Skip to main content
Journal of Urban Health : Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine logoLink to Journal of Urban Health : Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine
. 2008 May 15;85(5):642–661. doi: 10.1007/s11524-008-9288-5

Do Neighborhood Economic Characteristics, Racial Composition, and Residential Stability Predict Perceptions of Stress Associated with the Physical and Social Environment? Findings from a Multilevel Analysis in Detroit

Amy J Schulz 1,, Shannon N Zenk 2, Barbara A Israel 1, Graciela Mentz 1, Carmen Stokes 3, Sandro Galea 1
PMCID: PMC2527427  PMID: 18481182

Abstract

As the body of evidence linking disparities in the health of urban residents to disparate social, economic and environmental contexts grows, efforts to delineate the pathways through which broader social and economic inequalities influence health have burgeoned. One hypothesized pathway connects economic and racial and ethnic inequalities to differentials in stress associated with social and physical environments, with subsequent implications for health. Drawing on data from Detroit, Michigan, we examined contributions of neighborhood-level characteristics (e.g., poverty rate, racial and ethnic composition, residential stability) and individual-level characteristics (e.g., age, gender) to perceived social and physical environmental stress. We found that neighborhood percent African American was positively associated with perceptions of both social and physical environmental stress; neighborhood percent poverty and percent Latino were positively associated with perceived physical environmental stress; and neighborhood residential stability was negatively associated with perceived social environmental stress. At the individual level, whites perceived higher levels of both social and physical environmental stress compared to African American residents of the same block groups, after accounting for other variables included in the models. Our findings suggest the importance of understanding and addressing contributions of neighborhood structural characteristics to perceptions of neighborhood stress. The consistency of the finding that neighborhood racial composition and individual-level race influence perceptions of both social and physical environments suggests the continuing importance of understanding the role played by structural conditions and by personal and collective histories that vary systematically by race and ethnicity within the United States.

Keywords: Perceived social environmental stress, Perceived physical environmental stress, Residential stability, Urban health

Introduction

Disentangling the social processes that shape urban environments and the health of their residents is central to understanding and addressing health inequalities.1,2 The burgeoning literature on neighborhood effects has contributed to our understanding of relationships between neighborhood characteristics (e.g., poverty level) and a wide range of social and health outcomes.38 The majority of these analyses have been conducted in urban or metropolitan neighborhoods and have established clear relationships between concentrated structural disadvantage and poor health outcomes.6,912

Attention has increasingly turned to understanding more clearly the pathways or processes through which unequal urban contexts are created, reproduced, and translated into differential health outcomes.1315 In this article, we examine one hypothesized pathway: differentials in stress associated with neighborhood contexts. Drawing on data from Detroit, Michigan, we examine contributions of neighborhood-level characteristics (e.g., poverty rate, residential stability) and individual-level characteristics (e.g., age, gender) to perceptions of stress associated with the social and physical environment. In addition, we consider the extent to which poverty in adjacent neighborhoods influence perceptions of social and physical environmental stress, above and beyond the immediate residential neighborhood. Finally, we consider the contributions of individual-level characteristics and the extent to which they are modified by neighborhood characteristics.

Background

Substantial evidence links structural inequalities to health and suggests that the disproportionate concentration of African Americans and Latinos in urban neighborhoods that have experienced economic divestment contribute to racial and ethnic health disparities.1618 Among the potential pathways linking structural inequalities to health disparities are differential exposures to adverse social and physical environments, combined with differential access to social and economic resources that promote health. While a number of studies have examined relationships between neighborhood characteristics and health, relatively few studies have examined the factors that contribute to perceptions of social and physical environmental stress. Below, we briefly review the literature on stress process models that provides the overarching conceptual framework for this study. We then examine the literature specific to the relationships of particular interest concerning neighborhood characteristics (e.g., poverty, racial, and ethnic composition, neighborhood stability), perceived stress associated with social and physical environments, and health.

Conceptual Models of Inequality, Stress, and Health Stress process models1925 conceptualize stress as a complex and dynamic process in which social and physical environmental conditions conducive to stress (or stressors) influence but do not fully determine perceptions of stress. Stressors, perceived stress (i.e., stressors perceived as bothersome or worrisome, or that result in physiological adaptational responses), behavioral or physiological responses (e.g., smoking, tenseness), and conditioning variables or protective factors (e.g., social support, personal control, physical activity) influence each other and long-term health outcomes (e.g., cardiovascular disease).20,26,27 These processes are conceptualized as dynamic and complex, influenced by both contextual and individual characteristics and histories. Thus, stress process models explicitly distinguish between stressors (conditions conducive to stress) and perceptions of stress, recognizing that both may influence health independently28,29 and that no one observed stressor is likely to have the same impact on all who encounter it.30,31 For example, differences in access to economic, educational, or social resources may influence perceptions of stress and behavioral or physiological responses, together shaping implications for health outcomes.19,2125,32

Neighborhood Characteristics, Stress, and Health Population-based evidence has established relationships between socioeconomic position (SEP), exposure to stressors, perceptions of stress, physiological responses, and mental and physical health.20,24,26,27,33,34 Several studies have shown that residents of neighborhoods with higher poverty rates encounter environments that are more conducive to some types of stressors compared to residents of more affluent neighborhoods,3540 and that these contexts are, in turn, associated with child development outcomes,13 self-rated health,4143 depression,12,44,45 physical functioning,42,46 and mortality.47 Adolescents and adults residing in communities with higher levels of poverty report higher levels of a variety of indicators of chronic stress.29,3538,4850 and perceptions of stress substantially mediate neighborhood-level variations in mental and physical health.5153

There is evidence that perceptions of neighborhoods are associated with racial and ethnic composition. Neighborhoods with higher concentrations of African Americans and Latinos have higher levels of physical environmental contaminants than economically comparable neighborhoods with higher concentrations of white residents54,55 thus potentially influencing perceptions of stress among residents. Perceptions of neighborhood conditions also appear to be influenced by subjective attributions associated with race and ethnicity. For example, a recent study of Chicago neighborhoods reported that neighborhood percent poverty, African American, and Latino, were each independently associated with perceptions of “neighborhood disorder” after adjusting for compositional effects.48 This study found that whites reported higher levels of neighborhood disorder than African Americans in the same neighborhood and that Latinos were more sensitive to changes in percent African American compared to whites. These findings are consistent with theoretical and conceptual models that emphasize the joint contributions of objective conditions (stressors) and subjective responses to those conditions in shaping perceived stress. They suggest that perceptions of social and physical environmental conditions are influenced by both structural conditions and by personal and collective histories that vary systematically by race and ethnicity within the United States.48,56,57

Residential Stability, Stress, and Health Neighborhoods with high levels of residential stability may be conducive to strong ties among residents, lower levels of perceived stress, and more positive health outcomes. Conversely, high levels of residential turnover can disrupt existing social networks, contribute to tensions between long term and newer residents, and increase vulnerability to citing of noxious land uses.54 These factors may influence perceptions of the neighborhood and ultimately health. Despite these hypothesized relationships, we are not aware of any empirical research that directly examines the effects of residential stability on perceptions of the neighborhood environment.

