Abstract
The neocortex generates rhythmic electrical activity over a frequency range covering many decades. Specific cognitive and motor states are associated with oscillations in discrete frequency bands within this range, but it is not known whether interactions and transitions between distinct frequencies are of functional importance. When coexpressed rhythms have frequencies that differ by a factor of two or more interactions can be seen in terms of phase synchronization. Larger frequency differences can result in interactions in the form of nesting of faster frequencies within slower ones by a process of amplitude modulation. It is not known how coexpressed rhythms, whose frequencies differ by less than a factor of two may interact. Here we show that two frequencies (gamma – 40 Hz and beta2 – 25 Hz), coexpressed in superficial and deep cortical laminae with low temporal interaction, can combine to generate a third frequency (beta1 – 15 Hz) showing strong temporal interaction. The process occurs via period concatenation, with basic rhythm-generating microcircuits underlying gamma and beta2 rhythms forming the building blocks of the beta1 rhythm by a process of addition. The mean ratio of adjacent frequency components was a constant – approximately the golden mean – which served to both minimize temporal interactions, and permit multiple transitions, between frequencies. The resulting temporal landscape may provide a framework for multiplexing – parallel information processing on multiple temporal scales.
Keywords: EEG, neocortex, gamma rhythm, beta rhythm, inhibition
Introduction
Rhythmic activity in the EEG occurs over temporal scales from <1 Hz (Steriade, 1999), through well known EEG bands (2–80 Hz) up to very fast oscillations (Canolty et al., 2006). Long-term EEG spectra demonstrate a 1/f relationship between power and frequency – a pattern common in other cortical activity patterns and natural dynamic systems (Plenz and Thiagarajan, 2007). In contrast, spectral analysis of short epochs of EEG activity reveal discrete spectral peaks with underlying network mechanisms that can be related functionally to their associated psychophysical processes (Baker et al., 2006; Tallon-Baudry et al., 1999; Womelsdorf et al., 2006). Many of these rhythms occur with the apparent relationship between adjacent frequencies approximating to the natural log (e) (Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004). However, while this observation relates brain activity to other natural dynamical systems, the mechanisms underlying such a distribution of frequencies, and their inter-relationships, remain elusive. In vitro models of rhythmic activity provide a means to study in detail the range of possible, stable oscillatory states capable of being generated in neocortex (Cunningham et al., 2004; Roopun et al., 2006).
Linear dynamic techniques have allowed detailed analysis of interrelationships between coexistent rhythms in single cortical areas (Le van Quyen and Bragin, 2007). From these analyses two main patterns of interaction are apparent. Firstly, a single frequency of population rhythm may be amplitude modulated by a coexistent lower frequency, producing a phenomenon referred to as ‘nesting’ of one frequency in another. This pattern is seen when considering gamma (30–80 Hz) rhythms coexisting with theta (4–12 Hz) frequency oscillations in hippocampus (Bragin et al., 1995) and entorhinal cortex (Cunningham et al., 2003). Further examples of this type of temporal interaction include that of very fast oscillations (>80 Hz) within alpha rhythms (Grenier et al., 2001) and multiple, broad band frequencies within slow wave oscillations (Tononi et al., 2006). Well structured amplitude modulation of rhythms on multiple scales can be seen in hippocampus, with gamma rhythms nested within theta rhythms, also serving to amplitude modulate very fast oscillations in field potentials (Traub et al., 2003a) and excitatory synaptic inputs to interneurons (Gloveli et al., 2005). Secondly, when pairs of frequencies differ by approximately a factor of 2–3, phase synchrony between population rhythms have been seen (Palva et al., 2005). In this case, for example, a 40 Hz rhythm can be seen to synchronize with a 20 Hz rhythm only on every second period of the faster rhythm. Further ratios of frequencies can be shown to exhibit phase synchrony (Palva and Palva, 2007), but as yet, for methodological reasons, the ratios (n:m) of frequencies seen experimentally to support this interaction have to be integers. Here we demonstrate the pattern of interaction between coexistent frequencies when the ratio is <2 and non-integer. In this situation we show that modulation of excitatory drive to neocortical networks can produce a number of discrete oscillatory states interrelated in both frequency and period by a novel process of rhythm interaction period concatenation.
