stimulated and contralateral non-implanted cochleae. These findings suggest that early normal auditory experience may be essential for normal development and/or maintenance of the topographic precision of SG-to-CN projections. After early deafness, the CN is smaller than normal, the topographic distribution of these neural projections that underlie frequency resolution in the central auditory system is proportionately broader, and projections from adjacent SG sectors are more overlapping. Several months of stimulation by a cochlear implant (beginning at ∼7 weeks of age) did not lessen or exacerbate these degenerative changes observed in adulthood. One clinical implication of these findings is that congenitally deaf cochlear implant recipients may have central auditory system alterations that limit their ability to achieve spectral selectivity equivalent to post-lingually deafened subjects.
Keywords: auditory deprivation, cochlear spiral ganglion, cochlear prosthesis, congenital deafness, development, primary afferents, tonotopic organization, topographic maps
Introduction
In most sensory systems, the neighboring relationships of receptors within the sensory epithelium are maintained in their projections into the central nervous system. Such topographic maps comprise one of the fundamental organizing features of the central nervous system, and over the past few decades, there has been a major focus in developmental neurobiology on examining changes in the specificity of these maps during development and in defining the specific roles of afferent input and molecular cues in the formation and maintenance of precise topographic connections (see Rubel and Cramer 2002 for review). In the visual system, earlier studies of development emphasized that competition driven by activity is essential for refinement of initial broad or diffuse neuronal projections into more precise adult patterns (e.g., Guillery and Stelzner 1970; Shatz 1990; Goodman and Shatz 1993). Spontaneous waves of correlated rhythmic neural activity in retinal ganglion cells occur for some time before the onset of vision and are thought to be essential for the normal development of precise topographic maps at all levels of the visual system (Cang et al. 2005; Constantine-Paton et al. 1990; Maffei and Galli-Resta 1990; Shatz 1990, 1996; Goodman and Shatz 1993; Feller et al. 1996; McLaughlin et al. 2003b; O’Leary and McLaughlin 2005). Specific spatiotemporal patterns of spontaneous and visual input guide formation of precise synaptic connections (Zhou et al. 2003; Chiu and Weliky 2003), and progressive refinement of tectal receptive fields requires activity-dependent matching of the topography and strength of excitatory and inhibitory connections (Tao and Poo 2005).
In contrast, other studies have shown that the retinotectal projections are relatively precise from the time retinal fibers first reach the tectum (e.g., Chalupa and Snider 1998) and have emphasized the role of molecular cues in guiding the formation of precise projections (Cheng et al. 1995; Hansen et al. 2004; O’Leary and Wilkinson 1999; McLaughlin et al. 2003a; O’Leary et al. 1999; McLaughlin and O’Leary 2005; O’Leary and McLaughlin 2005). Thus, even for the retinotectal map, which is probably the best-studied of all topographic maps, the exact roles of and interactions between molecular mechanisms and neural activity in the formation and maintenance of spatially precise connections are still being debated.
The auditory nerve provides a particularly interesting model for examining molecular and activity-dependent mechanisms underlying the formation of topographically precise projections. Like the visual system, the auditory system is very immature at birth in many mammals but precisely organized in adults. A basic feature of its organization is tonotopy, the orderly arrangement of best frequencies of the cochlear hair cells that is preserved within the spiral ganglion and in its projections to the cochlear nucleus. In the normal adult cat, auditory nerve fibers send collaterals to all three subdivisions of the cochlear nucleus (CN), forming separate tonotopic maps and distinct synaptic connections in the anteroventral (AVCN), posteroventral (PVCN), and dorsal cochlear nuclei (DCN).
Functionally, newborn kittens are deaf due to the immaturity of the auditory system. By a few days postnatal (∼P3), the auditory nerve begins to exhibit spontaneous activity, but discharge rates and acoustic sensitivity remain very low until about P10–P14 (see Walsh and Romand 1992 for review; Ehret and Romand 1981; Dolan et al. 1985). Previous studies of central auditory system development have reported a clear tonotopic gradient as soon as stimuli elicit a response (Ryan and Woolf 1988; Sanes et al. 1989; Webster and Martin 1991; Friauf 1992; Friauf and Kandler 1993). Our earlier anatomical studies examined the late gestational and perinatal development of projections from the cochlea to the CN by making focal injections of Neurobiotin™ (NB) into the spiral ganglion (SG) to study the CN projections from restricted sectors of the SG representing a narrow range of frequencies. These CN projections form discrete “frequency band laminae” within the AVCN, PVCN, small cell cap, and DCN, which exhibit clear cochleotopic organization in animals studied as young 5 days prenatal after caesarian section (Leake et al. 2002). However, quantitative analyses indicated that when normalized for CN size, the topographic restriction of projections in the perinatal group was less precise than in older kittens or adults. Projections to the AVCN were proportionately >50% broader, and projections to both the PVCN and DCN were >30% broader in neonates. Thus, significant refinement in the CN projections occurs during the first week postnatal (Leake et al. 2002), after which the projections are proportionate to those in adults (Snyder and Leake 1997).
In a recent study, recordings made directly from cochlear spiral ganglion cells in neonatal kittens during this pre-hearing period demonstrated distinctive bursting spontaneous discharge patterns (Jones et al. 2007). This activity is similar to the rhythmic spontaneous activity in retinal ganglion cells, shown to be essential in central refinement (O’Leary and McLaughlin 2005; Ruthazer and Cline 2004). Moreover, it occurs at precisely the time when the CN projections are becoming topographically restricted and thus may play a similar role. Soon after, immature bursting and rhythmic responses to high-intensity acoustic stimuli emerge in the auditory nerve (Fitzakerley et al. 1995, 1998) just before the emergence of adult-like frequency selectivity and sensitivity.
Our recent study examined the consequences of early profound hearing loss upon the SG-to-CN projections and showed that their precision is significantly reduced in animals that are deafened as neonates (Leake et al. 2006). The present study extends this work by examining the consequences of early profound hearing loss followed by unilateral electrical stimulation from a cochlear implant, upon the cochleotopic organization of the SG projections. Our hypothesis was that normal cochlear input (spontaneous and driven activity in the auditory nerve) is required for the full topographic refinement of the primary afferent inputs to the central auditory system. A corollary of this hypothesis is that when the selectivity of SG-to-CN projections is reduced due deafness occurring early in life, restoration of electrical activity in the auditory nerve via a cochlear implant may be effective in ameliorating or preventing these alterations. On the other hand, previous studies (Kitzes et al. 1995; Kitzes 1996; Moore and Kitzes 1985; Moore and Kowalchuk 1988) have reported that neonatal removal of one cochlea results in substantial reorganization of the central auditory projections, with projections from the intact CN more than doubling in size in their distribution to the auditory midbrain and forming highly ectopic projections to the brainstem superior olivary complex and medial nucleus of the trapezoid body. Thus, an alternative hypothesis is that electrical stimulation of one cochlea after neonatal deafening might exacerbate the reduced selectivity of CN projections and elicit substantially greater expansion or reorganization of auditory nerve projections in the CN ipsilateral to the implant. To evaluate these hypotheses, SG injections were made in adult cats that had been deafened by ototoxic drug administration beginning immediately after birth and subsequently stimulated unilaterally with a two-channel cochlear implant. Our objectives were to determine whether the normal topographic precision of the cochleotopic organization of the SG projections to the CN that underlies frequency selectivity in the normal auditory system can be restored and maintained into adulthood by application of a cochlear prosthesis after severe auditory deprivation early in life or alternatively if direct electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve would result in substantial reorganization of CN frequency band laminae in neonatally deafened animals.
Methods
Neonatal deafening, cochlear implantation, and chronic electrical stimulation
All procedures involving animals in this study were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of California, San Francisco and conformed to all NIH guidelines. Nine experimentally deafened animals were included in this study, and all were bred in a closed colony maintained at the University of California San Francisco. In addition, four normal adult cats were studied as controls.
As mentioned previously, kittens are unresponsive to sound over the first few days of life (Walsh and Romand 1992; Ehret and Romand 1981). The nine deafened animals included in this study received daily injections of the ototoxic aminoglycoside antibiotic, neomycin sulfate, (60 mg/kg SQ, S.I.D.) beginning the day after birth and continuing for 18–24 days during the period when nearly adult-like hearing sensitivity normally develops. Profound hearing loss was confirmed by auditory brainstem response (ABR) testing as described previously (Leake et al. 1999, 2006). Briefly, at P17, animals were tranquilized with inhaled isoflurane (3% via mask for induction; 1–2% for maintenance) and placed in a double-walled sound-attenuating chamber. A hollow tube was inserted into the external auditory meatus, and auditory stimuli were delivered through a headphone speaker (STAX-54) enclosed in a small chamber connected to the tube in the ear canal. The sound delivery system was calibrated with a sound level meter (Bruel and Kjaer 2209). Thresholds were determined for click-evoked auditory brainstem responses, averaging responses for at least 500 trials. If residual hearing was noted, it was always at significantly elevated thresholds compared to normal, and neomycin injections were continued in 2–3 day increments until no response was observed at the maximum intensity (>105 dB). Profound hearing losses induced by this method result in virtually total degeneration of hair cells throughout the cochlea (Leake et al. 1997). Table 1 presents the individual histories of these animals. The first group of four animals averaged 9 weeks of age (range, 8–11 weeks) and were studied to provide control data at the approximate age at which the second group of five deafened animals received unilateral cochlear implants. Stimulation histories for the older group also are shown. In addition, four of these implanted animals received daily injections of the selegiline (−)-desmethyldeprenyl (100 mg/kg SQ) concomitant with the administration of the ototoxic drug and continuing until the day of implantation in an effort to maintain better survival of the SG neurons because this drug has been shown to reduce neuronal apoptosis by a mechanism that is relevant to the SG neurons (Tatton et al. 1999). The data for the cochleae and CN projections on the non-implanted side of the older deafened group and adult control data have been reported previously (Leake et al. 2006).
TABLE 1.
Cat number | Neomycin number of days | Age at initial stimulation (weeks) | Stimulus intensity channel (μA) | Stimulation period (weeks) | Stimulus frequency | Age at study (weeks) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neonatally deafened group studied at ∼9 weeks of age | ||||||
118 | 24 | – | – | – | – | 8 |
119 | 24 | – | – | – | – | 8 |
666 | 18 | – | – | – | – | 10.5 |
711 | 19 | – | – | – | – | 11 |
Mean | 9.4 | |||||
Neonatally deafened group after unilateral electrical stimulation | ||||||
K154 | 21 | 6 | 1, 2: 79–200 | 28 | Speech Proc. | 34 |
3, 4: 282–500 | ||||||
K158 | 19 | 7 | 1, 2: 89–251 | 34 | 325 pps/60 Hz | 41 |
3, 4: 178–355 | ||||||
K160 | 19 | 6 | 1, 2: 89–398 | 28 | 325 pps/60 Hz | 34 |
3, 4: 112–316 | ||||||
K163 | 19 | 6 | 1, 2: 112–178 | 14 | 325 pps/60 Hz | 20 |
3, 4: 100–224 | ||||||
K169 | 19 | 8 | 1, 2: 71–447 | 25 | 300 pps/30 Hz | 33 |
3, 4: 126–251 | ||||||
Mean | 32.4 |
Individual data on neomycin administration and age at study are shown for the groups of animals studied at 9 weeks and 8 months of age. In addition, for the older group that received a unilateral cochlear implant, the age at implantation, range of peak stimulus intensities applied, duration of stimulation periods, and stimulus frequency are shown. One subject (K154) received stimulation via an analogue speech processor (see text).