Evidence regarding relationships between residential stability and health is mixed, reflecting in part differences in measures and levels of analysis (e.g., the proportion of census tract residents who have moved versus individual change in residence in a given time period). There is some evidence that neighborhood residential stability may be protective of mental58 and physical52,59 health. Two recent studies found a positive association between neighborhood residential stability and mental well-being only for residents of affluent neighborhoods,5,60 and one found a negative relationship to mental health among an older adult population in an urban neighborhood.61 Together, this body of work suggests that neighborhood residential stability may be protective of health under some conditions or in some populations, but less positive or even detrimental in others. Better understanding relationships between residential stability and perceptions of neighborhood environments may help to clarify pathways and conditions under which residential stability contributes to health.

Research Questions We use multilevel, multivariate models to build on the extant literature in several important ways. First, we test the hypothesis that neighborhood percent poverty, percent African American and percent Latino are positively associated with perceptions of social and physical environmental stress, adjusting for individual-level characteristics. Second, we test the hypothesis that residential stability is negatively associated with perceived social and physical environmental stress, and if so, whether relationships are modified by neighborhood economic characteristics. Third, we test whether the mean poverty rate of adjacent neighborhoods is positively associated with perceived social and physical environmental stress. Fourth, we test the extent to which individual-level racial and ethnic differences in perceived social and physical environmental stress are mediated or moderated by neighborhood-level characteristics.

Data and Methods

Sample Data for this study are drawn from the Healthy Environments Partnership (HEP) community survey, one component of a community-based participatory research study conducted with a multiethnic sample of adults in Detroit, Michigan, and from 2000 census data. Academic, health care provider, and community-based organizational partners were actively involved in each step of the research process, including decisions about study design, implementation, methodology, interpretation, and dissemination of findings (see Acknowledgements for list of partners).62 The University of Michigan Institutional Review Board for Protection of Human Subjects approved the HEP study in January 2001.

The HEP survey is a stratified two-stage probability sample of occupied housing units designed for 1,000 completed interviews of adults aged 25 years and older in three areas of Detroit. The final study sample consisted of 919 face-to-face interviews: interviews were completed with 75% of households in which an eligible respondent was identified and 55% of households estimated to have an eligible respondent.63

Measures Dependent variables included indicators of perceived social and physical environmental stress, informed by focus groups conducted with neighborhood residents.29,62,64 Perceived social environmental stress was the mean of six items assessing the frequency with which the respondent indicated that each of the following were a problem in their neighborhood: gang activity, drug dealing or drug dealers, gunfire or shootings, prostitution, loitering or hanging around, or theft, vandalism, or arson. Response categories ranged from 1 = never to 5 = always (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83). Perceived physical environmental stress was the mean of seven items assessing agreement with the following statements: houses in my neighborhood are generally well maintained (reverse coded); there is heavy car or truck traffic in my neighborhood; my neighborhood has a lot of vacant lots or vacant houses; there is air pollution like diesel from trucks or pollution from factories or incinerators in my neighborhood; streets, sidewalks, and vacant lots in my neighborhood are kept clean of litter and dumping (reverse coded); there is a lot of loud noise from cars, motorcycles, music, neighbors, or airplanes in my neighborhood; and there is contaminated land in my neighborhood. Response categories ranged from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.69).

Individual-level independent variables included in the models were: age in years; gender (0 = male, 1 = female); marital status (0 = not currently married, 1 = currently married), number of people in the household, self-reported race and ethnicity (categorized as: African American; white; and Latino, including participants of any race and who reported Hispanic/Latino ethnicity. African American was used as the referent group), household income (<$10,000, $10,000–19,999, $20,000–34,999, >$35,000, with >$35,000 as the referent group); education (<12 years, 12 years, >12 years, with >12 as the referent group); labor force participation (0 = not currently working, 1 = currently working), number of years of residence in the neighborhood, and home ownership (0 = not a home owner; 1 = home owner).

Neighborhood-level independent variables included indicators of poverty, racial and ethnic composition, and residential stability of the census block groups (used as proxies for respondents’ neighborhoods). These data are drawn from the 2000 Census Summary Files 1 or 3. Respondents lived in 69 census block groups. Percent poverty, percent African American, and percent Latino were continuous variables defined as the percent of households with incomes below the poverty line, residents who identified their race as African American, and residents who identified their ethnicity as Latino, respectively. Residential stability was a continuous variable defined as the percent of census block group residents who had been living at the same address for 5 years or more. We also created a measure of the mean percent poverty in neighborhoods adjacent to (sharing a common border with) each neighborhood in which survey respondents resided.