Materials and Methods
Experiment
Horizontal slices of parietal cortex of 450 μm thick were prepared from adult male Wistar rats (150–250 g) following terminal anesthesia with ketamine/xylazine. All procedures were performed in accordance with the UK animals (scientific Procedures) act. Neocortical slices containing secondary somatosensory cortex were maintained at 34°C at the interface between warm wetted 95% O2/5% CO2 and acsf containing (in mM): 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 1 MgSO4, 1.2 CaCl2, 24 NaHCO3, 10 glucose and 126 NaCl. Extracellular recordings from Layers II and V of somatosensory cortex were obtained using glass micropipettes containing the above acsf (resistance <0.5 MΩ). Intracellular recordings of neurons with somata in layer II or V were taken with sharp microelectrodes filled with potassium acetate (resistance 30–90 MΩ). Signals were analog filtered at 2 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz. Coherence and cross corellograms were all calculated from 60 s epochs of field data using Matlab. Unit timing histograms were obtained by measuring the mean temporal separation between concurrent single unit recordings in LII and LV. Relative spike timings were obtained from concurrent intracellular and field recordings, with spikes binned (1 ms width) with respect to the peak positivity in the local field potential on each period.
Model
The mathematical model represents a reduction of a detailed single-column model (Traub et al., 2005). We implement populations of the four cell types observed in the in vitro preparations. In the superficial layer we include the regular spiking (RS) pyramidal cell, the fast spiking (FS) interneuron, and an inhibitory low threshold spiking (LTS) cell, and in the deep layer the intrinsic bursting (IB) cell. Each individual cell consists of one or three compartments and intrinsic currents consistent with previous models (Cunningham et al., 2004; Roopun et al., 2006; Traub et al., 2003b, 2005) and the experimental data. We connect the cell populations with chemical and electrical synapses to establish a network model of the two cortical layers. We developed the reduced model to capture the fundamental dynamical characteristics of the activity using simple, yet biophysical, currents and connections.
In the superficial layer we implemented a Pyramidal-Interneuron-Network-Gamma (PING) model with two cells: a RS cell and FS cell. The single compartment RS cell consisted of four intrinsic membrane currents: a transient inactivating sodium current (NaF current), a delayed rectifier potassium current (KDR current), a hyperpolarization activated (or anomalous rectifier) current (h-current), and a leak current. The single compartment FS cell consisted of three intrinsic membrane currents: a NaF current, a KDR current, and a leak current. We also included in the superficial layer a single compartment LTS interneuron with four intrinsic currents: a NaF current, a KDR current, an h-current, and a leak current. We connected the RS and FS cell, and the RS cell and LTS interneuron, with reciprocal synapses, included inhibitory autapses on the FS cell and LTS interneuron, and an inhibitory synapse from the FS cell to LTS interneuron. We did not include fast rhythmic bursting (FRB) neurons in the reduced gamma model. Recent experimental and modeling results suggest that these cells provide excitation via axonal plexus activity downstream from RS pyramidal cell somata to drive neocortical gamma rhythms (Cunningham et al., 2004). Instead, to avoid the complexity of such a system, while maintaining sufficient phasic drive to interneurons to generate gamma rhythms, we included tonic input currents injected to the RS and FS cells to maintain the gamma rhythm (Borgers et al., 2005). This simplifying strategy maintains the ability of the network to generate interneuron-mediated PING without the requirement of modeling a large (many thousands of neurons) axonally interconnected array of principal cells.
To model the deep layer beta2 rhythm, we implemented a three compartment reduction of the IB cell (Traub et al., 2005). The three compartments (an axon, soma, and dendrite) each contained the intrinsic membrane spiking currents: a NaF current, a KDR current, and a leak current. In addition, we included in the axon a muscarinic receptor suppressed potassium current (M-current), and in the dendrite we included an h-current, an M-current, and a high-threshold noninactivating calcium current (CaH current).