A four-wire scala tympani electrode in a molded silicone rubber carrier was surgically implanted in the left cochlea as previously described (Leake et al. 1999, 2007), and electrical stimulation was initiated at about 7 weeks postnatal (range, 6–8 weeks). All animals received electrical stimulation with signals considered to be “temporally challenging” to the central auditory system (see Leake and Rebscher 2004; Vollmer et al. 1999). Stimulation periods were 4 h/day, 5 days/week for at least 6 months, except in one subject (K163) that was studied earlier due to device failure (Table 1). Concurrent stimulation was delivered on two bipolar channels: an apical pair of electrodes positioned about 11–12 mm from the base (∼4–5 kHz) and a basal pair centered at ∼5–6 mm from the base (∼12–15 kHz). Electrodes comprising each pair were separated by 1 mm. In four animals, the maximum electrical stimulation intensity was set at 2 dB above the electrically evoked auditory brainstem response (EABR) threshold using 200 μs/phase biphasic pulses, as determined for each channel. EABR thresholds were determined regularly throughout stimulation periods and stimulators adjusted to maintain appropriate stimulation levels relative to EABR thresholds. Electrical stimuli were continuous trains of charge-balanced biphasic pulses delivered at 300 or 325 pps and 100% sinusoidally amplitude modulated at 30 and 60 Hz. One subject (K154) received stimulation from ambient environmental sounds through a modified human analogue cochlear implant speech processor with stimulation set at a maximum stimulus amplitude of 6 dB above EABR threshold and processor outputs bandpass filtered at 850–1,450 Hz for the basal channel and at 250–850 Hz for the apical channel as described previously (Leake et al. 2000).
Electrophysiological recording
At the completion of chronic stimulation periods, animals were studied in acute electrophysiological experiments to record neural responses from the auditory midbrain or inferior colliculus (IC). Animals were first tranquilized with isoflurane. An intravenous catheter was inserted into the cephalic vein and a surgical level of anesthesia induced and maintained by infusion of sodium pentobarbital (20–40 mg/kg IV, to effect). Respiratory rate, heart rate, and body temperature were monitored throughout all procedures, and body temperature was maintained using a warm water recirculating blanket. The electrophysiological recording methods have been described in detail in several previous publications (Snyder et al. 1990, 2000; Leake et al. 2000; Leake and Rebscher 2004; Vollmer et al. 1999, 2005). Briefly, tungsten microelectrodes were used to record responses of multi-neuronal clusters and single neurons in several penetrations through the IC that were made along a standardized trajectory orthogonal to the tonotopic gradient of the IC. In each penetration, response thresholds to biphasic electrical pulses (0.2 ms/phase) were determined at intervals of 100 μm in response to electrical activation by each of the two bipolar cochlear implant channels used for chronic stimulation. To estimate thresholds, the intensities just sufficient to activate the neuron(s) were determined using audio-visual criteria. Thresholds were plotted as a function of depth along the tonotopic gradient of the IC to obtain spatial tuning curves (STC). STC widths were measured in each cat at the maximum electrical pulse intensity delivered to each stimulating channel during the chronic stimulation period immediately before the terminal, acute experiment.
Neurobiotin™ injections
After completion of the IC recording experiment, the auditory bulla was surgically exposed and opened, and the round window membrane was excised for direct visualization of Rosenthal’s canal in the hook region and lower basal turn. In implanted cochleae, the mucosa and connective tissue over the Dacron cuff securing the electrode at the round window were dissected, and the electrode was carefully withdrawn. The fibrous and/or bony tissue encapsulating the electrode within the scala tympani was carefully dissected to access Rosenthal’s canal. For each injection, a small opening through the bone of the modiolus overlying Rosenthal’s canal was created using the tip of a 30-gauge needle as a curette.
The neuronal tracer Neurobiotin™ (from Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA) was injected using thick-walled glass micropipettes with tips broken to a diameter of ∼10–30 μm. The micropipettes were filled with a solution of 5% NB dissolved in distilled water. A small amount of the vital dye Trypan blue (<0.5%) was added to the NB solution so that injections could be confirmed visually. The micropipette was mounted on a 1-μl microsyringe (Unimetrics) or attached to a Picospritzer II™ (General Valve) picoliter injector, positioned over the opening in Rosenthal’s canal and inserted into the SG using a micromanipulator. A small amount of NB (∼0.5 μl) was injected either manually or with the Picospritzer II™. The tip of the pipette was left in place for about 10 min to allow the tracer to diffuse into the ganglion. In most animals, injections were made at two locations in each cochlea. The locations of injections were recorded, and the round window was sealed with a small disk of plastic wrap (Saran™ wrap, Dow Corning). The incisions were sutured closed, and animals were maintained lightly anesthetized for post-injection periods of 7–12 h to allow sufficient time for the tracer to be transported to the CN and to completely fill the central axons of the SG neurons.
Table 2 presents data for individual subjects in the first experimental group of four neonatally deafened animals studied at 9 weeks of age and in the second experimental group of five neonatally deafened animals that received a cochlear implant and intracochlear electrical stimulation. The table indicates the number of successful SG Neurobiotin™ injections made in each cochlea, the mean SG density (averaged for the entire cochlea), and CN volumes. Control data were obtained from four normal adult cats (eight CN from two male and two female cats). Note that some of the data for the normal cats and non-stimulated cochleae and CN of the older deafened group were presented previously (Leake et al. 2006). However, in the prior study, CN size was estimated from cross-sectional areas of representative sections, whereas in this study, we report CN volumes and projection data are normalized to cross-sectional areas throughout the CN.
TABLE 2.
Cat number | Age at study (weeks) | Number of SG injections: left, right | SG density (area fraction, percent of normal): left, right | CN volume (mm3): left, right |
---|---|---|---|---|
Neonatally deafened group studied at ∼9 weeks of age | ||||
118 | 8 | 1, 1 | – | 9.04,– |
119 | 8 | 2, 1 | – | 9.02, 9.39 |
666 | 10 | 2, 2 | – | 10.31, 10.15 |
711 | 11 | –, 1 | – | –, 10.19 |
Neonatally deafened group after unilateral electrical stimulation (left cochlea) | ||||
K154 | 34 | 1, 1 | 35.7, 40.1 | 14.70, 16.14 |
K158 | 41 | 2, 2 | 55.9, 37.1 | 13.81, 14.13 |
K160 | 34 | 1, 2 | 21.7, 37.3 | 12.46, 13.81 |
K163 | 20 | 2, 2 | 91.5, 84.2 | 14.80, 15.85 |
K169 | 33 | –, 2 | 28.2, 23.7 | 16.14, 14.10 |
Normal adult controls | ||||
423 | 124 | 3, 3 | – | 22.00, 21.18 |
596 | 157 | 2, 2 | – | 22.58, 20.98 |
654 | 37 | 2, 2 | – | 20.74, 23.38 |
944 | NA | 1, 1 | – | 20.05, 18.45 |
Age at study, number of successful SG injections made in each cochlea, and CN volume are presented for individual subjects in the groups of deafened animals studied at 9 weeks and 8 months of age and for normal adult controls. In addition, individual SG density data are shown for the implanted group.
Preparation of cochlear specimens
Fixation, NB cytochemistry, embedding, and sectioning for light microscopy After the post-injection period, the cochleae were preserved by infusion of mixed aldehyde fixative (2.5% paraformaldehyde and 1.5% glutaraldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer at pH 7.4) directly into the scalae through the round and oval windows. A lethal dose of sodium pentobarbital was administered, and transcardiac perfusion was carried out (5% lactated Ringer’s solution followed by fixative: 0.5% paraformaldehyde, 2.5% glutaraldehyde, and 4% sucrose in 0.1 M phosphate buffer at pH 7.4). The brain and temporal bones were removed for histology.
The cochlea was dissected (thinning the otic capsule bone with a diamond dental burr) and post-fixed for 12 h in 1% phosphate-buffered osmium tetroxide with 1.5% potassium ferricyanide. Specimens then were partially decalcified in 0.1 M EDTA for 18 h, dehydrated, and embedded in LX™ epoxy resin. Extra-thick surface preparations were made as previously described (Leake et al. 2006). Half-coils of the cochlea containing the organ of Corti and adjacent SG were removed using razor blades and mounted in LX on glass slides (or cut into smaller pieces as necessary to orient the basilar membrane parallel to the slide). The basilar membrane was measured from base to apex along the pillar cells, and blocks containing ∼0.5 mm of the organ of Corti and associated SG were removed at ∼2-mm intervals throughout the cochlea. The samples were remounted in the radial plane, and semi-thin sections (1–2 μm) were cut and stained with toluidine blue. Additional blocks were prepared and serial sections cut in the regions where the NB injection sites were identified, and serial sections were cut to define the limits of each injection site.
Analysis of injection sites Sections through cochlear injection sites were examined in the light microscope (Zeiss, Axioskop 2), and high-resolution video images (2,584 × 1,936 pixels) for illustrations were acquired using Photoshop 5.0, a Kodak digital camera, and a PC. For calibration, an image of a micrometer scale was captured and superimposed on each image.
In our previous studies of the SG-to-CN projections in normal cats, the extent of each NB injection site was defined by the distribution of labeled afferent fibers and terminals innervating inner hair cells (e.g., Leake et al. 2002). This method cannot be used in deafened animals because the cochlear hair cells and many of the radial nerve fibers have degenerated. Previous studies also showed that due to the relative impermeability of the bone, penetration of cytochemical reagents was inadequate to demonstrate NB reaction product in the Rosenthal’s canal. Therefore, the location and extent of injections were defined by examining serial sections through each injection site for evidence of penetration of the pipette into Rosenthal’s canal and to locate damaged, pyknotic, and missing SG cell somata. The injection site locations were defined first by absolute distance in millimeter from the basal end of the organ of Corti. Next, total basilar membrane length in each cochlea was used to calculate the positions of injection sites as percent distance from base. This allowed us to calculate the represented frequencies encompassed by each injection (and resulting CN projections) using Greenwood’s frequency-position function (Greenwood 1974, 1990) with the revised constants provided by Liberman (1982) for the cat cochlea.
Spiral ganglion cell density Morphometric analysis of the SG cell somata in Rosenthal’s canal was performed to quantify neuronal survival as a function of cochlear location, using a point counting method as described previously (Leake et al. 1999; Leake et al. 2007). Briefly, at each selected location, five sections cut at intervals of >50 μm were examined at ×400 in the light microscope. A counting grid (10 mm × 10 mm; field area = 90,000 μm2 at ×400) was placed in the microscope ocular and the section positioned with the grid over Rosenthal’s canal. The area fraction of the SG cells was determined by counting the number of grid line intersections that fell over cell somata and dividing by the number of intersections over Rosenthal’s canal. This value represents the ratio of Rosenthal’s canal area occupied by the SG cells. Previous studies provided normative data for the cat SG (Leake et al. 1999), allowing data to be expressed as percent of normal. Area fraction data were pooled into 10% sectors from base to apex based upon the total basilar membrane length as determined in each surface preparation. Note that the data for the non-stimulated cochleae were reported previously (Leake et al. 2006, Fig. 2B). For overall group comparisons to assess the effects of electrical stimulation on SG cell density, the mean area fraction for each cochlear sector was calculated, and data for paired cochlear sectors were compared using a two-way ANOVA pair-wise multiple comparisons procedure (Tukey test). All statistical analyses were performed using Sigma Stat (version 2.03) software.