Analysis Two-level weighted hierarchical generalized regression models were estimated using HLM 6.02 (Scientific Software International, Lincolnwood IL, 2005). Level 1 was the individual level; level 2 was the neighborhood. We first estimated unconditional random effect models to determine the percent of the total variability in perceived physical environmental stress and perceived social environmental stress explained at the neighborhood level. Next, we ran grand mean centered regression models to test the effects of neighborhood-level variables, accounting for individual-level compositional effects. Model 1 included neighborhood percent poverty, African American and Latino, adjusted for individual characteristics (age, gender, marital status, race and ethnicity, education, income, number of persons in the household, labor force participation, years of residence in the neighborhood, and home ownership) to test the hypothesis that neighborhood percent poverty, percent African American, and percent Latino are positively associated with perceptions of social and physical environmental stress. Model 2 added neighborhood residential stability to test the hypothesis that residential stability is negatively associated with perceived social and physical environmental stress, above and beyond the effects of neighborhood economic conditions, and racial and ethnic composition. To test the hypothesis that the mean poverty rate of adjacent neighborhoods is positively associated with perceived social and physical environmental stress, above and beyond the effects of neighborhood poverty, racial and ethnic composition, and residential stability, in model 3, we added the mean percent poverty in surrounding neighborhoods. We ran group mean centered models to test the hypothesis that individual-level characteristics predict perceptions of social and physical environmental stress within neighborhoods, accounting for neighborhood-level characteristics. Finally, we modeled interaction terms to test the hypothesis that: (1) effects of neighborhood residential stability are contingent on neighborhood poverty; and (2) individual-level racial differences are contingent upon neighborhood racial composition or poverty. All models are adjusted for sample weights for unequal probabilities of selection and to match the sample to Census 2000 population distributions for the study areas.

Results

Descriptive statistics illustrating study variables adjusted for the sample weights are shown in Table 1. Participants’ mean age was 46 years, 52% were female, 26% were currently married, and 57% were African American, 22% Latino, and 19% White. Thirty-three percent reported education beyond high school, and 23% reported annual household incomes of >$35K. There was an average of 2.8 persons per housing unit, the average years of residence in the neighborhood at the individual level was 18.5 years, and 65% of participants were currently in the labor force. The mean level of perceived physical environmental stress was 2.9, and the mean level of perceived social environmental stress was 2.7 (both variables on a five-point scale). Neighborhoods, on average, were 66.8% African American and 15.1% Latino. The mean poverty rate was 34.6 in the residential neighborhoods and 32.7 in the surrounding (adjacent) neighborhoods. The mean score for residential stability was 56.1% (i.e., 56% of neighborhood residents lived in the same house in which they had lived 5 years previously).

Table 1.

Descriptive tables of demographic variables

Individual-level variables N Percent Mean SD
Age 919 46.3 0.8
Gender
Male 287 47.7
Female 632 52.3
Marital status
Married 230 26.4
Not currently married 689 73.6
Race/ethnicity
Hispanic 182 22.2
White 199 18.8
Black 522 56.8
Other 16 2.3
Annual household income
<$10,000 250 27.3
$10,000-$19,999 238 26.0
$20,000–34,999 230 23.6
$35,000 or more 201 23.0
Education
<12 years 327 36.9
12 years 259 29.1
>12 years 321 32.8
Other 12 1.2
Number of members in HU 919 2.8 0.1
Length of residence in neighborhood 919 18.5 0.7
In labor force
No 339 35.1
Yes 580 64.9
Home owner
Yes 424 51.5
No 495 48.5
Perceived social environmental stress 919 2.7 0.1
Perceived physical environmental stress 919 2.9 0.0
Neighborhood-level variables
Percent poverty 69 34.6 12.6
Percent African American 69 66.8 36.0
Percent Latino 69 15.1 26.3
Residential Stability 69 56.1 14.3
Mean percent poverty, surrounding neighborhoods 69 32.7 7.7

Neighborhood Effects Based on results of fully unconditional models, 18% of the variability in perceived physical environmental stress and 14% of perceived social environmental stress was at the neighborhood level (not shown). Tables 2 and 3 present results for grand mean centered models testing neighborhood effects on perceived physical environmental stress and perceived social environmental stress, respectively. In grand mean centered models, neighborhood-level variables are interpreted as the effect of one unit of variation from the grand mean (across neighborhoods) for that variable.

Table 2.

Perceived physical environment stress regressed on neighborhood characteristics, adjusting for individual characteristics (grand mean centered models, individual-level variables not shown)

Parameter Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Est SE p value Est SE p-value Est SE p value
Intercept 3.131 0.058 <0.001 3.131 0.058 <0.001 3.128 0.059 <0.001
Neighborhood level
Percent poverty 0.017 0.006 0.003 0.018 0.006 0.003 0.011 0.006 0.063
Percent African American 0.006 0.003 0.009 0.009 0.003 0.005 0.008 0.003 0.013
Percent Latino 0.011 0.004 0.012 0.012 0.004 0.007 0.010 0.004 0.017
Residential stability −0.002 0.004 0.670 −0.004 0.005 0.441
Surrounding percent poverty (adjacent block groups) 0.019 0.011 0.085
Neighborhood-level variance 0.513 0.513 0.512
Individual-level variance 0.095 0.098 0.094
ICC 0.157 0.160 0.156

Table 3.

Perceived social environment stress regressed on neighborhood characteristics, adjusting for individual characteristics (grand mean centered models, individual-level variables not shown)

Neighborhood social environment
Parameter Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Est S.E. p-value Est S.E. p-value Est S.E. p-value
Intercept 2.674 0.073 <0.001 2.675 0.068 <0.001 2.673 0.067 <0.001
Neighborhood level
Percent poverty 0.004 0.007 0.588 0.005 0.007 0.418 0.001 0.007 0.919
Percent African American 0.010 0.004 0.016 0.013 0.004 0.004 0.012 0.004 0.006
Percent Latino 0.008 0.006 0.178 0.010 0.006 0.073 0.009 0.006 0.107
Residential stability -0.010 0.006 0.072 -0.012 0.006 0.050
Surrounding percent poverty (adjacent block groups) 0.014 0.012 0.235
Neighborhood-level variance 0.711 0.710 0.709
Individual-level variance 0.134 0.123 0.125
ICC 0.158 0.148 0.150

Perceived Physical Environmental Stress Results presented in Table 2 suggest that neighborhood percent poverty (p = .003), percent African American (p = .009), and percent Latino (p = .012) were each positively and independently associated with perceived physical environmental stress, above and beyond individual-level effects. These effects remained significant after adjusting for neighborhood residential stability (p = .670; model 2). Neighborhood percent African American and Latino remained significant after adjusting for mean percent poverty in surrounding neighborhoods (p = .085; model 3). Tests of interactions between neighborhood residential stability and neighborhood poverty rate were not significant (results not shown).