To establish population models of the superficial and deep layer activities, we created twenty replications of the RS-FS-LTS cell circuit and of the IB cell. We made all parameters identical for each cell type in the population except for a depolarizing input current which we independently varied for each cell. This heterogeneity resulted in a range of gamma activity (30–50 Hz) in the superficial layer and beta2 activity (20–30 Hz) in the deep layer. We connected the superficial to deep layer with synapses from the LTS interneurons to the IB cell dendrites, and connected the deep to superficial layer with synapses from the IB cell axons to the FS cells and LTS interneurons. In addition, we connected the twenty RS cells together, and the twenty IB cells together, with all-to-all electrical coupling. To model the transition to beta1, we included plastic NMDA synapses from the axonal to dendritic compartments of the IB cell population and decreased the excitation of the network.
Results
Gamma rhythms (38 ± 3 Hz, n = 25) were generated by 400 nM kainate in somatosensory neocortical slices in layers II and III (Cunningham et al., 2004), whereas beta2 rhythms (24 ± 2 Hz, n = 25) were concurrently generated in layer V (Roopun et al., 2006) (Figure 1A). Mean synchrony between laminae II/III and V was approximately 0 (Figure 2B), but instantaneous synchrony changed rhythmically at frequencies within the EEG beta1 band (15 ± 3 Hz, Figure 2A). Both rhythms persisted following resection of cortex at the level of layer IV (Roopun et al., 2006), but this physical separation of the two oscillating layers increased the power of the gamma rhythm significantly (215 ± 29%, n = 6, data not shown). This suggested some functional connectivity between layer V principal cells and superficial layer neurons involved in generating the gamma rhythm (Thomson and Morris, 2002).
Acute reduction in glutamatergic excitation with 2.5 μM NBQX [to mimic the decay in excitatory drive via neocortical ascending input (Metherate and Cruikshank, 1999)] abolished population gamma and beta2 rhythms and produced a single beta1 frequency rhythm (15 ± 2 Hz, n = 10) in both layers (Figure 1B). This persistent rhythm had a significantly lower modal frequency than both the previously expressed beta2 and the gamma rhythms (P < 0.05, n = 10). During beta1 frequency rhythms, local field potentials in deep and superficial cortical layers oscillated with stable phase difference (Figure 2B), with the rhythmic, beta1 frequency changes in phase between laminae seen during coexpression of gamma and beta2 rhythms now absent (Figure 2A). Local field potential and unit cross correlations between layers II and V revealed significantly asymmetric side-peaks (Figure 2C and D). Population activity in LII lagged behind activity in LV by 25 ± 5 ms (n = 25), and activity in LV, in turn, lagged behind LII by 41 ± 3 ms (n = 25, Figure 2B and C, P < 0.05 comparing the timing of the two side peaks). The temporal distribution of LV units, with respect to LII units, also showed a similar pattern of concatenation, with LII spiking alternately following and preceding LV spikes by 22 ± 6 ms and 40 ± 7 ms, respectively (n = 7, Figure 2C, P < 0.05). Separation of deep from superficial layers at the level of layer IV abolished the population beta1 rhythm (data not shown). This pattern of period concatenation, and the requirement of connectivity between laminae, suggested that microcircuits in LII and LV were still generating spikes in a manner subject to their respective network time constants, but now in a mutually interdependent manner.
Individual neuronal inputs and outputs, during beta1 population rhythms, revealed the presence of multiple frequency components. The two main principal cell populations [LV intrinsic bursting (n = 7) and LII regular spiking neurons (n = 12)] had unimodal interspike interval distributions corresponding to 2.2 ± 0.4 Hz and 5.9 ± 0.4 Hz, respectively (Figure 3A). Two subtypes of superficial layer interneurons [fast spiking (FS) and low threshold spiking (LTS)] also had different spike rates. FS interneurons generated outputs at gamma (39 ± 4 Hz, n = 3) and also beta1 and alpha frequencies (Figure 3A). LTS interneuron output was at beta1 frequency (interspike interval 64 ± 8 ms, n = 5). Excitatory synaptic inputs to these cells were small compared to the control (gamma/beta2 rhythm) condition owing to the presence of NBQX. Modal peaks in the distribution of excitatory potentials were seen at beta1 frequency in all neurons except regular spiking cells, with additional peaks in the theta (6.0 ± 1.6 Hz), alpha (11 ± 2 Hz), beta2 (24 ± 2 Hz) and gamma (42 ± 5 Hz) bands (Figure 3B). A similar pattern of multiple peaks in the spectra for inhibitory postsynaptic potentials was also seen (Figure 3C).