Preparation of cochlear nucleus sections and analysis of NB-labeled projections
Preparation of CN sections and NB cytochemistry Following the transcardiac perfusion, the brainstem was isolated (rostral midbrain to caudal medulla), rinsed in Ringer’s solution, and placed in 40% sucrose in 0.1 M phosphate buffer at pH 7.4 at 4°C until saturated. The brain was marked on the right side, frozen rapidly with dry ice, and serial coronal sections were cut at 50 μm. Sections were rinsed in 0.05 M TRIS (pH 7.6) and phosphate buffer, incubated in VECTASTAIN™ ABC reagent solution from Vector Laboratories for 12 h at 4°C and rinsed in 0.05 M TRIS buffer. The NB/ABC complex was visualized using 3,3′-diaminobenzidine tetrahydrochloride (DAB; Sigma) substrate with cobalt intensification as described previously (Leake et al. 2006). Sections were pre-incubated for 30 min in 100 ml TRIS buffer containing 20 mg of DAB, 1 mg cobalt chloride, and 1 mg nickel ammonium sulfate, transferred to fresh DAB with one drop of 3% hydrogen peroxide, and incubated for an additional 60–120 min until emerging background staining was evident in sections examined under the microscope. The sections were rinsed, mounted on gelatinized slides, dehydrated, cleared, and cover-slipped in Permount™. Tissue shrinkage was evaluated previously by direct measurements made in wet sections and repeated in the same sections after drying, clearing, and mounting. Mean shrinkage was less than 0.5% in brainstem sections processed in this manner (Leake et al. 2002).
CN cross-sectional area measurements, volume calculations, and scaling factors To evaluate the reduced size of the CN in deafened animals, measurements were made in sections selected at 10% intervals. Sections were imaged digitally and measured at a resolution of about 6.5 μm/pixel (640 × 480 = 4.18 mm × 3.13 mm) in Image J software (Rasband 2006; version 1.34 N). The perimeter of the CN was outlined using the tracing tool and the area calculated using the area function. The CN volume was calculated by multiplying the ten cross-sectional area values by the total number of sections and by section thickness (50 μm). The values for CN volume were compared among groups using an unpaired Student’s t test (Bonferroni correction).
For normalizing the measurements of labeled auditory nerve projections relative to the smaller size of the CN in deafened animals, we calculated the square root of the mean of the cross-sectional areas from the 20–80% intervals of the CN (region where the main development of the labeled projections was seen) in each subject to provide an estimate of relative size in a single dimension (re: CN projection thickness). These values were used to calculate a scaling factor: The square root of the mean of these CN areas was calculated for each deafened subject and divided by the mean of the values for the normal adult group.
Measurements of CN projection laminae The nomenclature used here for the cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and cell types of the cochlear nucleus follows that of Osen (1969, 1970) because smaller subdivisions defined in Golgi material (Brawer et al. 1974; Cant and Morest 1984; Tolbert and Morest 1982) are difficult to distinguish in our unstained sections reacted for demonstration of NB labeling. For descriptions of labeled fiber and synaptic terminal morphology, the terminology of Rouiller et al. (1986) is adopted. For quantitative analysis of projections, digital images were captured at a resolution of approximately 1 μm/pixel (640 × 480 pixels = 0.640 mm × 0.480 mm). The thickness of each lamina of labeled axons and terminals was measured using the “profile scan” analysis function of the public domain NIH Image program (developed at the U.S. National Institutes of Health; on the Internet at http://rsb.info.nih.gov/nih-image/) software, version 1.63, as described previously (Leake et al. 2006). Measurements were made in serial sections through the main development of each projection in each CN subdivision. Sections at the rostral and caudal extremes of laminae were excluded to avoid tangential measurements. Projection thickness was measured orthogonal to the plane of the individual CN projection lamina in order to estimate the distribution of labeled fibers across the frequency gradient within each subdivision. AVCN and PVCN laminae were measured in a roughly dorsal-to-ventral axis orthogonal to their long axes, and the DCN projections were measured in an axis parallel to the pial surface. Scans were executed beginning at the low frequency side of the laminae (ventral to dorsal) using a 0.05-mm by 0.4–0.8-mm window. Three scans were made in each selected image (∼90 scans in each CN). All scans for a given projection were averaged, and mean pixel density was displayed as a function of distance across the window. The threshold was set to a level at which some negative values occurred on both sides of the pixel–density peak within the window. The point at which the first negative number appeared was determined on each side of the peak, and the total pixel value for all bins between the two points was assigned a value of 100%. The distance containing 90% of the total pixel–density was calculated and defined as the projection thickness. The Student’s t test (unpaired) was used for comparisons of projections in deafened and control subjects, except when the data failed the t test assumption of normality (equal variance) in which case the Mann–Whitney rank sum test was used. The same tests were used to compare projection thickness in deafened subjects with “predicted” values (the values predicted if projections in the smaller CN of deafened subjects were proportionate to those in normal controls). Predicted values were calculated by multiplying the average projection thickness measured in each CN subdivision in controls by the scaling factor (as described above).
Digital images for photomicrograph illustrations were captured as tiff files (resolution of 2,584 × 1,936 pixels) using Axiovision AC software (v. 4.2.0.0). Images were imported into Photoshop (v. 8) or Canvas (v.9) to add labels and scale bars. In some images, background subtraction was used, and contrast and brightness were adjusted to match the images appearing within the same figure.
Results
Efficacy of chronic electrical stimulation by the cochlear implant
Electrical stimulation was delivered by the cochlear implant for periods of 25–34 weeks in four of the implanted animals. Acute recording experiments were conducted in these subjects to evaluate the efficacy of the electrical stimulation delivered by the implant over the previous months and to characterize the extent of activation elicited by the electrical stimuli within the central auditory system. Subject K163 damaged its implant and was studied after only 14 weeks of stimulation. In this animal, we were able to bypass the broken wires and reconnect to the intracochlear electrodes near the auditory bulla to conduct an electrophysiology experiment. However, EABR thresholds were found to be higher than before the wire breakage, and, therefore, the electrophysiology data collected in this animal were excluded from the present analysis.
Neural response thresholds were plotted as a function of depth along the cochleotopic gradient of the IC to obtain STC functions, as reported previously (Snyder et al. 1990, 1991; Vollmer et al. 1999; Leake et al. 2000, 2007; Leake and Rebscher 2004). In the present study, extent of activation of the IC was measured at the maximum electrical signal intensities of the amplitude modulated pulse trains (or analogue signal in K154) used for the electrical stimulation delivered to each implant channel in that particular subject. In previous studies, measured STCs for sinusoidal stimuli were typically W-shaped, and the highest threshold site between the two minimum thresholds was defined as the border between the external nucleus (ICX) and central nucleus of the IC (ICC). In the present study, STCs for pulse stimuli often did not exhibit the typical W-shaped functions and consequently did not allow for reliable distinction between the ICX and the ICC. However, these STC data do provide an estimate of the extent of excitation across the tonotopic gradient of the IC (spatial selectivity or tuning) for each channel of the implant. Table 3 presents the mean IC activation widths (average of all IC penetrations) in each subject, for the apical channel bipolar electrode pair 1, 2 and for the basal channel 3, 4. Values are expressed as percentage of the total IC depth over which responses were recorded.
TABLE 3.
Cat number | IC activation at maximum stimulus intensity (mean percent of total) | Minimum IC threshold pulses (0 dB = 1 μA) | Final stimulus intensity (0 dB = 1 μA) | Difference (dB) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1, 2 | 3, 4 | 1, 2 + 3, 4 | 1, 2 | 3, 4 | 1, 2 | 3, 4 | 1, 2 | 3, 4 | |
K154 | 91 | 26 | 91 | 43 | 56 | 55 | 57 | 12 | 1 |
K158 | 88 | 88 | 93 | 40 | 46 | 48 | 50 | 8 | 4 |
K160 | 82 | 89 | 96 | 42 | 40 | 52 | 47 | 10 | 7 |
K169 | 86 | 90 | 93 | 38 | 38 | 52 | 48 | 14 | 10 |
Average | 87 | 73 | 93 | 41 | 45 | 52 | 51 | 11 | 6 |
SD | 4 | 32 | 2 | 2 | 8 | 3 | 5 | 3 | 4 |
Summary of electrophysiological data for 4 of the individual subjects that received a cochlear implant and several months of electrical stimulation. The average spatial extent of activation area measured across the cochleotopic gradient in the inferior colliculus is estimated for the apical stimulation channel (electrodes 1, 2), for the basal channel (electrodes 3, 4), and for combined stimulation of both channels at the peak intensities of the modulated signals used for the final period of electrical stimulation in each cat immediately before study. For the animal stimulated with the speech processor (K154), the final stimulus levels have been adjusted to estimate equivalent charge for the sinusoidal stimulus waveforms of longer average phase durations. Levels for chronic stimulation were set 2 dB above EABR threshold (or 6 dB for the speech processor), and minimum IC thresholds were consistently lower than the final peak stimulation levels. (Differences shown in last 2 columns)
The areas activated by the two channels were summed (subtracting any overlapping area), and this value was divided by the total IC depth over which responses were recorded at in the same penetration. This value (means shown in third column) provides an estimate of the percentage of the entire IC cochleotopic (frequency) gradient activated at maximum stimulus intensity. The total activation areas were very broad, ranging from 91% to 96%. Thus, the amplitude modulated electrical signals applied for chronic stimulation in these animals (100% modulation depth) varied in intensity from below threshold to levels that activated quite broadly across the frequency range of the auditory midbrain (and presumably also broadly across the CN).
Table 3 also compares the minimum IC thresholds with the final stimulation intensities for each channel in each subject. Chronic stimulation levels were set at 2 dB above the EABR threshold (6 dB for SP), but note that IC thresholds were consistently, and often substantially, lower than the chronic stimulation levels, indicating that neural thresholds in the IC are systematically lower than the evoked response thresholds as reported previously (Beitel et al. 2000).
Spiral ganglion cell degeneration
In the experimental group studied at about 8 months of age after several months of electrical stimulation from a cochlear implant, examination of histological sections showed that all the cochlear hair cells had degenerated in both stimulated (left) and control deafened cochleae. Further, many (or most) of the peripheral radial nerve fibers within the osseous spiral lamina (the dendrites of the SG neurons) also had degenerated, and significant cell loss within the SG itself was observed due to the relatively long durations of deafness. The average SG area fraction (an unbiased stereological measure of the relative area of Rosenthal’s canal occupied by SG cells) was about 46% of normal for the left (stimulated) and 43% of normal for the right (non-stimulated) cochleae (Fig. 1). Four of the five deafened/implanted animals received 6–8 weeks of selegiline administration in an effort to reduce the degenerative effects of deafness. However, the degeneration of the spiral ganglion observed in these animals is similar to results seen previously in neonatally deafened subjects with no selegiline treatment (Leake et al. 2006, 2007). Further, the single cat that did not receive selegiline (K154) had SG densities and CN volumes (Table 2) that fell in the middle of ranges observed in the four other subjects, indicating that the selegiline actually had little or no effect. Note that the area fraction data reflect both cell loss and reduction in cell size from normal. Although we did not evaluate cell areas here, our previous studies have demonstrated significant reductions in SG cell size in these neonatally deafened cats, with cross-sectional soma areas measuring about 75% of normal after comparable durations of deafness (Leake et al. 1999, 2007). Thus, SG area fraction data likely underestimate the actual number (percentage of normal) surviving SG neurons and should be considered minimum estimates. One animal (K160) showed histopathological evidence of infection that affected the SG survival in the left cochlea, which exhibited the lowest survival of all of the studied cochleae (overall area fraction, 21.7% of normal).