Perceived Social Environmental Stress Results shown in Table 3 indicate that percent African American was significantly associated with perceived social environmental stress (p = .016; model 1), adjusting for individual-level characteristics. After adjusting for residential stability (p = .072; model 2), the coefficient for percent African American increased (p = .004). Neighborhood percent poverty and percent Latino were not significant predictors of perceived social environmental stress, after adjusting for the other variables included in the models. After adjusting for the mean percent poverty in surrounding neighborhoods (p = .235), residential stability was negatively significantly associated with perceived social environmental stress (p = .050). The relationship between residential stability and perceived social environmental stress was not modified by neighborhood percent poverty (results not shown). To more clearly understand relationships between residential stability, percent African American, and mean poverty in surrounding neighborhoods, we regressed residential stability on percent African American and found a significant positive relationship (p < .001, results not shown). In other words, in neighborhoods with higher percent of African American residents, a larger proportion of residents had lived in their current household for 5 years or more. We also regressed residential stability on percent poverty in surrounding neighborhoods and found a significant positive relationship (p = .013, results not shown), with greater residential stability in neighborhoods with higher mean levels of poverty in surrounding block groups.

Individual Effects Tables 4 and 5 present individual-level effects, using group mean centered models for perceived physical and social environmental stress, respectively. For each dependent variable, model 1 shows group mean centered individual-level effects, unadjusted for neighborhood characteristics; model 2 includes neighborhood percent poverty, African American and Latino; and model 3 adds neighborhood residential stability. Individual-level effects did not change with the addition of surrounding neighborhood poverty (not shown). In group mean centered models, individual-level variables are interpreted in comparison to other residents of the same neighborhood. Thus, for example, a significant effect of race is interpreted as the effect of being white versus African American within the same neighborhood.

Table 4.

Perceived physical environmental stress regressed on individual-level variables, adjusted for neighborhood effects (group mean centered models, neighborhood-level variables not shown)

Perceived physical environmental stress
Parameter Individual-level variables only Model 1a Model 2b
Est SE p value Est SE p value Est SE p value
Intercept 3.076 0.044 <0.001 3.095 0.041 <0.001 3.094 0.041 <0.001
Individual level
Age −0.005 0.003 0.105 −0.005 0.003 0.108 −0.005 0.003 0.108
Female (male referent) −0.015 0.075 0.839 −0.017 0.075 0.816 −0.017 0.075 0.817
Marital status (not married ref) 0.053 0.082 0.518 0.054 0.082 0.513 0.054 0.082 0.512
Race/ethnicity (black ref)
White 0.474 0.107 <0.001 0.472 0.107 <0.001 0.472 0.107 <0.001
Hispanic/Latino 0.084 0.116 0.470 0.081 0.116 0.484 0.081 0.116 0.483
Education (>12 years referent)
<12 years −0.049 0.089 0.580 −0.048 0.089 0.489 −0.048 0.088 0.585
12 years −0.060 0.074 0.417 −0.067 0.074 0.369 −0.067 0.074 0.369
Income (>35K referent)
<10K 0.052 0.147 0.726 0.052 0.147 0.726 0.051 0.147 0.726
10–<20K 0.020 0.139 0.887 0.019 0.139 0.893 0.019 0.139 0.894
20–<35K 0.177 0.123 0.151 0.177 0.123 0.152 0.177 0.123 0.152
Number in household 0.001 0.027 0.968 0.001 0.027 0.973 0.001 0.027 0.973
In labor force (not in labor force ref) −0.061 0.098 0.538 −0.058 0.098 0.562 −0.058 0.098 0.562
Length of residence in neighborhood 0.001 0.003 0.658 0.001 0.003 0.673 0.001 0.003 0.674
Home owner (renter ref) −0.092 0.102 0.369 −0.094 0.102 0.362 −0.093 0.102 0.363
Neighborhood-level variance 0.514 0.513 0.514
Individual-level variance 0.084 0.068 0.070
ICC 0.140 0.117 0.120

aModel 1: adjusted for neighborhood percent poverty, percent African American and percent Latino

bModel 2: adjusted for neighborhood percent poverty, percent African American, percent Latino and residential stability

Table 5.

Perceived social environmental stress regressed on individual-level variables, adjusted for neighborhood effects (Group mean centered models, neighborhood-level variables not shown)

Perceived social environmental stress
Parameter Individual-level variables only Model 1a Model 2b
Est SE p value Est SE p value Est SE p value
Intercept 2.631 0.058 <0.001 2.658 0.051 <0.001 2.647 0.049 <0.001
Individual level
Age −0.009 0.004 0.028 −0.009 0.004 0.029 −0.009 0.004 0.029
Female (male referent) −0.269 0.098 0.007 −0.272 0.098 0.006 −0.272 0.098 0.006
Marital status (not married ref) −0.187 0.099 0.059 −0.188 0.099 0.056 −0.187 0.099 0.059
Race/ethnicity (black ref)
White 0.444 0.155 0.005 0.447 0.155 0.004 0.450 0.155 0.004
Hispanic/Latino 0.292 0.167 0.080 0.294 0.166 0.076 0.396 0.166 0.075
Education (>12 years referent)
<12 years −0.215 0.123 0.079 −0.213 0.123 0.083 −0.217 0.123 0.077
12 years −0.219 0.127 0.086 −0.225 0.127 0.075 −0.225 0.126 0.074
Income (>35K referent)
<10K −0.187 0.160 0.242 −0.187 0.160 0.243 −0.190 0.160 0.236
10–<20K −0.167 0.146 0.253 −0.167 0.146 0.254 −0.169 0.146 0.249
20–<35K −0.138 0.152 0.361 −0.139 0.152 0.362 −0.142 0.152 0.350
Number in household 0.016 0.029 0.587 0.016 0.029 0.595 0.016 0.029 0.595
In labor force (not in labor force ref) −0.064 0.141 0.652 −0.061 0.141 0.669 −0.061 0.141 0.673
Length of residence in neighborhood −0.003 0.004 0.460 −0.003 0.004 0.456 −0.003 0.004 0.450
Home owner (renter ref) −0.240 0.132 0.068 −0.244 0.132 0.065 −0.243 0.132 0.066
Neighborhood-level variance 0.714 0.713 0.712
Individual-level variance 0.125 0.090 0.088
ICC 0.149 0.112 0.110

aModel 1: adjusted for neighborhood percent poverty, percent African American and percent Latino

bModel 2: adjusted for neighborhood percent poverty, percent African American, percent Latino, and residential stability

As shown in Table 4, white participants reported significantly higher levels of perceived physical environmental stress compared with African American participants in the same neighborhoods (p < .001). This difference remains after controlling for the neighborhood characteristics described above. Results presented in Table 5 indicate that older (p = .028), female (p = .007), and African American (p = .005) participants reported lower levels of social environmental stress compared with those in the same neighborhood who were younger, male, and white (model 1). These differences remained significant after accounting for neighborhood-level variables (models 2 and 3). While Latinos reported higher levels of social environmental stress than African Americans (p = .080), this relationship was not statistically significant. Interaction terms testing whether relationships between individual-level race and perceived social and physical environmental stress were modified by neighborhood percent poverty, African American, or Latino were not significant (results not shown).