Both EPSPs and particularly IPSPs in LTS cells revealed evidence for period concatenation (Figure 3B and C). Fast synaptic excitation and inhibition occurred in doublets during each field beta1 period, phase locked to the local field potential and with a doublet interval of 27 ± 4 ms and 24 ± 6 ms (for EPSPs and IPSPs, respectively, n = 3). Distribution of action potentials in each cell type, temporally averaged with respect to the peak positivity of the local field potential beta1 rhythm, suggested a different source for each component of the EPSP and IPSP doublets. The initial, broader EPSP in the sequence in low threshold spiking cells coincided with burst generation in IB cells whereas the later EPSP coincided with spiking in superficial RS cells (Figure 4A). The initial IPSP in LTS cells coincided with action potential generation in fast spiking interneurons whereas the later IPSP coincided with low threshold spiking interneuron output.
To test whether it was possible for such a sequence of events to generate concatenation of LV beta2 and LII/III gamma frequencies we constructed a model expressing both inter- and intralaminar interactions in LII and LV as suggested by the data (Figure 4Bi). With high driving conductances the model reproduced the gamma and beta2 rhythms seen in experiments. With reduction of these drives the model readily reproduced both the observed sequence of spike generation and the separation of principal cell spiking (IB and RS) by one gamma period (Figure 4Bii) within a temporal framework of gamma/beta2 period concatenation (Figure 4Biii). Both model and data indicated a pattern of events precipitated by IB neuronal activity projected up to superficial fast spiking interneurons, with subsequent rebound spiking of superficial pyramidal cells predominantly activating low threshold spiking neurons. In addition the model predicted that the beta2 component of the concatenated rhythm arose from low threshold spiking interneuronal inhibition of IB neurons, resetting the intrinsic rhythm-generating behavior of this cell population and thus delaying the next burst for one period of this intrinsic beta2 rhythm. This pattern of reset of intrinsic rhythmicity in principal cells appears analogous to that seen for interneuron inputs onto hippocampal principal cells (Cobb et al., 1995).
Discussion
Coexistence of multiple discrete frequencies of population rhythm is seen in neocortex (Lakatos et al., 2007; Womelsdorf et al., 2007), with interaction between frequencies proposed to underlie aspects of sensory processing (Palva and Palva, 2007; Womelsdorf et al., 2007). The pattern of distribution of frequency bands seen here suggests that the distribution of these bands is not random. The mean frequency of the beta1 rhythm was the difference of the mean frequencies of the gamma and beta2 rhythms, and the mean period of the beta1 rhythm was their sum. Also, the ratios of the upper and lower two adjacent bands was the same. Algebraically, this ratio must therefore be the golden mean (approximately 1.6 – the value observed experimentally here). In addition, the formula for the difference of the frequencies implies, from standard trigonometry, that there is a beating frequency at beta1 between the superficial gamma rhythm and the deep beta2 rhythm as observed (Figure 2A). This relationship between frequency bands appears different from the natural log relationship proposed in Buzsaki and Draguhn (2004). However, the two scales are closely related (golden mean ≈ e0.5), suggesting a similar overall temporal structure but with twice as many stable, discrete rhythms – for example with separation of the beta band into functionally (Haenschel et al., 2000) and mechanistically (the present data) distinct sub-bands.