Analysis of NB™ injection sites
Because no intact hair cells were present in these deafened animals, we emphasize that the “represented frequencies” of injections were calculated based upon cochlear location in deafened animals only for the purpose of comparing the sites and sizes of injections to the normal control group. These values are not meant to imply that the organ of Corti was functional in the deafened animals. Further, because frequency calculations are based upon percentage of basilar membrane length, it is important to note that the mean basilar membrane length in the deafened group (24.6 mm, n = 10) was identical to the mean for normal adult cats (24.6 mm). This finding is consistent with our previous study, demonstrating that the basilar membrane length in kittens studied at 0 to 3 days postnatal is not significantly different from that of adult cats (Leake et al. 2002). The basilar membrane was already full adult length at the time we began ototoxic drug injections, and, therefore, its length was normal in the deafened animals.
The extent of each injection site was evaluated in serial sections examined for evidence of the penetration of the NB pipette into Rosenthal’s canal and degenerating or missing SG cells (Fig. 2). Because injections were made through the round window, injection sites were limited to relatively high-frequency basal locations ranging from ∼1.5 to 6.5 mm from the base, equivalent to a frequency range of approximately 45 to 15 kHz in the normal adult cat cochlea (Greenwood 1974; Liberman 1982). Whenever possible, two injections were made in each cochlea at sites that were separated as widely as possible in the accessible cochlear region. The more apical injections were made at about where the basal stimulation channel (electrodes 3, 4) of the cochlear implant was centered, at 5–6 mm from the base. Due to the fibrotic and sometimes bony encapsulation of the cochlear implant (Fig. 2C), it was more difficult to make injections in implanted cochleae, and only six successful NB injections were made in the implanted ears (left side), compared to nine injections in the contralateral cochleae. These restricted injections labeled small groups of SG neurons that normally would innervate cochlear sectors ranging in length from ∼300 to 500 μm. The injections in the stimulated cochleae (five of six injection sites measured) and in the deafened control cochleae (nine of nine injection sites measured) averaged 490 and 407 μm, respectively, and these values were not significantly different from the average injection size in normal controls (412 μm). In a normal adult cat, there are about 100 inner hair cells per millimeter of basilar membrane distance (Liberman 1982), and 1 mm of basilar membrane is innervated by a sector of the spiral ganglion approximately 0.6 mm in length in the basal cochlea (Keithley and Cronin-Schreiber 1987).
Measurements of CN cross-sectional area and volume calculations
The overall size of each CN was determined in coronal sections selected at 10% intervals by tracing the perimeter of the CN complex in digitized images as illustrated in Figure 3A–D. The cross-sectional areas were calculated with NIH Image and used to estimate CN volume (see “Methods”). In normal adult controls, CN volume ranged from 18.5 to 23.6 mm3, with a mean of 21.2 mm3 (Table 2). In the deafened/implanted animals studied at ∼32 weeks, the CN volume varied substantially from 12.5 to 16.1 mm3. The volumes of the stimulated (left side) and contralateral CN were virtually identical (means of 14.4 and 14.8 mm3, respectively) and were significantly smaller than normal, averaging 68% and 70% of the CN volume in normal adult controls (Fig. 3E, F). In addition, the mean CN volume is shown for the six CN measured in four younger deafened animals studied at ∼9 weeks of age (Fig. 3G) to provide control data at the approximate age of implantation of the older deafened group. The CN volume in this younger deafened group was significantly smaller than that of the CN in normal animals studied at this same age (eight CN from five animals, mean age at study 8.9 weeks) and also significantly smaller than the CN volume in the older deafened group.
The CN cross-sectional area data were also used to normalize CN projection thickness for each subject to take into account the difference in CN size between the deafened animals and normal controls. The square root of the mean area was calculated for sections from the 20–80% intervals in each subject as an estimate of the mean CN width/height and as a metric to convert the units of the normalizing measure to match the measurements of the CN projection laminae. These values were used to calculate the lamina widths predicted if CN projections in the deafened animals were precisely proportionate to those in normal adults. On average, the square root of the mean CN area for the older, deafened, and implanted group was 1.99, whereas for normal adults, this value was 2.29. Therefore, if proportionate, the thickness of CN projection laminae in deafened and implanted animals, on average, would be 87% (1.99/2.29) of those in adults. In the younger deafened group, the measured thickness of CN projection laminae would be predicted to be 78% (1.79/2.29) of those in adults. Individual predicted values calculated for CN projections in each deafened subject were compared statistically to the actual values measured for projections in each CN subdivision.
Characterization of NB-labeled CN projections
Despite the early deafness and unilateral broadly distributed electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve experienced by these experimental animals, the major subdivisions of all CN were still clearly defined and identifiable by their cytoarchitecture. Neurobiotin-labeled central axons of SG neurons were observed within the auditory nerve in discrete fascicles. After entering the cochlear nerve root, the fibers bifurcated into ascending and descending branches and projected to all three major subdivisions of the CN. The ascending branches projected rostrally to terminate within the AVCN. The descending branches projected caudally to terminate within the PVCN and also gave off fine collaterals that projected medially and dorsally to the DCN. All projections appeared to be fully labeled at post-injection times as short as 7 h. Moreover, the injections in all these deafened animals resulted in discrete bands or laminae of labeled fibers and terminals that always exhibited a clear, normal cochleotopic organization. SG injections made at more basal (higher frequency) locations produced projection bands that were positioned dorsally within each CN subdivision, and apical injections resulted in labeled laminae projecting more ventrally in the CN (Figs. 3 and 5), as described previously in normal cats (Leake and Snyder 1989; Snyder and Leake 1997; Snyder et al. 1997), neonatal kittens (Leake et al. 2002), and deafened animals (Leake et al. 2006). Figure 3B and C illustrate the projections to the stimulated (left side) and contralateral CN in one neonatal subject (K158), and they appear remarkably similar despite the robust electrical excitation of the auditory nerve and CN by the cochlear implant on the left side. Note that the basal channel of our implant is centered at about 15 kHz (based on percent distance from the cochlear base) and thus was positioned in proximity to the SG neurons that were labeled by the 17-kHz injection.
The characteristics of the auditory nerve fibers and terminals labeled by HRP in normal cats have described in detail previously (Ryugo and Fekete 1982; Fekete et al. 1984; Rouiller et al. 1986; Leake and Snyder 1989; Snyder et al. 1997; Leake et al. 2002). The axons of passage and pre-terminal axonal ramifications in the neonatally deafened CN generally appeared somewhat more sparse (i.e., had fewer fibers) and less intensely labeled in all subdivisions of the CN than in controls (Fig. 4). Nevertheless, numerous calyceal endings were identified in the deafened projections to the AVCN on both the stimulated and contralateral sides (Fig. 4A, B), which corresponded in appearance to the endbulbs of Held in normal cats (Ryugo and Fekete 1982; Limb and Ryugo 2000; Leake et al. 2006). In the PVCN, the labeled projections formed laminae that projected in the rostral to caudal plane and were comprised of large axons of passage and finer pre-terminal axons with occasional simple bouton and en passant swellings. Terminals that were quite similar in shape to the calyceal endings of the AVCN, as well as some smaller en passant boutons, were seen in these projections (Fig. 4C, D). Caudally, the fibers terminated in string-like endings oriented orthogonal to the projection bands. In the DCN and small cell cap, the terminal fields were composed of relatively sparse, fine pre-terminal axons exhibiting mostly small bouton endings (Fig. 4E, F). No obvious differences were seen between labeled projections ipsilateral to the stimulated cochleae and those from non-implanted cochleae.
Measurements of CN projection laminae in implanted, electrically stimulated animals
Measurements of CN lamina thickness were made to evaluate the selectivity of projections relative to the frequency gradient of each CN subdivision. Figure 5 illustrates the morphometric method for quantitative analysis of projections. All sections with clear laminae were imaged near the center of each projection, and three scans were made in each image by using a window positioned orthogonal to the projection lamina(e). Figure 5A shows one image used to evaluate PVCN projections in a deafened, stimulated CN along with one raw scan from this image (Fig. 5B, C) and the mean pixel density plot (Fig. 5D). The numerical distributions for all the scans were averaged, background density was subtracted, and projection thickness was defined as the distance containing 90% of the total pixel density (Fig. 5D). This method was applied to obtain objective measurements of the projection widths in each CN subdivision.
Injections were made bilaterally in the five neonatally deafened, unilaterally stimulated animals (ten cochleae). Single injections were made in both cochleae of K154, and double injections were made in the four remaining subjects (total of 18 injections). However, not all injections produced measurable projections to all three CN subdivisions. In particular, after several months of cochlear implantation, the implanted electrodes often were encapsulated in dense fibrous connective tissue or bone that had to be dissected away from the modiolus before NB injections could be made. This pathology made it more difficult to identify Rosenthal’s canal, and fewer injections in the implanted and stimulated ears produced projections that were labeled intensely enough to be measured, as compared to the non-stimulated side.
AVCN projections A total of five projections to the stimulated AVCN and seven to the non-stimulated side were measured, as summarized in Figure 6A. Projection laminae in the stimulated AVCN averaged 0.250 mm in thickness across the CN frequency gradient. The projections in the contralateral AVCN were almost identical with a value of 0.258 mm, and this small difference between sides was not statistically significant (Student’s t test). The absolute values of projection widths measured in both AVCN in the deafened and implanted animals were slightly larger than the corresponding thickness of projections in normal controls (0.223 mm; n = 10 projections measured in six CN), but this difference was not statistically significant even when the projections in both stimulated and non-stimulated CN were pooled and compared to normal as a single group (Student’s t test).
As shown previously, the CN in the deafened animals was significantly smaller, and its volume averaged about 70% of that measured in normal adult controls. To normalize for CN size, we divided the AVCN projection thickness in each CN by the square root of the mean CN areas measured in the 20–80% region (see “Methods”). In normal adults, this ratio averaged 0.097 (0.223/2.29). If the CN and its AVCN projections in deafened animals were proportionately reduced in size, then this ratio should be the same in deafened subjects. For example, the mean square root of CN area on the stimulated side was 1.95 mm, and the average “predicted” AVCN projection thickness if proportionate to normal would be x = 0.097 × 1.95 or 0.189 mm (Fig. 6A). Instead, the measured AVCN projections averaged ∼32% broader (0.250 mm) in the stimulated CN and 34% broader (0.258 mm) in the deafened non-stimulated CN than would be predicted by the scaling calculations. These differences were highly significant (Student’s t test; p < 0.001).