Discussion

Neighborhood Level Our findings that neighborhood percent African American was associated with perceptions of social and physical environmental stress, and that neighborhood percent poverty and Latino were associated with perceptions of physical environmental stress after accounting for individual-level variables, are consistent with results reported elsewhere.48 The significant relationship between percent African American and perceived social environmental stress was evident after accounting for neighborhood residential stability, suggesting that the relationship between percent African American and perceived social environmental stress is suppressed until the effects of residential stability are accounted for.61,65 Said differently, the protective effects of greater residential stability in neighborhoods with a higher proportion of African Americans masked the relationship between percent African American and perceived social environmental stress, until those effects were specified. In contrast to others,48 we do not find a significant association of percent poverty or Latino with perceptions of the social environment, although these associations were in the expected direction. These differences may reflect limitations of statistical power, or differences in measures or study samples, discussed in greater detail below.

We found partial support for the hypothesis that neighborhood residential stability is associated with perceptions of the neighborhood environment, with perceived social but not physical environmental stress declining with increasing residential stability. Furthermore, the protective effects of neighborhood residential stability were not significant until we accounted for mean poverty level in surrounding neighborhoods. The relationship between residential stability and perceived social environmental stress was not modified by poverty level in the neighborhood of residence. The finding of a protective effect of residential stability is consistent with the hypothesis that high levels of turnover within neighborhoods may disrupt existing social networks and contribute to tensions between residents who have lived there for longer periods of time and newer residents. If, as some have suggested,54 residential instability influences physical environments by decreasing residents’ capacity to resist citing of noxious land uses, the absence of a significant relationship between residential stability and physical environmental stress may reflect the cross-sectional nature of our data. Such effects may be more visible in panel or longitudinal analyses.

Finally, although relationships are in the expected direction, relationships between mean percent poverty in surrounding neighborhoods and perceived social or physical environmental stress were not significant. The relationship between residential stability and neighborhood social environmental stress was significant only after accounting for lagged poverty in surrounding neighborhoods, and a similar, although not significant, effect is seen for neighborhood physical environmental stress. Given that relatively few studies have examined the effects of adjacent neighborhoods, the mixed results reported in the literature to date, and our finding of a suppressor effect of mean poverty in surrounding neighborhoods on residential stability, additional studies examining this question in neighborhoods with a wider range of poverty characteristics would be beneficial.

Individual Level Our finding that whites report significantly higher levels of both perceived social and physical environmental stress compared with African American residents of the same neighborhoods adds to a body of evidence that race influences perceptions of contextual characteristics. These findings are consistent with results reported for a Chicago-based sample48 which also accounted for observed indicators of the environment, above and beyond neighborhood, and individual-level characteristics included here. Our findings extend those reported elsewhere by demonstrating that racial differences persist after accounting for neighborhood residential stability and concentration of poverty in surrounding neighborhoods. While relatively few studies reported to date have examined this issue, the consistency of the finding of race differences in perceptions of the social and physical environment after accounting for a range of neighborhood and individual characteristics, with different samples and comparable but not identical measures of neighborhood physical and social conditions, suggests the resilience of these relationships.

Limitations There are several limitations of this analysis. The data are cross-sectional, and hence, we cannot definitively determine the direction of associations between variables. Our model posits a primary direction of association, with for example, higher neighborhood poverty resulting in heightened perceptions of physical and social environmental stress. While the hypothesized direction of effects is supported by the extant literature, there may be some reciprocal effects. For example, residents who perceive environments as unduly stressful may move out of the neighborhood, with the result that we only record the somewhat lower self-reports of stress among those who remain. Such a selection process would bias the findings reported here in a conservative direction. In addition, we have already noted that some relationships, such as those between residential stability and perceived physical environmental stress, may emerge over time. An important consideration for future analyses will be the availability of longitudinal data to further disentangle the direction and dynamic nature of these relationships.

The addition of self-reported data for perceived physical and social environmental stress has both strengths and limitations. On the one hand, self reports of stress associated with physical and social environments are subjective, and may be influenced by a variety of factors, including objective conditions, personal and collective histories, and life experiences. Stress process models emphasize the importance of assessing individuals’ perceptions to capture the subjective nature of these phenomena, with the understanding that individuals act or respond on the basis of their perceptions. On the other hand, indicators of stress used here may be subject to “same source bias” in that they were derived from the same survey instrument as the individual-level independent variables used in the analysis. However, the significant individual-level predictors of perceived social and physical environmental stress in these models (age, marital status, gender, race, and ethnicity) are relatively stable, minimizing this potential risk. The addition of objective or independently assessed indicators of social and physical environmental conditions would extend the analyses reported here, allowing us to disentangle the contributions of variations in observed indicators and subjective responses to those conditions (see Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW for an example). 48 Even without such controls, however, the use of group mean centered models for these comparisons helps to reduce this risk, comparing individuals who live in close proximity to each other.

Statistically insignificant results for some neighborhood variables may be due, in part, to the relatively modest sample size (919 individuals nested within 69 neighborhoods). The mean poverty rate for the study neighborhoods (34.6%, SD = 12.6, min = 7.8, max = 63.1) is higher than mean neighborhood poverty rate for the city of Detroit as a whole (28.1%, SD = 14.2). It is also high compared to the national mean (12.7%)66 as well as those reported in similar studies.13,48 Our failure to reject the null hypothesis that poverty is associated with perceived social environmental stress and that poverty in adjacent neighborhoods is associated with either measure of perceived environmental stress, may reflect the relatively modest sample size and compressed range of economic variance.