The mechanisms underlying concatenation of beta2 and gamma frequency rhythms here involves the interaction between oscillations whose peak frequencies are determined by network interactions (the gamma rhythm) and intrinsic neuronal properties (the beta2 rhythm), via a single interneuron subtype – the LTS cell. Some subtypes of LTS cell impart dendritic events with kinetics compatible with beta frequency rhythms in layer V neurons (Silberberg and Markram, 2007) through interaction between inhibitory postsynaptic events and intrinsic conductances (in particular hyperpolarizing activated conductance Ih). Both excitatory and inhibitory LTS cells have been reported in neocortex (de la Peña and Geijo-Barrientos, 1996). However, it has been shown that excitatory neurons may change their firing patterns (in a continuum from RS to IB type firing) in a highly labile manner depending on conditions (e.g., ionic composition of the cerebrospinal fluid, neuromodulatory influences) (Steriade, 2004) making definitive identification of subtle pyramidal neuron subclasses difficult. The pattern of LTS output seen in the present study corresponded (in frequency and phase) with slow inhibitory potentials in layer V IB neurons, suggesting that they were inhibitory. To test whether the same temporal pattern of concatenation could occur with excitatory LTS cells in the circuit we used the model, as described, changing only the profile of LTS cell output from inhibitory to excitatory (Figure 5). In this case the model did not generate the pauses in firing essential for concatenation – no beta1 rhythm was seen. Instead, both in conditions generating gamma/beta2 rhythms and beta1 rhythms with inhibitory LTS cells, a switch to excitatory LTS cells produced a global, poorly correlated high frequency firing pattern in all cells simulated.
The interrelationships between the gamma and beta oscillations seen in the present work suggest a possible general scheme of rhythm concatenation. Event frequencies (spikes, EPSPs and IPSPs) were also seen with modal peaks at delta, theta, alpha and high gamma (c.70 Hz) in neuronal behavior during the field potential beta1 rhythm (see Figure 3). However, the experimental model used represents a reduced and highly stable rhythm generating environment. Transient, as opposed to persistent, cortical drive generates a broad range of frequencies, and thalamic – and external sensory – involvement in cortical function also generate variable frequencies. For example, thalamocortical spindle oscillations may vary in frequency from within the delta band up to beta frequencies (Destexhe and Sejnowski, 2003), variable whisking frequency (around the theta/alpha bands) can be matched by oscillations in superficial barrel cortex (Ahissar et al., 1997) and cortical theta rhythms may be variably paced by the sniff cycle in rodents (Uchida and Mainez, 2003). Clearly then, a concatenation sequence represents only one aspect of cortical dynamics.
Given its overt expression in isolated, persistently oscillating cortex (as seen here), concatenated sequences of frequency bands may represent a spectral baseline upon which sensory and neuromodulatory influences may act. The functional significance of the ratio between multiple frequencies seen here for gamma and beta rhythms is not yet understood for neuroscience, though it has been much discussed in the context of other natural systems, notably phyllotaxis (Atela et al., 2002). In the current context, this ratio fosters minimal interference between pairs of co-existent frequencies (Figure 6). It has been suggested that multiple EEG frequencies in humans are arranged as harmonics related to the golden mean (Weiss and Weiss, 2003). In addition, it has been proposed that sensory information may be handled more efficiently if it is processed along multiple ‘channels’ with differing temporal scales (Wiskott and Sejnowski, 2002). For example, evidence for the segregation of sensory information into different ‘frequency channels’ is apparent from studies examining how different levels of detail in the visual field are processed (Smith et al., 2006). In this study perception based on processing coarse level features occurred at theta frequencies whereas perception of objects requiring more detailed features was associated with a faster, beta frequency rhythm. A ratio of c.1.6 between coexpressed frequencies may permit many ‘channels’ for information processing to coexist with minimal temporal interference (multiplexing). A framework of discrete network frequencies, as opposed to a continuum of oscillation states also better fits the discrete anatomical organization of larger scale networks in neocortex into hierarchies and clusters (Sporns et al., 2004).
Concatenation of gamma and beta2 rhythms serves to unite activity in these coexistent frequency channels, generating population activity coherent throughout cortical laminae. It is interesting to note that neurons in primary targets for motor activity in neocortex (e.g., the cuneate nucleus) also have resonant frequencies tuned to either beta1 or beta2 frequencies (Sanchez et al., 2006), suggesting that period concatenation may shape responses to neocortical activity in the periphery. However, it is not yet clear whether interactions between other dominant neocortical rhythms (at alpha and lower frequencies), where adjacent frequency ratios can also be less than two, also demonstrate concatenation.