PVCN projections In these same deafened/implanted experimental animals, a total of six PVCN projections were measured on the stimulated side, and all nine projections were measured in the contralateral CN (Fig. 6B). PVCN projections had a mean thickness of 0.235 mm on the stimulated side and 0.214 mm for the contralateral non-stimulated CN. This small difference between sides was not significant, and when the data from the stimulated and contralateral PVCN projections were pooled, they were not significantly different from normal (mean, 0.205 mm.) However, when normalized for the smaller size of the CN, the PVCN projections in the stimulated CN were 34% broader than predicted, and projections to the contralateral CN were 20% broader than would be predicted if they were precisely proportionate to normal adult PVCN projections. These differences between measured and predicted values for PVCN projections were statistically significant (Student’s t test or Mann–Whitney rank sum test; p = 0.011 stimulated CN; p = 0.022 deafened CN).
DCN projections The fibers projecting to the DCN are very fine in caliber and also have the longest distance to travel from the injection sites in the SG, which tended to reduce the yield of projections that could be measured in the DCN. Four DCN projections in the stimulated CN and five DCN projections in contralateral deafened CN of three animals were measured (Fig. 6C). The DCN projections ipsilateral to the stimulated cochlea had a mean thickness of 0.241 mm, and projections from the non-stimulated cochlea had a mean thickness of 0.225 mm. This difference was not statistically significant. In normal controls (five projections in five CN), the DCN projections averaged 0.188 mm. The absolute values for DCN projection thicknesses in deafened subjects were slightly larger than the corresponding normal values, but these differences were not statistically significant. When normalized for CN size, DCN projections in the deafened implanted animals were 53% broader on the stimulated side and 41% broader on the contralateral side than predicted if they were proportionate to normal DCN projections. Again, these differences between the measured and predicted (proportionate) projection widths were statistically significant (Mann–Whitney, p = 0.029 for projections in the stimulated CN; p = 0.037 for the contralateral CN).
Measurements of CN projections at 9 weeks of age in deafened animals
Figure 7 presents the data for CN projections measured in the younger deafened group studied at an average age of 9 weeks, in comparison to both normal adult controls and to the older deafened, implanted group (data are pooled from the stimulated and non-stimulated CN because there was no difference between them). Interestingly, there is no significant difference between the projection thickness values measured in AVCN (0.262 mm), PVCN (0.199 mm), or DCN (0.221) in the younger deafened group as compared to the respective projections measured in the older deafened, implanted group studied at ∼32 weeks of age (AVCN 0.254; PVCN 0.223; DCN 0.232). Moreover, CN projection thicknesses measured in the all three CN subdivisions of the 9 week-old group also were not significantly different from projections measured in normal adult controls.
When normalized for the much smaller CN size in the deafened 9 week-old animals, AVCN projections were broader by 51% than predicted if they were proportionate to AVCN projections in normal adults, PVCN projections were 25% broader, and DCN projections were 50% broader than predicted. These differences between the measured projections and the predicted proportionate projection widths in each subdivision were highly significant (Student’s t test or Mann–Whitney).
Density of projections
Figure 8 illustrates the mean pixel densities for projections measured in the AVCN, PVCN, and DCN of normal adult controls and in neonatally deafened, implanted animals studied at ∼32 weeks of age. There was no difference in pixel densities between CN projections from the stimulated cochlea and the CN projections from the non-implanted ear. Therefore, the data from both sides have been pooled for comparison to the respective projection densities in normal cats. These data demonstrate that pixel density is significantly higher in normal animals than in the deafened projections to each of the three CN subdivisions (Student’s t test, unpaired), as expected due to the significant reduction in the SG cell populations projecting to the CN in deafened animals.
Discussion
The main goal of this work was to examine the topographic specificity of the neural projections from the cochlear spiral ganglion to the cochlear nuclei in animals that had undergone profound hearing loss early in life and received a unilateral cochlear implant at about 7 weeks of age. To our knowledge, this is the first study of the specificity of the auditory nerve projections after chronically applied electrical stimulation in an animal model of congenital profound hearing loss. Our findings help to elucidate the role of normal auditory experience and the effects of early deafness and electrical stimulation from a cochlear implant on the development and maintenance of the precise cochleotopic maps formed by the primary afferents within the CN.
Molecular mechanisms, neuronal activity, and tonotopic map formation
It is generally agreed that neural activity is not involved in the initial formation of topographically ordered sensory pathways. However, there is little agreement about the specific roles of molecular cues and neuronal activity in the subsequent development and refinement of various topographic maps in the central nervous system (Rubel and Cramer 2002). In the auditory system, several lines of evidence suggest that patterns of neural connections are essentially correctly organized from the time of their initial arrival at target locations (Rubel and Fritzsch 2002). In chickens, a tonotopically ordered map of the auditory nerve axons projecting within the CN analogue (nucleus magnocellularis) is present from early embryonic ages, even before the formation of hair cell synapses (Molea and Rubel 2003). Our previous study in cats demonstrated a clear cochleotopic order of SG projections in the CN of kittens examined as young as several days prenatal (Leake et al. 2002), well before hearing onset. Moreover, studies in mutant mice have demonstrated that at least a rudimentary cochleotopic order is established in connections from the SG, even in the absence of differentiated hair cells (Xiang et al. 2003), and central projections can maintain relatively normal organization despite massive rerouting of fibers in the periphery, as seen in Ntf3tgBDNF mice in which vestibular fibers project to the cochlea (Fritzsch et al. 2005).
Recent studies of the molecular mechanisms underlying the establishment of auditory brainstem circuits have demonstrated that Eph protein gradients are important in the formation of tonotopic projections (e.g., from the auditory nerve to the nucleus magnocellularis and from the nucleus magnocellularis to the nucleus laminaris) in chicks and suggesting that Eph signaling may be a conserved mechanism for establishing topographic specificity in diverse sensory systems (Person et al. 2004; Siddiqui and Cramer 2005; see Cramer 2005 for review). Thus, relatively accurate tonotopic maps likely form initially through processes independent of activity and guided by specific molecular mechanisms (Rubel and Fritzsch 2002; Fritzsch et al. 2005, 2006). Eph/ephrin signaling is of particular interest because of its well-established role in formation of topographic maps in other sensory systems, including the visual system (Feldheim et al. 2000), the somatosensory (Prakash et al. 2000), and olfactory systems (Cutforth et al. 2003).
However, after the initial pathway formation, some sensory systems exhibit significant refinement of connections during late prenatal or early postnatal development, with projecting axons undergoing “pruning” of their axonal domains, resulting in more precise topographic maps of the afferent input (LeVay et al. 1978; Cowan et al. 1984; Easter et al. 1985; Sachs et al. 1986; Shatz 1996), with some examples reported in the auditory system (Jackson and Parks 1982; Young and Rubel 1986; Friauf and Lohmann 1999; Leake et al. 2002). As outlined in the “Introduction”, some studies of visual system development have emphasized that neuronal activity plays a critical role in this topographic refinement (Shatz 1996). Competition among ganglion cells is thought to influence the size of terminal arbors of retinogeniculate neurons (Sur et al. 1984; Friedlander and Tootle 1990), and interocular competition is required for normal segregation and refinement of projections from the two eyes (LeVay et al. 1978; Sretavan and Shatz 1986, 1987). Specific spatiotemporal patterns of spontaneous and visual input have been shown to guide the formation of precise synaptic connections (Zhou et al. 2003; Chiu and Weliky 2003), and the progressive refinement of visual receptive fields in the tectum involves an activity-dependent matching of the topography and strengths of excitatory and inhibitory connections (Tao and Poo 2005).
The SG-to-CN projections are of particular interest for studying the role of neuronal activity in the development of topographic maps because their precise organization forms the basis for the tonotopic organization that is maintained at each successive level throughout the central auditory system. Each auditory nerve axon sends collaterals to all three CN subdivisions, where their specialized terminal arborizations provide the basis for all initial signal processing in the central auditory system. Because the cat auditory system is altricial, it provides an excellent opportunity to study the topography of projections before and during the emergence of initial function in the primary afferents. At birth, the cat auditory system is so immature that it is essentially non-functional. Behavioral thresholds are extremely high (Foss and Flottorp 1974; Clements and Kelly 1978; Ehret and Romand 1981), and spontaneous activity cannot be recorded reliably in auditory nerve fibers until several days postnatal (Romand 1984; Walsh and McGee 1986; Jones et al. 2007). Auditory nerve fibers thresholds remain high, tuning is broad, and spontaneous discharge rates are very low (mean <10 spikes/s) until at least P8 to P10 (Romand 1984; Dolan et al. 1985; Walsh and McGee 1986, 1990; Walsh and Romand 1992). During the first postnatal week, sound-evoked discharges in the auditory nerve and CN display rhythmic, bursting patterns in response to long-duration acoustic stimuli (Pujol 1972; Walsh and McGee 1987; Fitzakerley et al. 1995, 1998; Walsh et al. 1998), and this activity is markedly different from the random interspike intervals seen in adult auditory nerve fibers (Kiang et al. 1965).
Postnatal refinement of auditory nerve projections to the CN
In a previous study, we used the same methods employed here to study the normal development of the SG-to-CN projections in kittens from P6–P45 (Snyder and Leake 1997) and found that the topographic specificity of projections in each of the three major CN subdivisions is about as precise in P6–7 kittens as in adults. In contrast, projections in neonates (or in late gestation) are proportionately broader in their distributions across the CN frequency gradient than adult projections (Leake et al. 2002). AVCN projections (normalized for CN size) were more than 50% broader than their adult counterparts, and both PVCN and DCN projections were broader by more than 30% in neonates. Further, two injections that were well-separated in the cochlea resulted in AVCN projections that were incompletely segregated and fused PVCN projections in subjects studied after cesarean section at 2–3 days before the average day of birth. Thus, significant refinement in the primary auditory nerve projections occurs during the postnatal period immediately prior to the onset of hearing (i.e., the period when bursting activity is observed in auditory nerve and CN responses). On the other hand, a clear tonotopic order of the projections to all three CN subdivisions was evident even in the youngest animals examined. Thus, despite the greater extent of spatial overlap of projections demonstrated by quantitative analysis, the clear tonotopic order and relative selectivity exhibited by the SG inputs to the CN at this stage of development are noteworthy.
Neonatally deafened cats
In animals deafened for this study, daily neomycin injections were started at birth and continued for 18–24 days until ABR testing demonstrated profound hearing loss. Thus, the ototoxic drug is administered prior to and during the time when hearing onset would normally occur and results in virtually total loss of hair cells throughout the cochlea by the time profound hearing loss occurs (Leake et al. 1997). Neomycin injections had to be continued beyond the first ABR testing at P16 in all subjects, indicating that some high-threshold acoustic responses were still present at this time, but these animals clearly were severely deprived of normal auditory experience from birth. It is unclear whether spontaneous activity develops and/or persists in the auditory nerve in deafened animals, but even if some spontaneous activity does develop initially, it seems unlikely that it could be maintained for long given complete hair cell loss. Electrophysiological studies in animals acutely deafened by ototoxic drugs have indicated that auditory nerve fibers do not exhibit spontaneous activity (Hartmann et al. 1987; Parkins 1989), whereas two studies in chronically deafened animals have reported some highly aberrant spontaneous activity (Hartmann and Klinke 1989; Shepherd and Javel 1997). It seems reasonable to assume that the deafened non-stimulated CN was largely deprived of direct input throughout the animals’ lives and that the activity elicited by the cochlear implant beginning at 7 weeks of age provided the only major input to the deafened auditory system.