The mean percent African American for block groups included in this analysis was 67% (SD = 36.0, min = 0.0, max = 100.0). While this mean is somewhat lower than the mean percent African American at the block group level for Detroit city as a whole (77.1%, SD = 27.3), it is substantially higher than the national population mean of 12.7% African American.66 Despite the relatively high proportion of African Americans in this sample, our findings are consistent with those reported by Sampson and Raudenbush in a Chicago-based sample with mean percent African American of 36% (SD = 42). However, further research with block groups reflecting a more nationally representative sample will be important to examine the extent to which the findings reported here are applicable to neighborhoods with a broader distribution of racial and ethnic groups.

The mean level of residential stability within block groups included in this analysis (56.1%, SD = 14.3, min = 21.5, max = 87.9) is fairly comparable to the mean for Detroit city (59.6%, SD = 12.9) and to the national mean (54%).66 The mean percent Latino at the block group level for our sample (15.1%, SD = 26.3, min = 0.0, max = 83.7) is higher than the Detroit city block group-level mean (5.6%, SD = 14.6) and comparable to the national population mean of 15%.66 As noted above, these differences may influence our findings, and future studies which include neighborhoods that are more nationally representative, and with larger sample sizes, would help to confirm these results. However, despite these differences, the results reported here are generally consistent with those reported elsewhere in the literature, as described in the following section.

Implications and Concluding Comments The findings reported here contribute to our understanding of relationships between structural conditions (e.g., economic conditions that create areas of concentrated poverty, racial segregation) and residents’ perceptions of social and physical environmental stress associated with their neighborhoods. They extend a growing body of evidence linking neighborhood characteristics with a wide range of social and health indicators, over and above the effects of the composition of neighborhood residents. These findings reported here suggest the importance of understanding structural conditions such as concentration of poverty and residential stability, in conjunction with racial and ethnic composition of neighborhoods, and their relationships to perceptions of stress associated with neighborhood environments.

Our findings also suggest dynamic relationships between structural conditions within neighborhoods. The higher levels of residential stability in neighborhoods with proportionately more African American residents may reflect strong neighborhood ties. Conversely, they may reflect suppressed mobility due to limited alternative housing options, an undesirable housing market that limits movement of new residents into the neighborhoods, or some combination of these factors. The positive relationship between residential stability and mean poverty in surrounding neighborhoods may suggest the latter mechanisms. Future efforts to disentangle the mechanisms that account for relationships between residential stability, racial composition, and perceptions of the neighborhood environment, are necessary to shed additional light on the mixed findings reported in the literature on residential stability and health.

Our finding that neighborhood racial composition is associated with perceptions of both social and physical environmental stress, above and beyond the effects of poverty and residential stability, is consistent with a growing body of literature that suggests the critical role of racial composition and racial attitudes or attributions in shaping contemporary urban contexts. Our finding of racial differences in perceived environmental stress, after accounting for neighborhood characteristics, is consistent with findings reported elsewhere indicating differences between white and African American residents’ perceptions of neighborhood characteristics48,67,68 and neighborhood desireability.67,69,70 These findings join a body of literature suggesting that negative racial stereotypes shape perceptions of neighborhoods,67,71 and that among whites, neighborhoods with higher proportions of African American residents are associated with heightened perceptions of crime and fear of victimization, above and beyond actual crime rates.68,72,73 Together, these findings lend credibility to the suggestion that the personal and collective histories of whites and African Americans in the United States systematically influence interpretations and meanings associated with neighborhood characteristics.48,7476 These differential interpretations or perceptions must be considered seriously as contributors to white flight from urban neighborhoods48,70,74,75 reproducing patterns of racial segregation that define many urban neighborhoods and their suburban surroundings. Our finding that residential stability is negatively associated with perceptions of social environmental stress suggests the importance of understanding the potential protective effects of efforts to stabilize urban environments.

Finally, the findings reported here suggest the importance of understanding not only contemporary characteristics of neighborhoods, but the social histories that influence racial and ethnic differences in perceptions or interpretations of those characteristics. The main effects of neighborhood racial composition on perceived social and physical environmental stress across racial and ethnic groups, combined with the persistent difference between white and African American residents of the same block group, suggests the urgency of understanding how racial and ethnic categories and the meanings associated with them influence residents’ perceptions of neighborhoods and the actions that they take based on those perceptions.76 Race is a fundamental category inextricably tied to histories of inequality in the United States, operating through negative racial stereotypes and prejudices.71,76 It is therefore perhaps not surprising that perceptions of stress associated with neighborhood environments would be associated with neighborhood racial composition. Understanding and addressing the factors that influence urban environments and the health of their residents will require careful attention to both structural conditions that differentially affect racial and ethnic groups in the United States, and histories of race and racism that reflect and continue to drive those inequalities.

Acknowledgements

The Healthy Environments Partnership (www.hepdetroit.org) is affiliated with the Detroit Community-Academic Urban Research Center (www.sph.umich.edu/urc). We thank the members of the HEP Steering Committee for their contributions to the work presented here, including representatives from Brightmoor Community Center, Detroit Department of Health and Wellness Promotion, Detroit Hispanic Development Corporation, Friends of Parkside, Henry Ford Health System, and the University of Michigan School of Public Health and Survey Research Center. HEP is funded by the National Institute of Environmental Health Science (NIEHS), no. R01 ES10936 and no. R01 ES014234. The results presented here are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of NIEHS. Finally, we thank Sue Andersen for her assistance with the preparation of this manuscript.