The present data suggest that cortical activity can be composed of a discrete sequence of stable oscillations with ratios close to the golden mean. In this framework of frequency relationships, interactions between frequencies in the beta and gamma bands are minimized. The phase relationship between two coexistent rhythms is not stable, but the frequency of the concatenation sum is seen in the periodic changes in this phase relationships. Changes in network drive may expose the phase-encoded rhythm in the frequency domain – serving to transform the temporal structure of neocortical activity through period concatenation.
Acknowledgments
We thank The MRC, The Wolfson Foundation, the NIH and the Royal Society for financial support, and S. Baker for discussion of data.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
- Ahissar E., Haidarliu S., Zacksenhouse M. (1997). Decoding temporally encoded sensory input by cortical oscillations and thalamic phase comparators. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 11633–11638 10.1073/pnas.94.21.11633 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atela O., Gole C., Hotten S. (2002). A dynamical system for plant pattern formation: a rigorous analysis. J. Nonlinear Sci. 12, 641–676 10.1007/s00332-002-0513-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baker S. N., Chiu M., Fetz E. E. (2006). Afferent encoding of central oscillations in the monkey arm. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 3904–3910 10.1152/jn.01106.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borgers C., Epstein S., Kopell N. J. (2005). Background gamma rhythmicity and attention in cortical circuits: a computational study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 7002–7007 10.1073/pnas.0502366102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bragin A., Jando G., Nadasdy Z., Hetke J., Wise K., Buzsaki G. (1995). Gamma (40–100 Hz) oscillations in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. J. Neurosci. 15, 47–60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G., Draguhn A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929 10.1126/science.1099745 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canolty R. T., Edwards E., Dalal S. S., Soltani M., Nagarajan S. S., et al. (2006). High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science 313, 1626–1628 10.1126/science.1128115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cobb S. R., Buhl E. H., Halasy K., Paulsen O., Somogyi P. (1995). Synchronization of neuronal activity in hippocampus by individual GABAergic interneurons. Nature 378, 75–78 10.1038/378075a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham M. O., Davies C. H., Buhl E. H., Kopell N., Whittington M. A. (2003). Gamma oscillations induced by kainate receptor activation in the entorhinal cortex in vitro. J. Neurosci. 29, 9761–9769 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham M. O., Whittington M. A., Bibbig A., Roopun A., LeBeau F. E., et al. (2004). A role for fast rhythmic bursting neurons in cortical gamma oscillations in vitro. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 7152–7157 10.1073/pnas.0402060101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Peña E., Geijo-Barrientos E. (1996). Laminar localization, morphology, and physiological properties of pyramidal neurons that have the low-threshold calcium current in the guinea-pig medial frontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 16, 5301–5311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Destexhe A., Sejnowski T. J. (2003). Interactions between membrane conductances underlying thalamocortical slow-wave oscillations. Physiol. Rev. 83, 1401–1453 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gloveli T., Dugladze T., Saha S., Monyer H., Heinemann U., Traub R. D., Whittington M. A., Buhl E. H. (2005). Differential involvement of oriens/pyramidale interneurons in hippocampal network oscillations in vitro. J. Physiol. 562, 131–147 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.073007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grenier F., Timofeev I., Steriade M. (2001). Focal synchronization of ripples (80–200 Hz) in neocortex and their neuronal correlates. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 1884–1898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haenschel C., Baldeweg T., Croft R. J., Whittington M., Gruzelier J. (2000). Gamma and beta frequency oscillations in response to novel auditory stimuli: a comparison of human electroencephalogram (EEG) data with in vitro models. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 7645–7650 10.1073/pnas.120162397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakatos P., Chen C.-M., O'Connell M. N., Mills A., Schroeder C. E. (2007). Neuronal oscillations and multisensory interaction in primary auditory cortex. Neuron 53, 279–292 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le van Quyen M., Bragin A. (2007). Analysis of dynamic brain oscillations: methodological advances. Trends Neurosci. 30, 365–373 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metherate R., Cruikshank S. J. (1999). Thalamocortical inputs trigger a propagating envelope of gamma-band activity in auditory cortex in vitro. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 160–174 10.1007/s002210050726 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva J. M., Palva S., Kaila K. (2005). Phase synchrony among neuronal oscillations in the human cortex. J. Neurosci. 25, 3962–3972 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4250-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva S., Palva J. M. (2007). New vistas for alpha-frequency band oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 30, 150–158 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plenz D., Thiagarajan T. C. (2007). The organizing principles of neuronal avalanches: cell assemblies in the cortex? Trends Neurosci. 30, 101–110 10.1016/j.tins.2007.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roopun A. K., Middleton S. J., Cunningham M. O., LeBeau F. E., Bibbig A., et al. (2006). A beta2-frequency (20–30 Hz) oscillation in nonsynaptic networks of somatosensory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 15646–15650 10.1073/pnas.0607443103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez E., Reboreda A., Romero M., Lamas J. A. (2006). Spontaneous bursting and rhythmic activity in the cuneate nucleus of anaesthetized rats. Neuroscience 141, 487–500 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.03.050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silberberg G., Markram H. (2007). Disynaptic inhibition between neocortical pyramidal cells mediated by Martinotti cells. Neuron 53, 735–746 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith M. L., Gosselin F., Schyns P. G. (2006). Perceptual moments of conscious visual experience inferred from oscillatory brain activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 5626–5631 10.1073/pnas.0508972103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O., Chialvo D. R., Kaiser M., Hilgetag C. C. (2004). Organisation, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 418–415 10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steriade M. (1999). Coherent oscillations and short-term plasticity in corticothalamic networks. Trends Neurosci. 22, 337–345 10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01407-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steriade M. (2004). Neocortical cell classes are flexible entities. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 121–134 10.1038/nrn1325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallon-Baudry C., Kreiter A., Bertrand O. (1999). Sustained and transient oscillatory responses in the gamma and beta bands in a visual short-term memory task in humans. Vis. Neurosci. 16, 449–459 10.1017/S0952523899163065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomson A. M., Morris O. T. (2002). Selectivity in the inter-laminar connections made by neocortical neurones. J. Neurocytol. 31, 239–246 10.1023/A:1024117908539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi G., Massimini M., Riedner B. A. (2006). Sleepy dialogues between cortex and hippocampus: who talks to whom? Neuron 52, 748–749 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub R. D., Contreras D., Cunningham M. O., Murray H., LeBeau F. E., et al. (2005). Single-column thalamocortical network model exhibiting gamma oscillations, sleep spindles, and epileptogenic bursts. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 2194–2232 10.1152/jn.00983.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub R. D., Cunningham M. O., Gloveli T., LeBeau F. E., Bibbig A., Buhl E. H., Whittington M. A. (2003a). GABA-enhanced collective behavior in neuronal axons underlies persistent gamma-frequency oscillations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 11047–11052 10.1073/pnas.1934854100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub R. D., Pais I., Bibbig A., LeBeau F. E., Buhl E. H., et al. (2003b). Contrasting roles of axonal (pyramidal cell) and dendritic (interneuron) electrical coupling in the generation of neuronal network oscillations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 1370–1374 10.1073/pnas.0337529100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchida N., Mainez Z. F. (2003). Speed and accuracy of olfactory discrimination in the rat. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1224–1226 10.1038/nn1142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss H., Weiss V. (2003). The golden mean as clock cycle of brainwaves. Chaos Solitons Fractals 4, 643–652 10.1016/S0960-0779(03)00026-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wiskott L., Sejnowski T. J. (2002). Slow feature analysis: unsupervised learning of invariances. Neural Comput. 14, 715–770 10.1162/089976602317318938 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womelsdorf T., Fries P., Mitra P. P., Desimone R. (2006). Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection. Nature 439, 733–736 10.1038/nature04258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womelsdorf T., Schoffelen J. M., Oostenveld R., Singer W., Desimone R., et al. (2007). Modulation of neuronal interactions through neuronal synchronization. Science 316, 1609–1612 10.1126/science.1139597 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]