Auditory nerve projections after chronic electrical stimulation
An important finding in the present study is that the cochleotopic organization of the primary afferent auditory nerve (SG) input to the CN is well developed at 9 weeks of age in neonatally deafened animals and is maintained essentially unchanged into adulthood, confirming and extending the results of our previous study (Leake et al. 2006). It is noteworthy that the CN projections in all the deafened CN examined formed discrete “frequency band laminae,” with a clear cochleotopic gradient and normal orientation from dorsal to ventral within each CN subdivision, despite profound bilateral auditory deprivation and lack of normal auditory input throughout life. Furthermore, projections to the CN ipsilateral to a cochlear implant that broadly activated the auditory nerve and central auditory system over periods of several months were indistinguishable from projections to the contralateral CN from the non-stimulated ear, and projections from two injections in a single cochlea always formed segregated projections into each CN subdivision. The maintenance of topographic specificity of the auditory nerve input to the CN supports the finding in previous electrophysiological studies suggesting that animals deafened by this same protocol exhibit relatively normal cochleotopic organization in the auditory midbrain when studied in adulthood (Snyder et al. 1990; Leake et al. 2000). However, those studies also demonstrated that aberrant electrical stimulation of the cochlea (e.g., stereotyped signals delivered on a single channel of a cochlear implant) can significantly distort and expand the central representation of the stimulated channel within the IC.
From this perspective, our data on the specificity of projections in implanted animals argue strongly for an inherent stability in the organization of the primary afferent projections. The classic studies of Moore and Kitzes (Kitzes et al. 1995; Kitzes 1996; Moore and Kitzes 1985; Moore and Kowalchuck 1988) showed that adult animals studied after neonatal removal of one cochlea exhibited substantial reorganization of the central auditory projections, with projections from the intact CN more than doubling in size in their distributions to the ipsilateral IC and forming highly ectopic projections to the superior olivary nuclei and the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body. Our present study provides a parallel experimental paradigm in that animals were deafened as neonates: One ear remained deaf throughout life, but the other ear was strongly activated by electrical stimulation beginning at 7 weeks of age and continuing to adulthood. The findings of equivalent CN frequency band projections on stimulated and deafened sides and absolute widths similar to normal animals provide evidence that there is no major reorganization, formation of ectopic projections, or massive expansion of the primary afferent neural pathways equivalent to changes seen by Moore and Kitzes at higher levels of the central auditory system.
On the other hand, although the SG-to-CN projections in deafened animals formed discrete laminae and exhibited clear spatial segregation, our data also showed that the projection laminae were proportionately broader than normal in both CN of the deafened, implanted subjects. This result is largely due to the fact that the size of the CN was markedly smaller than normal in neonatally deafened animals. When CN projection laminae in deafened animals were normalized for the smaller CN size, projections to all three CN subdivisions were proportionately broader than in normal controls. AVCN projections were 32% broader than normal on both the stimulated and contralateral sides in their relative distributions across the CN frequency gradient. PVCN laminae averaged 34% (stimulated CN) and 20% (non-stimulated side) broader than normal, and DCN projections were 53% and 41% broader than normal in the stimulated and contralateral CN, respectively. Although the data suggest a larger difference in the DCN projections than in the VCN, we believe that this is an artifact of the NB labeling technique rather than a real difference in connectional selectivity because the fibers to the DCN are fine and have longer trajectories from the SG. NB must be transported over a longer distance to label them, and the yield of DCN projections labeled sufficiently for measurement was less than for the other subdivisions. Because there is also some inter-subject variability in injection size, we suggest that the larger DCN difference is due to the more limited number of cases with adequate labeling of DCN fibers and the likelihood that larger injections would better label these fine projections and also result in broader DCN projection widths.
The finding of a markedly smaller CN size in neonatally deafened animals, coupled with the evidence that the SG-to-CN projections in all deafened subjects have widths across the frequency gradient that are similar to those in normal animals (but proportionately broader), indicates that the projections from adjacent sectors of the SG must be more overlapping in these deafened subjects. Our interpretation of these findings is that the topographic specificity of the SG projections to the CN may be significantly degraded after deafness occurring early in life. Because CN projections representing adjacent frequencies in the SG are more overlapping in deafened animals, we infer that the selectivity of the cochleotopic map underlying frequency resolution within the CN also may be less precise (e.g., for discrete stimulation delivered by multiple channels of a cochlear implant). Interestingly, the extent to which CN projections were proportionately broader in these neonatally deafened animals is quite similar to results in normal neonatal kittens as described previously (Leake et al. 2002). When deafness occurs at a young age, the CN fails to grow to its normal adult size, whereas the CN projections appear to attain their normal adult dimensions, perhaps due to an intrinsic tendency for the SG projections to develop axonal arbors of a specific size. Thus, the imprecision in the topographic specificity of the SG-to-CN projections that occurs during early postnatal development may be maintained (or reestablished) in adulthood due to the failure of the CN to achieve normal size when the auditory system is severely deprived of early normal auditory experience. The finding of equivalent projection widths on the stimulated and contralateral sides suggests that neuronal activity elicited by electrical stimulation from a cochlear implant beginning at 7 weeks postnatal (at least the particular stimulation paradigms applied here) is not sufficient to promote normal growth of the CN or for development and maintenance of normal topographic precision of the SG-to-CN projections. Perhaps this is not surprising because the electrical stimulation also failed to maintain normal SG survival. Given that bursting spontaneous activity in the auditory nerve and refinement of CN projections occur before hearing onset, it is also possible that auditory nerve activity must occur within a limited time frame or must more precisely replicate normal input (e.g., must be delivered with specific temporal patterns across frequencies) and that the electrical stimulation in our study took place too late or was too crude to effectively promote normal CN growth and precise refinement of the CN frequency band laminae. It would be interesting to address this issue in the future by introducing electrical stimulation at a very young age. However, it must be emphasized that with respect to clinical relevance, the present study provides a reasonable model of the practical limitations for the application of cochlear implants in the pediatric population (implantation ∼a month after deafness occurs), especially because the onset of hearing in humans occurs in utero.
In this regard, it is important to remember that SG density was reduced by more than 50% in both ears of the deafened/implanted animals. If it were possible to implement a protocol (e.g., electrical stimulation combined with exogenous administration of neurotrophic agents) that maintained more nearly normal SG survival, perhaps closer to normal CN size and projections also would be maintained. Our previous studies have demonstrated that electrical stimulation can partially prevent SG degeneration in cats deafened by the same protocol (Leake et al. 1999, 2007), but in the present study, there was no difference between stimulated and non-implanted cochleae. At least two factors compromised survival in the stimulated ears in the present study. First, one implanted cochlea (K160L) exhibited inflammation and infection that obviously impacted SG survival in that cochlea, which had the lowest SG survival of all the studied ears. In our previous studies of the trophic effects of stimulation, this animal would have been eliminated due to the infection, but because successful NB injections were made on both sides, the data are included here. Secondly, as mentioned earlier, the NB injections per se compromised SG survival in the base of the cochlea, where the largest stimulation-induced differences are typically seen. Schweitzer and Cecil (1992) studied the development of the central terminal arbors of individual labeled auditory nerve fibers in the DCN (in hamsters) and suggested that terminal fields are relatively restricted at birth and actually expand as the animal matures. In agreement with their findings, our previous study showed that absolute widths of projection laminae in all the CN subdivisions in neonatal kittens are smaller than those in adults, although when the small size of the CN is taken into account, the kitten projections are proportionately broader than those in adults (Leake et al. 2002). Together, the available data suggest that the increased precision of the CN frequency map with maturation results primarily from growth of the CN relative to the size of the auditory nerve terminal arbors rather than by retraction of initially large arbors. The initial overlap observed in the projections to adjacent frequency band laminae normally sharpens to adult-like precision during the first postnatal week, but in deafened animals, the CN fails to achieve normal adult size, resulting in normal-sized projection laminae that are more overlapping in the deafened CN. It is conceivable that nearest neighbor SG neurons actually maintain the precision of their synaptic specificity in the deafened CN, such that the proportionately broader projections observed in our NB labeling studies represent an interdigitation of adjacent frequency band laminae. However, given the severe degeneration that occurs in the CN after neonatal deafening, we propose the more conservative interpretation that the proportionately broader distribution relative to the cochleotopic gradient represents a reduction (degradation) in the specificity of the topographic map. Further, previous studies have shown that the target neurons in the CN are markedly reduced in size after neonatal deafening (Osofsky et al. 2001), a finding that would also strongly suggest that the CN projections should be reduced in size (proportionate to the CN) if topographic precision were maintained after deafness.
Ryugo et al. (1997) examined the auditory nerve endings in the CN of congenitally deaf white cats, an animal model of early onset deafness that mimics Scheibe deformity in humans. As in the current study, their results showed well-defined, clearly recognizable endbulbs of Held in the AVCN in adult deaf white cats, but the endbulbs had fewer/less complex branches, and electron microscopy demonstrated reduced synaptic vesicle density and hypertrophy of the postsynaptic densities (Ryugo et al. 1997). A later study also showed modest alterations in the bouton endings on multipolar cells in the AVCN, with ultrastructural analysis demonstrating a smaller bouton size in congenitally deaf cats but no other notable differences (Redd et al. 2002). Our findings in ototoxic drug deafened cats are consistent with these previous reports suggesting that, even after deafness very early in life, the remaining auditory neurons form and maintain well-developed endings that are appropriately specialized for the specific target cell types within the CN. Interestingly, a recent study by Ryugo et al. (2005) demonstrated reversal of the hypertrophic changes in the endbulb synapses following electrical stimulation delivered by a cochlear implant initiated at 2.5 to 5 months of age. This provides an interesting contrast to the present data. Alterations in individual synapses induced by early deafening are reversible with restoration of input from a cochlear implant, but the broader spatial distribution of the synaptic inputs from a restricted population of SG neurons (i.e., a critical band distance) induced by deafening is not reversed by electrical stimulation with an implant.
Auditory nerve projections in deafened animals at 9 weeks of age
Larsen (1984) previously reported that the CN in cats reaches full adult dimensions by 50 days postnatal. Our findings from high-resolution measurements of CN cross-sectional areas in digital images and calculations of volume suggest that the CN continues to grow over a significantly longer period (Fig. 3). Specifically, data from normal animals studied at ∼9 weeks of age showed that the CN cross-sectional area was only 70% of full adult size. Further, data from deafened animals examined at ∼9 weeks showed that the CN continued to grow despite a profound hearing loss early in life and was significantly larger in the older deafened animals examined at ∼32 weeks. This finding raises the question of whether ipsilateral stimulation may have also activated the contralateral CN via commissural or descending (Shore et al. 2003) and thus elicited bilateral effects. However, we previously reported that CN size in a separate group of neonatally deafened animals studied at 16 weeks of age (with no electrical stimulation) was not significantly different from CN size in the contralateral non-stimulated CN of the present group of implanted animals studied at 8 months of age (Leake et al. 2006; Fig. 4C). Thus, we suggest that the CN growth that occurs after 9 weeks of age in these deafened animals is not dependent upon electrical stimulation; and although we cannot define the precise age when this growth is complete, our findings indicate that it continues beyond 9 weeks of age and is complete by 16 weeks of age. Given that the deafened CN is clearly capable of growth during this period, it is unclear why unilateral electrical stimulation did not significantly affect CN volume in the present study.