References

  • 1.Galea S, Freudenberg N, Vlahov D. Cities and population health. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60(5):1017–1033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 2.Galea S, Vlahov D. Urban health: evidence, challenges, and directions. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26:341–365. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 3.Zenk S, Schulz AJ, Israel BA, James SA, Bao S, Wilson ML. Fruit and vegetable access differs by community racial composition and socioeconomic position in Detroit, Michigan. Ethn Dis. 2006;16:275–280. [PubMed]
  • 4.Zenk S, Schulz AJ, Israel BA, James SA, Bao S, Wilson ML. Neighborhood racial composition, neighborhood poverty, and supermarket accessibility in metropolitan Detroit. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(4):660–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 5.Ross CE, Reynolds JR, Geis KJ. The contingent meaning of neighborhood stability for residents’ psychological well-being. Am Sociol Rev. 2000;65(4):581–597. [DOI]
  • 6.Diez-Roux AV, Merkin SS, Arnett D, et al. Neighborhood of residence and incidence of coronary heart disease. N Engl J Med. 2001;345(2):99–106. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 7.Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. Future Child. 1997;7(2):55–71. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 8.Boardman JD, Finch BK, Ellison CG, Williams DR, Jackson JS. Neighborhood disadvantage, stress, and drug use among adults. J Health Soc Behav. 2001;42(2):151–165. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 9.Diez-Roux A, Link BG, Northridge ME. A multilevel analysis of income inequality and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Soc Sci Med. 2000;50(5):673–687. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 10.Stafford M, Marmot M. Neighborhood deprivation and health: does it affect us all equally. Int J Epidemiol. 2003;32:357–366. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 11.Williams DR, Collins C. Racial residential segregation: a fundamental cause of racial disparities in health. Public Health Rep. 2001;116:404–416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 12.Ross CE. Neighborhood disadvantage and adult depression. J Health Soc Behav. 2000;41(2):177–187. [DOI]
  • 13.Caughy M, Hayslett-McCall KL, O’Campo P. No neighborhood is an island: incorporating distal neighborhood effects into multilevel studies of child developmental competence. Health Place. 2007;13:788–798. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 14.Krieger N, Waterman PD, Chen JT, Soobader M, Subramanian SV. Monitoring socioeconomic inequalities in sexually transmitted infections, tuberculosis, and violence: Geocoding and choice of area-based socioeconomic measures—the public health disparities geocoding project. Public Health Rep. 2003;118(3):240–260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 15.Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Gannon-Rowley T. Assessing “neighborhood effects”: social processes and new directions in research. Annu Rev Sociol. 2002;28:443–478. [DOI]
  • 16.Anderson RT, Sorlie PD, Backlund E, Johnson NJ, Kaplan GA. Mortality effects of community socioeconomic status. Epidemiology. 1997;8(1):42–47. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 17.Diez-Roux AV, Nieto FJ. Epidemiology, clinical science and beyond. Epidemiology. 1997;8(4):459–461. [PubMed]
  • 18.Morland K, Wing S, Diez-Roux A, Poole C. Neighborhood characteristics associated with the location of food stores and food service places. Am J Prev Med. 2002;22(1):23–29. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 19.Baker EA, Israel BA, Schurman SJ. The integrated model: implications for worksite health promotion and occupational health and safety practice. Health Educ Q. 1996;23(2):175–190. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 20.French JRP Jr, Kahn RL. A programmatic approach to studying the industrial environment and mental health. J Soc Issues. 1962;18:1–48.
  • 21.House JS. Work Stress and Social Support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1981.
  • 22.Katz D, Kahn RL. The Social Psychology of Organizations. 2nd edn. New York: Wiley; 1978.
  • 23.Israel BA, Schurman SJ, House JS. Action research on occupational stress: involving workers as researchers. Int J Health Serv. 1989;19(1):135–155. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 24.Israel BA, Schurman SJ. Social support, control and the stress process. In: Glanz K, Lewis FM, Rimer BK, eds. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research and Practice. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1990:187–215.
  • 25.Israel BA, Checkoway BN, Schulz AJ, Zimmerman MA. Health education and community empowerment: conceptualizing and measuring perceptions of individual, organizational, and community control. Health Educ Q. 1994;21(2):149–170. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 26.Selye H. The Stress of Life. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1956.
  • 27.Seeman TE, Singer BH, Rowe JW, Horwitz RI, McEwen BS. Price of adaptation-allostatic load and its health consequences: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Arch Intern Med. 1997;157(19):2259–2268. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 28.Thomas W, Thomas D. Situations defined as real are real in their consequences. In: Stone GP, Farberman HA, eds. Social Psychology Through Symbolic Interaction. Waltham, MA: Xerox College Publishing; 1970:154–155.
  • 29.Israel BA, Schulz AJ, Estrada-Martinez L, et al. Engaging urban residents in assessing neighborhood environments and their implications for health. J Urban Health. 2006;83(3):523–539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 30.Boehmer TK, Hoehner CM, Deshpande AD, Brennan Ramirez LK, Brownson RC. Perceived and observed neighborhood indicators of obesity among urban adults. Int J Obes. 2007;31(6):968–977. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 31.Schulz AJ, Williams DR, Israel BA, Lempert LB. Racial and spatial relations as fundamental determinants of health in Detroit. Milbank Q. 2002;80(4):677–707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 32.Strauss A, Corbin J. Basics of Qualitative Research: Grounded Theory Procedures and Techniques. 2nd edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1998.
  • 33.Karasek RA, Baker D, Marxer F, Ahlbom A, Theorell T. Job decision latitude, job demands and cardiovascular disease: a prospective study of Swedish men. Am J Public Health. 1981;71(7):694–705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 34.Geronimus AT, Hicken M, Keene D, Bound J. “Weathering” and age patterns of allostatic load scores among blacks and whites in the United States. Am J Public Health. 2006;96(5):826–833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 35.Allison KW, Burton L, Marshall S, et al. Life experiences among urban adolescents: examining the role of context. Child Dev. 1999;70(4):1017–1029. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 36.Aneshensel CS, Sucoff CA. The neighborhood context of adolescent mental health. J Health Soc Behav. 1996;37(4):293–310. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 37.Fang J, Madhavan S, Bosworth W, Alderman MH. Residential segregation and mortality in New York City. Soc Sci Med. 1998;47(4):469–476. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 38.Schulz AJ, Israel BA, Williams DR, Parker EA, James SA. Social inequalities, stressors and self-reported health status among African American and white women in the Detroit metropolitan area. Soc Sci Med. 2000;51:1639–1653. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 39.Schulz AJ, Northridge ME. Social determinants of health and environmental health promotion. Health Educ Behav. 2004;31(4):455–471. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 40.Maantay J. Zoning, equality, and public health. Am J Public Health. 2001;91(7):1033–1041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 41.Wen M, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Objective and perceived neighborhood environment, individual SES and psychosocial factors, and self-rated health: an analysis of older adults in Cook County, Illinois. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63(10):2575–2590. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 42.Steptoe A, Feldman PJ. Neighborhood problems as sources of chronic stress: development of a measure of neighborhood problems, and associations with socioeconomic status and health. Ann Behav Med. 2001;23(3):177–185. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 43.Franzini L, Caughy M, Spears W, Esquer MEF. Neighborhood economic conditions, social processes, and self rated health in low income neighborhoods in Texas: a multilevel latent variables model. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61(6):1135–1150. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 44.Latkin CA, Curry AD. Stressful neighborhoods and depression: a prospective study of the impact of neighborhood disorder. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(1):34–44. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 45.Cutrona CE, Wallace G, Wesner KA. Neighborhood characteristics and depression: an examination of stress processes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2006;15(4):188–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 46.Feldman PJ, Steptoe A. How neighborhoods and physical functioning are related: the roles of neighborhood socioeconomic status, perceived neighborhood strain, and individual health risk factors. Ann Behav Med. 2004;27(2):91–99. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 47.Cohen DA, Farley TA, Mason K. Why is poverty unhealthy. Soc Sci Med. 2003;57(9):1631–1641. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 48.Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW. The social structure of seeing disorder. Soc Psychol Q. 2004;67:319–342.
  • 49.Schulz AJ, Israel BA, Zenk S, et al. Psychosocial stress and social support as mediators of relationships between income, length of residence and depressive symptoms among African American women on Detroit’s east side. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62(2):510–522. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 50.Schulz AJ, House JS, Israel BA, et al. Relational pathways between socioeconomic position and cardiovascular risk in a multiethnic urban sample: complexities and their implications for improving health in economically disadvantaged populations. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 51.Agyemang C, van Hooijdonk C, Wendel-Vos W, Lindeman E, Stronks K, Droomers M. The association of neighborhood level social stressors and self rated health in Amsterdam, The Netherlands. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61:1042–1049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 52.Boardman JD. Stress and physical health: the role of neighborhoods as mediating and moderating mechanisms. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58(12):2473–2483. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 53.Yen IH, Yelin E, Katz P, Eisner MD, Blanc PD. Impact of perceived neighborhood problems on change in asthma-related health outcomes between baseline and follow-up. Health Place. 2008;14(3):468–477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 54.Pastor M Jr, Sadd JL, Hipp J. Which came first? Toxic facilities, minority move-in, and environmental justice. J Urban Affairs. 2001;23(1):1–21. [DOI]
  • 55.Gee GC, Payne-Sturges DC. Environmental health disparities: a framework integrating psychosocial and environmental concepts. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112(17):1645–1653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 56.Williams DR, Jackson JS. Race/ethnicity and the 2000 census: recommendations for African American and other black populations in the United States. Am J Public Health. 2000;90(11):1728–1730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 57.Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: socioeconomic status, stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol. 1997;2(3):335–351. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 58.Silver E, Mulvey EP, Swanson JW. Neighborhood structural characteristics and mental disorder: Faris and Dunham revisited. Soc Sci Med. 2002;55(8):1457–1470. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 59.Larson A, Bell M, Young AF. Clarifying the relationships between health and residential mobility. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59(10):2149–2160. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 60.Browning CR, Cagney KA. Moving beyond poverty: neighborhood structure, social processes, and health. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(4):552–571. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 61.Aneshensel CS, Wight RG, Miller-Martinez D, Botticello AL, Karlamangla AS, Seeman TE. Urban neighborhoods and depressive symptoms among older adults. J Gereontol B Physocol Sci Soc Sc. 2007;62(1):S52–S59. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 62.Schulz AJ, Zenk S, Kannan S, Israel BA, Koch MA, Stokes C. Community-based participatory approach to survey design and implementation: The Health Environments Partnership Survey. In: Israel BA, Eng E, Schulz AJ, Parker E, eds. Methods for Conducting Community-Based Participatory Research for Health. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2005:107–127.
  • 63.Schulz AJ, Kannan S, Dvonch JT, et al. Social and physical environments and disparities in risk for cardiovascular disease: The Healthy Environments Partnership conceptual model. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113(12):1817–1825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 64.Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997;277(5328):918–924. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 65.MacKinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prev Sci. 2000;1(4):173–181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 66.U.S. Census Bureau. USA People Quickfacts. www.Quickfacts.census.gov/gfd/states/00000.html. Accessed on April 2, 2008.
  • 67.Krysan M. Community undesirability in black and white: examining racial residential segregation through community perceptions. Soc Probl. 2002;49(4):521–543. [DOI]
  • 68.Chiricos T, McEntire R, Gertz M. Perceived racial and ethnic composition of neighborhood and perceived risk of crime. Soc Probl. 2001;48(3):322–340. [DOI]
  • 69.Krysan M, Farley R. The residential preferences of blacks: do they explain persistent segregation. Social Forces. 2002;80(3):937–980. [DOI]
  • 70.Krysan M. Whites who say they’d flee: who are they and why should they leave. Demography. 2002;39(4):675–696. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 71.Zubrinsky CL, Bobo L. Prismatic metropolis: race and residential segregation in the City of the Angels. Soc Sci Res. 1996;25(4):335–374. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 72.St. John C, Heald-Moore T. Racial prejudice and fear of criminal victimization by strangers in public settings. Sociol Inquiry. 1996;66(3):267–284. [DOI]
  • 73.St. John C, Heald-Moore T. Fear of black strangers. Soc Sci Res. 1995;24:262–280. [DOI]
  • 74.Farley R, Steeh C, Jackson T, Krysan M, Reeves K. Continued racial residential segregation in Detroit: ‘Chocolate city, vanilla suburbs’ revisted. J Hous Res. 1993;4(1):1–38.
  • 75.Farley R, Steeh C, Krysan M, Jackson T, Reeves K. Stereotypes and segregation: Neighborhoods in the Detroit area. Am J Sociol. 1994;100(3):750–780. [DOI]
  • 76.Schwalbe M, Godwin S, Holden D, Schrock D, Thompson S, Wolkomir M. Generic processes in the reproduction of inequality: an interactionist analysis. Soc Forces. 2000;79(2):419–452. [DOI]

Articles from Journal of Urban Health : Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine are provided here courtesy of New York Academy of Medicine

RESOURCES