In addition, the finding that CN projections in the 9-week-old group were not significantly different in absolute thickness from the older deafened group or from normal adult cats again suggests that the CN projections maintain relatively constant projection widths over this age range and despite substantial differences in CN size across groups.
Clinical implications for cochlear implants
The observation that the cochleotopic order of the SG input to the central auditory system is maintained in deafened animals despite profound hearing loss at a very young age and before hearing onset may have important implications for pediatric cochlear implants. If similar principles pertain to human auditory system development, then our findings suggest that the selectivity of the primary afferent connections, which underlie the tonotopic organization of the central auditory system, is basically intact even in congenitally deaf individuals. Further, data also suggest that this organization is not significantly altered by a period of several months of unilateral electrical stimulation from a cochlear implant. This may have important implications for the application of contemporary cochlear implants in congenitally deaf individuals because these devices utilize the cochleotopic organization of the auditory nerve to appropriately encode acoustic information across multiple channels of electrical stimulation. Clinical results with implants generally tend to be poorer in congenitally deaf subjects than in individuals who have had some prior auditory experience (for reviews see Niparko 2004; Pisone and Cleary 2004). However, over time, many congenitally deaf children obtain substantial benefit from their devices, and we suggest that maintenance of the basic cochleotopic organization of the auditory nerve projections to the central auditory system must be a key component underlying that success. On the other hand, if the proportionately broader SG-to-CN projections observed in neonatally deafened animals result in poorer frequency resolution, this would suggest that there may be some inherent limitations in the efficacy of multichannel cochlear implant stimulation in congenitally deaf subjects. In particular, spatial (spectral) selectivity of electrical signals delivered on adjacent channels may be poorer in congenitally deaf cochlear implant users due to the greater overlap of central axons representing nearby frequencies within the CN. Given this limitation, such a cochlear implant user may be more dependent upon temporal features of the electrical stimuli delivered by the implant, and it may be advantageous to enhance the salience of such cues, e.g., even by removing some electrodes from the processor “map” to reduce channel interaction or by using moderate rates of stimulation to enhance the spectral contrasts in the signals.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders of the U.S. National Institutes of Health, Grant #5R01-DC00160 and Contracts N01-DC-3-1006 and #HHS-N-263-2007-00054-C. The authors gratefully acknowledge the expert technical assistance of Steve Rebscher, who designed and fabricated the custom feline cochlear implants used in this study, and Beth Dwan, who assisted in animal surgery, daily stimulation, and care of chronically implanted animals.
References
- Beitel RE, Snyder RL, Schreiner MW, Raggio MW, Leake PA. Electrical cochlear stimulation in the deaf cat: comparisons between psychophysical and central auditory neuronal thresholds. J. Neurophysiol. 83:2145–2162, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Brawer JR, Morest DK, Kane EI. The neuronal architecture of the cochlear nucleus of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 155:251–300, 1974. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cang J, Renteria RC, Kaneko M, Liu X, Copenhagen DR, Stryker MP. Development of precise maps in visual cortex requires patterned spontaneous activity in the retina. Neuron 48:797–809, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Cant NB, Morest DK. The structural basis for stimulus coding in the cochlear nucleus of the cat. In: Berlin C (ed) Hearing Science: Recent Advances. San Diego, College-Hill, pp. 371–421, 1984.
- Chalupa LM, Snider CJ. Topographic specificity in the retinocollicular projection of the developing ferret: an anterograde tracing study. J. Comp. Neurol. 392:35–47, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cheng HJ, Nakamoto M, Bergemann AD, Flanagan JG. Complementary gradients in expression and binding of ELF-1 and Mek4 in development of the topographic retinotectal projection map. Cell 82:371–381, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Chiu C, Weliky M. Synaptic modification by vision. Science 300:1890–1891, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Clements M, Kelly JB. Directional responses by kittens to an auditory stimulus. Dev. Psychobiol. 11:505–511, 1978. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Constantine-Paton M, Cline HT, Debski E. Patterned activity, synaptic convergence and the NMDA receptor in developing visual pathways. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13:129–154, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cowan WM, Fawcett JW, O’Leary DDM, Stanfield BB. Regressive events in neurogenesis. Science 225:1258–1265, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cramer KS. Eph proteins and the assembly of auditory circuits. Hearing Res. 206:42–51, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cutforth T, Moring L, Mendelsohn M, Nemes A, Shah NM, Kim MM, Frisen J, Axel R. Axonal ephrin-As and odorant receptors: coordinate determination of the olfactory sensory map. Cell 114:311–322, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Dolan DF, Teas DC, Walton JP. Postnatal development of physiological responses in auditory nerve fibers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 78:544–554, 1985. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Easter SSJ, Purves D, Rakic P, Spitzer NC. The changing view of neural specificity. Science 230:507–511, 1985. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ehret G, Romand R. Postnatal development of absolute auditory thresholds in kittens. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 95:304–311, 1981. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Fekete DM, Rouiller EM, Liberman MC, Ryugo DK. The central projections of intracellularly labeled auditory nerve fibers in cats. J. Comp. Neurol. 229:432–450, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Feldheim DA, Kim YI, Bergemann AD, Frisen J, Barbacid M, Flanagan JG. Genetic analysis of ephrin-A2 and ephrin-A5 shows their requirement in multiple aspects of retinocollicular mapping. Neuron 25:563–574, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Feller MB, Wellis DP, Stellwagen D, Werblin DFS, Shatz CJ. Requirement for cholinergic synaptic transmission in the propagation of spontaneous retinal waves. Science 272:1182–1187, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Fitzakerley JL, McGee J, Walsh EJ. Characteristics of the bursting behavior found in spontaneous activity and in near-threshold discharge response of immature auditory neurons. Neurosci Abstr. 21(1):127, 1995.
- Fitzakerley JL, McGee J, Walsh EJ. Paradoxical relationship between frequency selectivity and threshold sensitivity during auditory-nerve fiber development. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103(6):3464–3477, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Foss I, Flottorp G. A comparative study of the development of hearing and vision in various species commonly used in experiments. Acta Otolaryngol. 77:202–214, 1974. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Friauf E. Tonotopic order in the adult and developing auditory system of the rat as shown by c-fos immunocytochemistry. Eur. J. Neurosci. 4:798–812, 1992. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Friauf E, Kandler K. Cell birth, formation of efferent connections and establishment of tonotopic order in the rat cochlear nucleus. In: Merchan MA, Juiz JM, Godfrey DA, Mugnaini E (eds) The Mammalian Cochlear Nuclei: Organization and Function. New York, Plenum, pp. 19–28, 1993.
- Friauf E, Lohmann C. Development of auditory brainstem circuitry. Activity-dependent and activity-independent processes. Cell Tissue Res. 297:187–195, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Friedlander MJ, Tootle JS. Postnatal anatomical and physiological development of the visual system. In: Coleman JR (ed) Development of Sensory Systems in Mammals. New York, Wiley, pp. 61–124, 1990.
- Fritzsch B, Pauley S, Matei V, Katz DM, Xiang M, Tessarollo L. Mutant mice reveal the molecular and cellular basis for specific sensory connections to inner ear epithelia and primary nuclei of the brain. Hearing Res. 206:52–63, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Fritzsch B, Pauley S, Feng F, Matei V, Nichols DH. The evolution of the vertebrate auditory system: transformations of vestibular mechanosensory cells for sound processing is combined with newly generated central processing neurons. Intl. J. Comp. Psych. 19:1–24, 2006.
- Goodman CS, Shatz CJ. Developmental mechanisms that generate precise patterns of neuronal connectivity. Cell 72/Neuron 10:77–98, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Greenwood DD. Critical bandwidth in man and some other species in relation to the traveling wave envelope. In: Moskowitz HR, Stevens JC (eds) Sensation and Measurement. Boston, Reidel, pp. 231–239, 1974.
- Greenwood DD. A cochlear frequency-position function for several—29 years later. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 87:2593–2605, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Guillery RW, Stelzner DJ. The differential effects of unilateral lid closure upon the monocular and binocular segments of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 139:413–421, 1970. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hansen MJ, Dallal GE, Flanagan JG. Retinal axon response to ephrins shows a graded, concentration dependent transition from growth promotion to inhibition. Neuron 42:717–730, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hartmann R, Klinke R. Response characteristics of nerve fibers to patterned electrical stimulation. In: Miller JM, Spelman FA (eds) Cochlear Implants: Models of the Electrically Stimulated Ear. New York, Springer, pp. 135–160, 1989.
- Hartmann R, Topp G, Klinke R. Single fiber recordings from the cat auditory nerve with electrical stimulation of the cochlea at different stimulation places. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 96(Suppl. 128):30–31, 1987.
- Jackson H, Parks TN. Functional synapse elimination in the developing avian cochlear nucleus with simultaneous reduction in cochlear nerve axon branching. J. Neurosci. 2:1736–1743, 1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Jones T, Leake PA, Snyder RL, Bonham B, Stakhovskaya O. Spontaneous discharge patterns in cochlear spiral ganglion cells prior to the onset of hearing in cats. J. Neurophysiol. 98(4):1898–1908, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Keithley EM, Cronin-Schreiber R. Frequency map of the spiral ganglion in the cat. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 81:1036–1042, 1987. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kiang NYS, Watanabe T, Thomas EC, Clark LF. Discharge patterns of single fibers in the cat’s auditory nerve. Cambridge, MA, MIT, 1965.
- Kitzes L. Anatomical and physiological changes in the brainstem induced by neonatal ablation of the cochlea. In: Salvi RJ, Henderson D (eds) Auditory System Plasticity and Regeneration. New York, Thieme Medical, pp. 256–274, 1996.
- Kitzes LM, Kageyama GH, Semple MN, Kil J. Development of ectopic projection s from the ventral cochlear nucleus to the superior olivary complex induced by neonatal ablation of the contralateral cochlea. J. Comp. Neurol. 353(3):341–363, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Larsen SA. Postnatal maturation of the cat cochlear nuclear complex. Acta Otolaryngol. Suppl. 417:1–43, 1984. [PubMed]
- Leake PA, Rebscher SJ. Anatomical considerations and long-term effects of electrical stimulation. In: Zeng FG, Popper AN, Fay RR (eds) Auditory Prostheses. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research. New York, NY, Springer, pp. 101–148, 2004.
- Leake PA, Snyder RL. Topographic organization of the central projections of the spiral ganglion in cats. J. Comp. Neurol. 281:612–629, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Leake PA, Kuntz AL, Moore CM, Chambers PL. Cochlear pathology induced by aminoglycoside ototoxicity during postnatal maturation in cats. Hearing Res. 113:117–132, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Leake PA, Hradek GT, Snyder RL. Chronic electrical stimulation by a cochlear implant promotes survival of spiral ganglion neurons after neonatal deafness. J. Comp. Neurol. 412:543–562, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Leake PA, Snyder RL, Rebscher SJ, Moore CM, Vollmer M. Plasticity in central representations in the inferior colliculus induced by chronic single- vs. two-channel electrical stimulation by a cochlear implant after neonatal deafness. Hearing Res. 147:221–24, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Leake PA, Snyder RL, Hradek G. Postnatal refinement of auditory nerve projections to the cochlear nucleus in cats. J. Comp. Neurol. 448(1):6–27, 2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Leake PA, Hradek GT, Chair L, Snyder RL. Neonatal deafness results in degraded topographic specificity of auditory nerve projections to the cochlear nucleus in cats. J. Comp. Neurol. 497:13–31, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Leake PA, Hradek GT, Vollmer M, Rebscher SJ. Neurotrophic effects of GM1 ganglioside and electrical stimulation on cochlear spiral ganglion neurons in cats deafened as neonates. J Comp Neurol. 501:837–853, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- LeVay S, Stryker MP, Shatz CJ. Ocular dominance columns and their development in layer IV of the cat’s visual cortex: a quantitative study. J. Comp. Neurol. 179:1–51, 1978. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Liberman MC. The cochlear frequency map for the cat: labeling auditory nerve fibers of known characteristic frequency. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 72:1441–1449, 1982. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Limb CJ, Ryugo DK. Development of primary axosomatic endings in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus of mice. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol 1(2):103–119, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Maffei L, Galli-Resta L. Correlation in the discharges of neighboring rat retinal ganglion cells during prenatal life. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 87:2861–2864, 1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- McLaughlin T, O’Leary DDM. Molecular gradients and development of retinotopic maps. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28:327–355, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- McLaughlin T, Hindges R, Yates PA, O’Leary DD. Bifunctional action of ephrin-B1 as a repellent and attractant to control bidirectional branch extension in dorsal–ventral retinotopic mapping. Development 130:2407–2418, 2003a. [DOI] [PubMed]
- McLaughlin T, Torborg CL, Feller MB, O’Leary DD. Retinotopic map refinement requires spontaneous retinal waves during a brief critical period of development. Neuron 40:1147–1160, 2003b. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Molea D, Rubel EW. Timing and topography of nucleus magnocellularis innervation by the cochlear ganglion. J. Comp. Neurol. 466:577–591, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Moore DR, Kitzes LM. Projections from the cochlear nucleus to the inferior colliculus in normal and neonatally cochlea-ablated gerbils. J. Comp. Neurol. 240:180–195, 1985. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Moore DR, Kowalchuk NE. Auditory brainstem of the ferret: effects of unilateral cochlear lesions in cochlear nucleus volume and projections to the inferior colliculus. J. Comp. Neurol. 272:503–515, 1988. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Niparko J. Cohlear implants: Clinical applications. In: Zeng FG, Popper AN, Fay RR (eds) Auditory Prostheses. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research. New York, NY, Springer, pp. 53–100, 2004.
- O’Leary DD, McLaughlin T. Mechanisms of retinotopic map development: Ephs, ephrins, and spontaneous correlated retinal activity. Prog Brain Res 147:43–65, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- O’Leary DDM, Wilkinson DG. Eph receptors and ephrins in neural development. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9:65–73, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed]
- O’Leary EE, Yates PA, McLaughlin T. Molecular development of sensory maps: representing sights and smells in the brain. Cell 96:255–269, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Osen KK. Cytoarchitecture of the cochlear nuclei in the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 136:453–484, 1969. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Osen KK. Course and termination of the primary afferents in the cochlear nuclei of the cat: an experimental anatomical study. Arch. Ital. Biol. 108:21–51, 1970. [PubMed]
- Osofsky MR, Moore CM, Leake PA. Does exogenous GM1 ganglioside enhance the effects of electrical stimulation in ameliorating degeneration after neonatal deafness? Hearing Res. 159:23–35, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Parkins CW. Temporal response patterns of auditory nerve fibers to electrical stimulation in deafened squirrel monkeys. Hearing Res. 41:137–168, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Person AL, Cerretti DP, Pasquale EB, Rubel EW, Cramer KS. Tonotopic gradients of Eph family proteins in the chick nucleus laminaris during synaptogenesis. J. Neurobiol. 60:28–39, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Pisone DB, Cleary M. Learning, memory and cognitive processes in deaf children following cochlear implantation. In: Zeng FG, Popper AN, Fay RR (eds) Auditory Prostheses. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research. New York, NY, Springer, pp. 377–426, 2004.
- Prakash N, Vanderhaeghen P, Cohen-Cory S, Frisen J, Flanagan JG, Frostig RD. Malformation of the functional organization of somatosensory cortex in adult ephrin-A5 knock-out mice revealed by in vivo functional imaging. J. Neurosci. 20:5841–5847, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Pujol R. Development of tone-burst responses along the auditory pathway in the cat. Acta Otolaryngol. 74:383–391, 1972. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rasband WS. Image J, U. S. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA, http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/, 2006.
- Redd EE, Cahill HB, Pongstaporn T, Ryugo D. The effects of congenital deafness on auditory nerve synapses: type I and type II multipolar cells in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus of cats. JARO 3:403–417, 2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Romand R. Functional properties of auditory-nerve fibers during postnatal development in the kitten. Exp. Brain Res. 56:395–402, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rouiller EM, Cronin-Schreiber R, Fekete DM, Ryugo DK. The central projections of intracellularly labeled auditory nerve fibers in cats: an analysis of terminal morphology. J. Comp. Neurol. 249:261–278, 1986. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rubel EW, Cramer KS. Choosing axonal real estate: location, location, location. J. Comp. Neurol. 448:1–5, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rubel EW, Fritzsch B. Auditory system development: primary auditory neurons and their targets. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 25:51–101, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ruthazer ES, Cline HT. Insights into activity-dependent map formation from the retinotectal system: a middle-of-the-brain perspective. J. Neurobiol. 59:134–146, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ryan AF, Woolf NK. Development of tonotopic representation in the Mongolian gerbil: a 2-deoxyglucose study. Dev. Brain Res. 41:61–70, 1988. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ryugo DK, Fekete DM. Morphology of primary axosomatic endings in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus of the cat: a study of the endbulbs of Held. J. Comp. Neurol. 210:239–257, 1982. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ryugo DK, Ponstaporn T, Huchton DM, Niparko JK. Ultrastructural analysis of primary endings in deaf white cats: morphologic alteration in endbulbs of Held. J. Comp. Neurol. 385:230–244, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ryugo DK, Kretzmer EA, Niparko JK. Restoration of auditory nerve synapses in cats by cochlear implants. Science 310:1490–1492, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sachs GM, Jacobson M, Caviness VS. Postnatal changes in arborization patterns of murine retinocollicular axons. J. Comp. Neurol. 246:395–408, 1986. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sanes DH, Merickel M, Rubel EW. Evidence for an alteration of the tonotopic map in the gerbil cochlea during development. J. Comp. Neurol. 279:436–444, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Schweitzer L, Cecil T. Morphology of HRP-labeled cochlear nerve axons in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of the developing hamster. Hearing Res. 60:34–44, 1992. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Shatz CJ. Impulse activity and the patterning of connections during CNS development. Neuron 5:745–756, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Shatz CJ. Emergence of order in visual system development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 93:602–608, 1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Shepherd RK, Javel E. Electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve. I. Correlation of physiological responses with cochlear status. Hearing Res. 108:112–144, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Shore SE, Sumner CJ, Bledsoe SC, Lu J. Effects of contralateral sound stimulation on unit activity of ventral cochlear nucleus neurons. Exp. Brain Res. 153(4):427–435, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Siddiqui SA, Cramer KS. Differential expression of Eph receptors and Ephrins in the cochlear ganglion and eighth cranial nerve of the chick embryo. J. Comp. Neurol. 482:309–319, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Snyder RL, Leake PA. Topography of spiral ganglion projection to the cochlear nucleus during postnatal development in cats. J. Comp. Neurol. 384:293–311, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Snyder RL, Rebscher SJ, Cao K, Leake PA. Effects of chronic intracochlear electrical stimulation in the neonatally deafened cat. I: expansion of central spatial representation. Hear Res. 50:7–33, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Snyder RL, Leake PA, Hradek GT. Quantitative analysis of spiral ganglion projections to the cat cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 379:133–149, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Snyder RL, Snyder RL, Vollmer M, Moore CM, Rebscher SJ, Leake PA. Responses of inferior colliculus neurons to amplitude modulated intracochlear electrical pulses in deaf cats. J. Neurophysiol. 84:166–183, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sretavan DW, Shatz CJ. Prenatal development of retinal ganglion cell axons: segregation into eye-specific layers within the cat’s lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Neurosci. 6:234–251, 1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Sretavan D, Shatz C. Axon trajectories and pattern of terminal arborization during prenatal development of the cat’s retinogeniculate pathway. J. Comp. Neurol. 255:356–400, 1987. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sur M, Weller RE, Sherman SM. Development of X- and Y-cells retinogeniculate terminations in kittens. Nature Lond. 310:246–249, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Tao HW, Poo M-m. Activity-dependent matching of excitatory and inhibitory inputs during refinement of visual receptive fields. Neuron 45:829–836, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Tatton WG, Chalmers-Redman RME, Pong A, Rideout HJ, Tatton NA. Neuronal death and deprenyl in Parkinson’s disease: a conceptual journey from MAO-B to glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase. GreyCell Pharm. Res. 914:1–13, 1999.
- Tolbert LB, Morest DK. The neuronal architecture of the anteroventral cochlear nucleus of the cat in the region of the cochlear nerve root: electron microscopy. Neurosci 7:3053–3067, 1982. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Vollmer M, Leake PA, Beitel R, Rebscher SJ, Snyder RL. Degradation in temporal resolution in the auditory midbrain after prolonged deafness is reversed by electrical stimulation of the cochlea. J. Neurophysiol. 93:3339–3355, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Vollmer M, Snyder RL, Leake PA, Moore CM, Rebscher SJ. Temporal properties of chronic intracochlear electrical stimulation determine temporal resolution of neurons in the cat inferior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 82:2883–2902, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Walsh EJ, McGee J. The development of function in the auditory periphery of cats. In: Altschuler RA, Bobbin RP, Hoffman DW (eds) Neurobiology of Hearing: The Cochlea. New York, Raven, pp. 247–269, 1986.
- Walsh EJ, McGee J. Postnatal development of auditory nerve and cochlear nucleus neuronal responses in kittens. Hearing Res. 28:97–116, 1987. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Walsh EJ, McGee J. Frequency selectivity in the auditory periphery: similarities between damaged and developing ears. Am. J. Otolaryngol. 11:23–32, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Walsh EJ, Romand R. Functional development of the cochlea and cochlear nerve. In: Romand R (ed) Development of Auditory and Vestibular Systems 2. Amsterdam, Elsevier, pp. 161–219, 1992.
- Walsh EJ, McGee J, McFadden SL, Liberman MC. Long-term effects of sectioning the olivocochlear bundle in neonatal cats. J. Neurosci. 18(10):3859–3869, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Webster WR, Martin RL. The development of frequency representation in the inferior colliculus of the kitten. Hearing Res. 55:70–80, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Xiang M, Maklad A, Privola U, Fritzsch B. Brn3c null mutant mice show long-term, incomplete retention of some afferent inner ear innervation. BMC Neurosci. 4:2, 2003, DOI 10.1186/1471-2202-4-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Young SR, Rubel EW. Embryogenesis of arborization pattern and topography in nucleus laminaris of the chicken brain stem. J. Comp. Neurol. 254:425–459, 1986. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Zhou Q, Tao HW, Poo MM. Reversal and stabilization of synaptic modifications in a developing visual system. Science 300:1953–1957, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed]