Abstract
In neurons, spike timing is determined by integration of synaptic potentials in delicate concert with intrinsic properties. Although the integration time is functionally crucial, it remains elusive during network activity. While mechanisms of rapid processing are well documented in sensory systems, agility in motor systems has received little attention. Here we analyze how intense synaptic activity affects integration time in spinal motoneurons during functional motor activity and report a 10-fold decrease. As a result, action potentials can only be predicted from the membrane potential within 10 ms of their occurrence and detected for less than 10 ms after their occurrence. Being shorter than the average inter-spike interval, the AHP has little effect on integration time and spike timing, which instead is entirely determined by fluctuations in membrane potential caused by the barrage of inhibitory and excitatory synaptic activity. By shortening the effective integration time, this intense synaptic input may serve to facilitate the generation of rapid changes in movements.
Introduction
Spike timing in nerve cells is determined by temporal integration of synaptic potentials and intrinsic response properties. However, little is known about the timescale of this integration during functional network activity and how it is affected by synaptic events. In the absence of synaptic input the spike afterhyperpolarization (AHP) determines spike timing during repetitive firing. In motoneurons (MNs), the frequency range of this firing is well suited for force regulation in the muscle fibers they innervate [1]. This suggests that main role of AHP is temporal filtering that converts the continuous asynchronous synaptic bombardment to a regular output discharge of action potentials. Furthermore, firing maintained by AHP and other slow intrinsic properties is also appealing because it is a metabolically inexpensive way of shaping the spike patterns to suit particular functions, e.g. spinal motor rhythms [2]–[5].
On the other hand, the AHP and other slow intrinsic properties would impede rapidly changing motor responses and it is not known how resilient they are to a noisy background of synaptic activity. Recent evidence suggests that intrinsic response properties may be shunted by synaptic conductance in cortical and sub-cortical networks [6]–[10]. In the spinal cord of the adult turtle, scratch motor network activity is associated with a dramatic rise in conductance and in fluctuations of the membrane potential (Vm) in both MNs and interneurons during spiking [11], [12]. This is due to a concurrent intense inhibitory and excitatory synaptic activity [13]. Under these conditions of high synaptic conductance, the temporal resolution is predicted to be enhanced [14]–[16] and the role of slow intrinsic properties becomes less obvious. Surprisingly few experimental studies have explored this interplay between high synaptic conductance, AHP and temporal integration in active networks.
For this reason, we have conducted experiments on spinal motoneurons embedded in a functionally active network during fictive motor behavior. In earlier studies the conductance increase in motoneurons during fictive locomotor and scratch network activity was first measured in vivo in the cat [17], [18] and in the turtle [12]. The isolated spinal cord-carapace preparation from the turtle [19] offers uniquely stable recording conditions in which intrinsic and synaptic conductance changes during network activity can be measured against a background of very low leak conductance [11], [13], [20]. This allows us for the first time to quantify the relative importance of active and passive intrinsic properties and the dynamics of synaptic input for spike timing during functional network activity.
We measured the effective integration time in MNs during network activity using a novel statistical approach that quantified the Vm-fluctuations before and after the action potential. Three temporal features were characterized: the membrane time-constant, effective synaptic integration time (eSIT) and the effective recovery time (eRT). We define the eSIT as the time it takes to sum up enough synaptic input to cause a spike. The eRT is defined as the time it takes for the Vm-distribution following a spike to return to the pre-spike condition, i.e. how long it takes the cell to “forget” that a spike has occurred. We report eRT as short as 4 ms during network activity, which is more than a 10-fold decrease compared with quiescent network. Our results show that even prominent intrinsic response properties like the AHP are severely attenuated concurrent with increase in synaptic conductance. For this reason, the contribution of synaptic activity and active membrane properties to network dynamics can only be captured by a conductance-based model [8], [16], [21], [22] (Figure 1). We conclude that a shortening of the recovery time and integration time of motoneurons cause an increase in the temporal resolution of the motor system during activity, which we suggest as a mechanism to facilitate rapidly changing movements.
Results
The question we ask is how the surge in conductance from intense synaptic bombardment during network activity modifies intrinsic properties exemplified by the spike AHP and shrinks response time and integration time of the neuron. The sensitivity of spike generation and spike pattern to conductance is illustrated in simulations in a simple model (Figure 1). In the absence of synaptic input the regular firing during maintained depolarization in the model neuron was determined by the AHP (red trace in Figure 1A). Synaptic input simulated as current noise, i.e. linear summation of synaptic potentials, resulted in greater variability in spike timing, but did not change the averaged AHP (black trace Figure 1A, B). Thus, current-based synaptic input fluctuations changed neither total conductance nor neuronal integration time. However, when synaptic activity was modeled more realistically as a noisy conductance increase, spike timing was strongly influenced by the stochastic fluctuations in Vm (blue trace, Figure 1A, B), the AHP was severely attenuated (arrow in Figure 1B) and the integration time was shortened [22], [23]. Thus, the simple simulation showed that a fluctuating current is not likely to affect the integration time or reduce the importance of the AHP whereas intense and fluctuating synaptic conductance is.
For the purpose of making these statements applicable to quantitative evaluation of experimental data, we developed statistics to capture the temporal features of Vm before and after the spike. We define two measures linked to the superposition of spikes (Figure 1C and D). First, the effective synaptic integration time (eSIT), defined as the time period prior to spikes during which fluctuations in membrane potential showed a significant depolarizing trend compared with a baseline template (Figure 1D). Likewise, we define the effective recovery time (eRT), as the time it takes to regain pre-spike Vm-levels. Notice that both eRT and eSIT are absent in current noise model whereas they are present in the conductance noise model (cf. Figure 1 C and D).
To evaluate whether the neuronal integration time and AHP are substantially affected during intense network activity we measured eSIT and eRT in MN during scratching. The data set consisted of more than 10.000 spikes in 185 scratch epochs in 17 MNs. The analysis is organized as follows. First, we estimated the conductance increase in a sub-sample of MN to confirm that strong synaptic components were present (Figure 2). Next, we applied our statistics to measure the eSIT and eRT and estimated the effective membrane time-constant from Vm-fluctuations at different levels of synaptic activity. Since intensity of synaptic activity may vary among cells, we compared the eRT with indicators of input, i.e. the effective membrane time constant as well as the smallest inter-spike interval (ISI) for each MN. Finally, since the functional expression of AHP accumulation in motoneurons is spike frequency adaptation (SFA) [4], [24], we also tested for adaptation during each cycle of the scratch epochs.
Synaptic conductance
A scratch epoch was induced by rhythmic cutaneous stimulation of the skin in the hind-limb pocket and the concurrent synaptic conductance (Figure 2A, B) was estimated from the voltage deflections to injected current pulses (Figure 2C–F) for a subset of MN. Conductance due to action potentials is a potential source of error in these measurements [25]. We therefore avoided spikes by injecting a steady hyperpolarizing current (typically −2 nA) or selected a part of the scratch episode without spikes. The pair of spikes in Figure 2F was left in for illustration, but the sweep was not included in the final estimate. Even with this conservative approach, the estimated conductance increased 2–5 times during scratching (table 1 and Figure 2G)[9]. We attribute this increase to balanced inhibitory and excitatory synaptic input [13] and conclude that temporal processing in MNs could be affected by synaptic conductance during network activity.
Table 1. Total input conductance for 5 MN during both quiescence and active network.
Motoneuron number | Gquiescence [nS] | Gactive [nS] | Fraction [100%] |
2 | 73 | 320 | 438 |
3 | 45 | 181 | 405 |
10 | 71 | 224 | 314 |
13 | 20 | 40 | 200 |
14 | 55 | 258 | 470 |
Effective synaptic integration time
We first considered the Vm-statistics prior to spikes. The time of the earliest statistical sign of depolarization prior to action potentials we dub “the effective synaptic integration time” (eSIT). This depolarization may be caused by a rise in excitatory conductance or a fall in inhibitory conductance. For each cell, the eSIT was estimated by comparing a template distribution of Vm with distributions of Vm as a function of time prior to action potentials, as in figure 1. The comparison was attained using a KS-two-sample test at each point in time (method, Figure 3). The template distribution was chosen at a time, ttemplate, well before the spikes (arrows Figures 3A and B). The eSIT did not depend systematically on the choice of ttemplate as long as it was at least 10 ms prior to the spike (Figure 3C) even though the number of samples in the distribution decreased (Figure 3D).
Memory of a spike–eRT
The same statistical test was used to evaluate the impact of a spike by comparing the Vm-distribution after action potentials with template distributions well before the spike (arrow Figure 3B). In the graph of the KS-test, the gray area represents rejection of the null hypothesis that the Vm-distribution before and after the spike was statistically similar to the template distribution. When the Vm-distribution after the spike was indistinguishable from the template distribution, we considered the impact of a spike to have ceased (graph of P-values, Figures 3A and B). Both eRT and eSIT had no clear dependence on ttemplate earlier than ∼20 ms (Figure 3C). Since the number of spikes in the distribution decreased with ttemplate (Figure 3D) the eRT estimate should not be performed with a high ttemplate. We chose to calculate the mean eRT over the range 20 ms<ttemplate<40 ms.
Impact of τeff and AHP on recovery time
Right after the occurrence of an action potential Vm is hyperpolarized. The time it takes for Vm to re-polarize back to the level prior to the spike depends on the AHP and on the passive effective time-constant of the membrane, which we refer to as τeff. In our attempt to sort out which part of the re-polarization is due to the passive decay of Vm and which is due to the cessation of AHP conductance, it is important to estimate τeff during network activity. The increase in total conductance will also diminish the relative importance of the AHP conductance and the AHP will appear shorter. To quantify the vestige of the AHP under different levels of input conductance we define the effective After-Hyperpolarisation Period (eAHP). This period represents the time it takes until the AHP ceases to affect the Vm trajectory. If there is no overlap between the passive membrane decay and the eAHP, the eRT is just the sum of τeff and eAHP, while if there is overlap eRT will be less than the sum:
It is important to emphasize the eRT also represents the upper bound on both eAHP and τeff, i.e. eRT≥eAHP and eRT≥τeff. Hence, it was important to determine τeff for each cell in order to determine the contribution of AHP to the integration process.
Estimating τeff during activity
For the spinal motor activity we differentiated three situations: The quiescent state with little or no synaptic input; the on-cycle with motor nerve activity and spike activity in motoneurons; and the off-cycle, which is at the low point in between the on-cycles (Figure 4A–C). As expected the effective membrane time constant decreased significantly during scratching. For representative data, τeff was 27 ms in the quiescent state, while it was only 2.8 ms in the On-cycle and 5.2 ms in the off-cycle (Figure 4C–E). Membrane time constants during quiescence are ordinarily estimated from Vm-decay times after injected current pulses (figure S2). During the dynamic and intense synaptic input in the on-cycle and off-cycle, this method was both difficult and imprecise. Instead, if Vm is assumed to follow a stochastic process known as Ornstein-Uhlenbeck-process (OU-process), then maximum likelihood estimation would be the proper way to obtain τeff [26], [27]. The estimated values of τeff with this technique are listed in Data and Methods 1 in Data and Methods S1 and see Figures S1 –S3. It turned out that Vm did not obey an OU-process (Figure S4) and these estimates of τeff were systematically much higher than eRT and eSIT. Therefore, τeff was instead estimated empirically by fitting an exponential decay to the initial part of the auto-correlation sequence (Figure 4F). It was necessary to fit to the initial part (from 0 to 3 ms, vertical gray line) to assure that the auto-correlation lag was small compared to the total length of the sample (200 ms), since the premise of exponential decay is infinite-length of data trace (see Data and Method S1).
Temporal features for the population
The effective synaptic integration times, the effective recovery times and the effective membrane time constants across the population of neurons are listed in Figure 5A. The average eSIT was μ = 7.7±0.8 ms and the average eRT was μ = 7.5±0.7 ms (mean±SE, n = 17). The median of eSIT and eRT were both 7.7 ms. The eRT was assumed to be approximately equal to the sum of τeff and the effective AHP (see above). However, the population average of τeff was μ = 9.3±1.4 ms (mean±SE, n = 17) while the median of τeff was 7.0 ms. In some cases (41%, n = 7/17) τeff was longer than the corresponding eRT, which suggests two things. First, the reset potentials were closer to the steady state mean Vm than the natural fluctuations of Vm around the mean, so the decay from reset back to the mean was faster than τeff. Secondly, the eAHP was close to zero or no more than a couple of milliseconds. This value of eAHP is a dramatic decrease from the 200 ms AHP duration previously reported during quiescence [4], [24]. Further, not only did the population spread in eRT across cells correlate with the population spread in eSIT (R2 = 0.81), the eRT and the eSIT both had significant correlation with τeff (Figure 5B) (ReSIT 2 = 0.60, p = 0.0007, ReRT 2 = 0.47, p = 0.005, when ignoring the two outliers, cell 7 and 16). Since eRT is dependent on both τeff and eAHP, while eSIT is only dependent on τeff, these strong correlations also indicate that the contribution of eAHP to eRT must be minor in most cells.
Hence, the population spread in eRT, eSIT, and τeff probably reflected different levels of synaptic intensity in different cells. Since the eRT is the major contribution to the refractory period, we expected most of the inter-spike intervals (ISI) to be longer than or equal to the eRT. If this is the case, the data points in a plot of the shortest ISI versus eRT in each cell should fall at the 45°-line or below. Indeed, all points were within their error bars or below (Figure 5C). In this way, the inverse of the eRT represents an upper bound for the spike frequency during network activity.
Absence of spike frequency adaptation
The AHP contributes to spike frequency adaptation (SFA) in motoneurons at rest [4], [24]. A classical example of SFA in absence of synaptic input is during bursting induced by N-Methyl-D-Aspartic acid (NMDA). We therefore performed a heuristic comparison between NMDA-bursting in MNs in slices with the bursting induced by synaptic input in functional networks (cf. Figures 6A and 6B). During each burst in the case of NMDA-bursting (Figure 6Aa), spike frequency peaked at onset and adapted to a lower level at the end of the burst (Figure 6Ab). The histogram of interspike-intervals confirmed similarity in firing rate distribution between NMDA-bursting and network bursting [28] (cf. Figure 6Ac and 6Bc). ISIN plotted against ISIN+1 displayed a ring-like pattern (Figure 6Ad), as expected from adapting spike trains in each successive burst [29]. Note that the vast majority of points (83%/17%, SE = 6%) are above the 45°-line, which is a consequence of SFA, presumably in large part produced by the prominent AHP. Under these conditions neighboring ISIs were significantly correlated (R2 = 0.17) as expected from AHP-mediated adaptation (Figure 6Ae).
The firing pattern in MNs during scratching was qualitatively different (Figure 6Ba). Though the distribution of ISI times resembled the NMDA-bursting (cf. Figure 6Bc and 6Ac), spike timing was irregular (Figure 6Bb) and points scattered nearly symmetrically around the 45°-line in the return map (51%/49%, SE = 6%) (Figure 6Bd). This shows that spike frequency acceleration was as prevalent as spike frequency adaptation during bursts [29] and therefore that the AHP and other intrinsic mechanisms for adaptation or acceleration in spike frequency did not have a detectable influence on the firing pattern during network activity. Furthermore, the correlation between ISIN and ISIN+1 was marginal (R2 = 0.03) (Figure 6Be), which shows that spikes were driven by a stochastic process rather than by deterministic intrinsic properties. In all 17 motoneurons SFA was insignificant during depolarizing waves, i.e. the numbers above the line were not significantly higher than 50% (Figure 7). Thus, we conclude that the mechanisms causing SFA such as AHP accumulation were not pervasive enough to overcome the increase in synaptic conductance during motor activity.
Discussion
Spike timing, the principal output of neurons, is determined by interacting synaptic and intrinsic ionic conductances. Recent decades have provided a wealth of information about the intrinsic response properties and their proposed roles in specific cell types in many parts of the nervous system [5], [30], [31]. Properties of the AHP in particular have been linked to classification of fast and slow spiking neurons with clear functional connotations [5], [32]. The present study reports up to ten-fold decrease in the membrane time constant (Figure 4) and severe reduction in the AHP during functional activity (Figure 3 and 5) which emphasize that assertions about the functional significance of intrinsic response properties must be validated in neurons embedded in an active network. In our experimental paradigm, the role of the AHP in spike timing is essentially eliminated and replaced by rapid temporal integration in the high conductance state provided by intense synaptic input (Figures 3, 5, 6). We therefore propose that motor systems perform rapidly changing movements by letting intense synaptic input transiently supplant slow intrinsic properties when necessary.
Coincidence detection
This transition has previously been referred to as a transition from temporal integration to coincidence detection in sensory perception [33]. Konig and colleagues defined a coincidence detector as a neuron in which the integration time for synaptic potentials is short compared with the average ISI, and a temporal integrator if the reverse is true. Adopting this definition, the motoneurons in the present study were all coincidence detectors during motor activity, since their eSIT and eRT on average are an order of magnitude shorter than their average ISI (cf. Figure 5A and 6Bc, and see Figure S5). The functional effect and benefit of MNs working as coincidence detectors could be to minimize aberrant firing due to the background barrage of noisy synaptic input [28]. The potentially undesirable consequence of having such a coincidence detection scheme is irregular spike patterns, which we indeed did observe (Figure 6). However, irregular patterns are not likely to be a problem, since muscle fibers are slow integrators, and therefore the exact temporal structure of the MN firing, whether irregular and yet fast coincidence detectors or slow regular temporal integrators, is unimportant for securing a smooth contraction.
Origins of spike pattern
The network mechanisms underlying the phasic spike activity in motoneurons during rhythmic motor behaviors are unknown. It has been hypothesized that certain intrinsic properties are crucial mediators of bursting rhythms in spinal networks [3], [5], [34]. It is also widely accepted that the after-hyperpolarization in MN secures repetitive firing at low rates appropriate for regulation of muscle contraction [3], [4]. The high conductance state, however, compromises the ability to fire repetitively during steady depolarization [11]. This is probably a direct consequence of the reduced slow AHP since the same effect is observed when the AHP is reduced by blocking the underlying ionic current pharmacologically rather than by shunting [35]. Nevertheless, MNs still fired in a broad range of rates in the high conductance state while driven by rapid fluctuations in membrane potential during motor activity (Figure 6B) [13]. Further evidence for fluctuation driven irregular firing is the fact that the shortest ISIs in the active network are longer than the eRT (and thus eAHP) (Figure 6C). This is in contrast to the regular firing and frequency adaptation in motoneurons in slices during NMDA induced bursting (Figure 6A). The reduced AHP amplitude, increased spike time variability and the absence of adaptation during locomotor network activity have also been observed in MNs in the decerebrate cat [36]. In the light of our findings it will be interesting to investigate if the underlying mechanism is modulation of intrinsic properties [1], [24], [37] or parallel increase in excitatory and inhibitory synaptic activity [13], [38].
In conclusion, the rhythm-generation could have two origins: a pattern generating subset of neurons elsewhere in the network in which intrinsic response properties are protected from shunting by intense synaptic bombardment or alternatively, the motor rhythm can be an emergent distributed network phenomenon as suggested for respiratory rhythms [39], [40], i.e. the network bursting hypothesis. Decisive experimental tests of these hypotheses are still missing.
Caveats of constant current protocols
The role of AHPs in repetitive firing in MNs induced by depolarizing current through the recording electrode has been thoroughly investigated [4], [5], [24]. Based on the linear summation of synaptic potentials under certain experimental conditions [41], [42] this approach takes injected current as a simplifying representation of synaptic input. However, important aspects of synaptic input are overlooked in this approach. First, synaptic variability adds fluctuations as a second moment to Vm [22]. Synaptic Vm-fluctuations may have important computational roles [43]–[45]. Secondly, the effect of surge in conductance from synaptic input is unaccounted for in the constant current protocol (Figure 1). Therefore the functional role of the AHP and other intrinsic response properties should be assessed either in functional networks with real synaptic input, as in the present study, or by dynamic clamp analysis in neurons at rest [7], [8], [46] if a valid estimate of the temporal structure of the functional synaptic conductances is available.
Balanced state in the spinal cord
Though anatomical evidence suggests an approximate balance between inhibitory and excitatory contacts in cat motoneurons [4], the circuit substrate for variations in the balance of inhibition and excitation during motor activity and its prevalence in other spinal networks is yet unresolved. High-conductance states produced by parallel increase in inhibitory and excitatory synaptic activity is a common occurrence in other functional networks [6], [38], [47]–[51]. In the neocortex, the link between excitation and inhibition is provided by feed-forward and recurrent pathways [48], [49], [52]–[55]. In the spinal cord, recurrent inhibition is unlikely to contribute significantly to the high conductance state in MNs since recurrent collaterals are scarce in the turtle [56], [57] and Renshaw inhibition has not been documented. Feed-forward inhibition provided by Ia inhibitory interneurons can produce synaptic conductance that clearly reduces intrinsic response properties in cat MNs in vivo [58]. However, reciprocal inhibition contributes insignificantly to the high conductance state in MNs during breathing [38] and scratching [13]. We propose that inhibitory input to MN is mostly due to local feedforward inhibitory connections, but it remains to be seen if balanced spinal motor networks resemble architectures of the much better investigated networks in other parts of the nervous system [6], [38], [48], [51], [55].
Materials and Methods
All the experiments were performed in an integrated spinal cord-carapace preparation from the adult turtle except the heuristic control experiment of NMDA-induced spike frequency adaptation in figure 6A, which was performed in a transverse slice from adult turtle. In the integrated preparation the spinal cord remains in the spinal canal with the tactile sensory nerves from the carapace intact. The motor nerves are carefully transected to avoid muscle movements and dissected out to for electro-neurogram recordings. The scratch reflex was activated by mechanical somato-sensory stimulation of selected regions on the carapace (see below) as described [11], [20].
Integrated preparation
Red-eared turtles (Trachemys scripta elegans) were placed on crushed ice for 2 hrs to ensure hypothermic anesthesia. Animals were killed by decapitation and blood substituted by perfusion with a Ringer solution containing (mM): 120 NaCl; 5 KCl; 15 NaHCO3; 2 MgCl2; 3 CaCl2; and 20 glucose, saturated with 98% O2 and 2% CO2 to obtain pH 7.6. The carapace containing the D4-D10 spinal cord segments was isolated by transverse cuts and removed from the animals, similar to studies published elsewhere [19], [20]. The surgical procedures complied with Danish legislation and were approved by the controlling body under the Ministry of Justice.
Slice preparation
One mm thick slices of the turtle spinal cord were placed in a chamber for intracellular recording and submerged in and perfused with oxygenated Ringer solution. The pharmacological agent N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) was added to the ringer medium to induce bursting activity (10 µM).
Recordings
Intracellular recordings in current-clamp mode were performed with an Axoclamp-2A amplifier (Axon Instruments, Union City, CA). Glass pipettes (part no. 30-0066, Havard Apparatus, UK) were pulled with a electrode puller (model P-87, Sutter instrument co., USA) and filled with a mixture of 0.9 M potassium acetate and 0.1 M KCl. Intracellular recordings were obtained from neurons in segment D10. Recordings were accepted if neurons had a stable membrane potential more negative than −50 mV. Data were sampled at 20 kHz with a 12-bit analog-to-digital converter (Digidata 1200, Axon Instruments, Union City, CA), displayed by means of Axoscope and Clampex software (Axon Instruments, Union City, CA), and stored on a hard disk for later analysis. Hip flexor nerve activity was recorded with a differential amplifier Iso-DAM8 (WPI) using a suction pipette. The bandwidth was 100 Hz–1 kHz.
Activation of network
Mechanical stimulation was performed with the fire polished tip of a bent glass rod mounted to the membrane of a loudspeaker in the cutaneous region known to elicit “pocket scratch” [12] which results in a broad activation of cells [59]. The duration, frequency, and amplitude of the stimulus were controlled with a function generator (Figure 2A). This tactile stimulus induced the scratch-like network activity, which was monitored by the suction electrode nerve recordings from the Hip-flexor nerve (Figure 2B).
Model data
The data used to illustrate the difference between synaptic-current and synaptic-conductance based fluctuating inputs (Figure 1) was based on a one-compartment model simulation [22] supplemented with Ca2+ conductance and a Ca2+-activated K+-conductance. The synaptic noise was modeled as white current noise in the current-based regime with the heuristic expression,
where the fast conductances of the action potential and Ca2+-conductance were not shown here for simplicity (for complete description see Data and Methods S1) and the total membrane conductance was
which contains no synaptic component. In this scheme, the synaptic input was represented as a current, Isyn. The membrane capacitance is C, and Gleak, Eleak, GAHP, EK are conductance and reversal potential of leak and AHP, respectively.
In a more realistic regime, the high intensity synaptic input was modeled as a conductance [22]. Here, the AHP was reduced as a consequence of the increase in total input conductance (Figure 1). The conductance-based regime applies when the synaptic conductance (Gsyn) is so large that it can no longer be considered small compared with the Gtotal [6]. Heuristically expressed similar to the synaptic current model
where Esyn is the weighted reversal potential of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic reversal potentials and Gsyn is the sum of both conductances. Gsyn is competing with GAHP in controlling inter-spike intervals, and if it is large enough it can render GAHP insignificant at steady state:
when GSyn≫GAHP. The action potentials were largely unaffected by the conductance increase, since fast Na/K conductances of the action potential were much larger. In a simple Hodgkin-Huxley-model added AHP- and Ca2+- conductances and including synaptic input as either current noise (Isyn = 12 nA, σsyn = 3 nA, OU-simulated with time-constant = 1 ms and D = 0.0005) or conductance noise we verified the theoretical importance of synaptic conductance (Figure 1). The conductance noise was a mixture between inhibition and excitation balanced at −60 mV (see Data and Method S1 for details).
Evolution of Vm-distribution
The variable constituting the estimation of effective eSIT and eRT was the membrane potential (Vm). This variable was stochastic and a sample measurement drawn from an underlying probability distribution function (PV), which we assumed had the same statistics in all interspike intervals. The probability distribution depends on time after occurrence of spike and this dependence was a manifestation of intrinsic current generators like SK-channels. Because of the large synaptic fluctuations, it was necessary to look at the distribution PV instead of just isolated instances of Vm. These fluctuations were assumed uncorrelated from trial to trial, so we could estimate Pv by superimposing spikes.
The key assumption is, if the distribution at some given point in time, Pv(t1), is different from the distribution at a later point in time, Pv(t2), then there has been a change in the intrinsic current generation (cf. Figure 3A and B). We selected Pv at one point in time (t = ttemplate) as a template distribution, which all distributions PV(t≠ttemplate) were compared with. The PV(ttemplate) was chosen more than 10 ms before the spike, since this region constitute a background Vm, and was compared via the Kolmogorov-Smirnov-2 sample test (KS-test) [60] expressed formally as:
F(V, t) is the empirical cumulative probability distribution function of Vm. N is the total number of traces (and spikes) used to estimate the distribution at time t (Figure 3D). The maximal difference between the cumulatives, D(t), was the measure for rejecting or accepting the null hypothesis. If the difference was larger than a critical value, then we rejected the null hypothesis that Vm(t) and Vm(ttemplate) were drawn from the same distribution. The binary test outcome was plotted (Figure 3A and B) where 1 represented rejection (gray area) and 0 represented no rejection of null hypothesis, at a 5% confidence limit. The p-value of the test was plotted below.
Effective Recovery time
The first point in time after the spike, where the KS-test was zero (i. e. no rejection of hypothesis of same distribution) was where we defined the AHP conductance and other transient intrinsic current generators no longer had a significant impact on the Vm and the passive diffusive spread had reach steady state. We dubbed this period effective recovery time (eRT, arrow in Figure 3B) in analogy to the effective membrane resistance and effective membrane time constant [14], [16], [21], [22]. This recovery time told us how long time after the spike had occurred that there was still a memory of the spike in Vm (Figure 3B).
Effective synaptic integration time
Similar to eRT, we could ask how long time prior to the spike, that Pv was different from the template distribution. This point represented a net depolarization caused either by reduced inhibition or increased excitation. We named this period effective synaptic integration time (eSIT) indicating the time prior to the spike where its occurrence can be predicted (arrow in Figure 3B).
Location of template
Obviously, the choice of template distribution is important. The template is always chosen prior to the spike. The earlier before the spike we choose the template, the more independent it is. However, there is a trade off, since the inter-spike interval has to be longer than the window between the template distribution and the spike. As a result, the larger the window is, the fewer spikes in a finite dataset will participate in the distribution (Figure 3D). Fortunately, the estimation of both eRT and eSIT is largely independent on the window size (Figure 3C). We chose to average the values of eRT and eSIT from templates 20 to 40 ms prior the spike, since these locations gave nearly constant values (Figure 3C).
Critique of method
The above described statistical testing of the evolution of Pv only accounts for changes that are locked to the occurrence of a single action potential, such as the AHP. Accumulative events that build up over several spikes as e.g. spike frequency adaptation or plateau potentials are not easily accounted for using this statistics. One way to test for slow changes would be to divide the spikes according into several different groups depending on their position in the epoch. These groups could then be compared to evaluate if the distributions have changed. However, we decided this was outside the scope of the present study and it was not necessary since the test of spike frequency adaptation (Figure 6) came out negative.
Inter-spike interval analysis
The inter-spike intervals were extracted from the intracellular recording during scratch episodes and processed. The auto-correlations were calculated as the normalized covariance function [26], [61] between ISIN and ISIN+1, ISIN+2, ISIN+3 etc (Figure 6d). The test for spike frequency adaptation (Figure 6e) was performed assuming statistical independence of observation of (ISIN, ISIN+1)-pairs [29] and thus a binomial distribution with chance of 50% above and 50% below the line ISIN = ISIN+1. If the number of points above was within a standard deviation of
of the 50%-line, as expected from an even binomial distribution [61], there was no significant spike frequency adaptation. K is the total number of points and p is the probability of a point being above the line, when assuming no correlation.
Data processing
All analysis was performed in Matlab (version 7.3, Mathworks). The data was converted from Axoclamp format to matlab and the spikes were identified and superimposed (Figure 3) to study the Vm statistics before and after the spike as described above. KS-testing of the Vm distributions was done with the matlab procedure “kstest2.m”. The custom made procedures for calculating eRT and eSIT has been uploaded to mathworks code sharing web site (http://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/) with the name “eRT.m” for the interested reader. The remaining matlab code is available on request.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
Thanks to Jens Midtgaard for carefully reading an earlier version of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This work was funded by the Carlsberg foundation (RB), the Novo Nordisk foundation (RB), the Lundbeck foundation (SD and JH) and FIST (JH). Role of sponsors: None.
References
- 1.Miles GB, Hartley R, Todd AJ, Brownstone RM. Spinal cholinergic interneurons regulate the excitability of motoneurons during locomtion. PNAS. 2007;104:2448–2453. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611134104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Rekling JC, Funk GD, Bayliss DA, Dong XW, Feldman JL. Synaptic control of motoneuronal excitability. Physiological reviews. 2000;80:767–852. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2000.80.2.767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Grillner S. Biological pattern generation: The cellular and computational logic of networks in motion. Neuron. 2006;52:751–766. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kernell D. The motoneurone and its muscle fibres. Oxford: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Toledo-Rodriguez M, El Manira A, Wallen P, Svirskis G, Hounsgaard J. Cellular signalling properties in microcircuits. TINS. 2005;28:534–540. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Destexhe A, Rudolph M, Pare D. The high-conductance state of neocortical neurons in vivo. Nature Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:739–751. doi: 10.1038/nrn1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wolfart J, Debay D, Masson GL, Destexhe A, Bal T. Synaptic background activity controls spike transfer from thalamus to cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1760–1767. doi: 10.1038/nn1591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fernandez FR, White JA. Artificial Synaptic Conductances Reduce Subthreshold Oscillations and Periodic Firing in Stellate Cells of the Entorhinal Cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:3790–3803. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5658-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Borg-Graham LJ, Monier C, Fregnac Y. Voltage-clamp measurement of visually-evoked conductances with whole-cell patch recordings in primary visual cortex. J Physiology (Paris) 1996;90:185–188. doi: 10.1016/s0928-4257(97)81421-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Borg-Graham LJ, Monier C, Y. F. Visual input evokes transient and strong shunting inhibition in visual cortical neurons. Nature. 1998;393:369–373. doi: 10.1038/30735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Alaburda A, Russo R, MacAulay N, Hounsgaard J. “Periodic high-conductance states in spinal neurons during scratch-like network activity in adult turtles”. J Neurosci. 2005;25:6316–6321. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0843-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Robertson GA, Stein PSG. Synaptic control of hindlimb motoneurones during three forms of fictive scratch reflex in the turtle. J Physiol. 1988;404:101–128. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Berg RW, Alaburda A, Hounsgaard J. Balanced inhibition and excitation drive spike activity in spinal half-centers. Science. 2007;315:390–393. doi: 10.1126/science.1134960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Barret JN. “Motoneuron dendrites: role in synaptic integration”. Federation proceedings. 1975;34:1398–1407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Destexhe A, Rudolph M, Fellous J-M, Sejnowski TJ. Fluctuating synaptic conductances recreate in vivo-like activity in neocortical neurons. Neuroscience. 2001;107:13–24. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00344-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bernander OJDR, Martin KAC, Koch C. Synaptic background activity influences spatiotemporal integration in single pyramidal cells. PNAS. 1991;88 doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.24.11569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shefchyk SJ, Jordan LM. Motoneuron input-resistance changes during fictive locomotion produced by stimulation of the mesencephalic locomotor region. J Neurophysiol. 1985;54:1101–1108. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.54.5.1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Perreault MC. Motoneurons have different membrane resistance during fictive scratching and weight support. J Neurosci. 2002;22:8259–8265. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-18-08259.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Keifer J, Stein PSG. Invitro motor program for the rostral scratch reflex generated by the turtle spinal-cord. Brain research. 1983;266:148–151. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)91318-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Alaburda AJH. Metabotropic modulation of motoneurons by scratch-like spinal network activity. J Neurosci. 2003;24:8625–8629. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08625.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Rapp M, Yarom Y, Segev I. The impact of parallel fiber background activity on the cable properties of cerebellar purkinje cells. Neural Computation. 1992;4:518–533. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Tiesinga PHE, Jose JV, Sejnowski TJ. Comparison of current-driven and conductance-driven neocortical model neurons with Hodgkin-Huxley voltage-gated channels. Physical review E. 2000;62:8413–8419. doi: 10.1103/physreve.62.8413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Destexhe A, Pare D. Impact of network activity on the integrative properties of neocortical pyramidal neurons in vivo. J Neurophysiol. 1999;81:1531–1547. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Brownstone RM. Beginning of the end: repetitive firing properties in the final common pathway. Progress in Neurobiology. 2006;78:156–172. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Guillamon A, McLaughlin DW, Rinzel D. Estimation of synaptic conductances. J Physiology Paris. 2006;100:31–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tuckwell HC. 1988. Introduction to theoretical neurobiology: Nonlinear and stochastic theories: Cambridge University Press.
- 27.Ditlevsen S, Lansky P. Estimation of the input parameters in the Ornstein-Uhlenbeck neuronal model. Physical review E. 2005;71:011907. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.71.011907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mazzoni A, Broccard FD, Garcia-Perez E, Bonifazi P, Ruaro ME, et al. On the dynamics of the spontaneous activity in neuronal networks. PLoS one. 2007;2:e439. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rodieck RW, Kiang NY-S, Gerstein GL. Some quantitative methods for the study of spontaneous activity of single neurons. Biophysical J. 1962;2:351–368. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3495(62)86860-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Llinás RR. The intrinsic electrophysiological properties of mammalian neurons: insights into central nervous system function. Science. 1988;242:1654–1664. doi: 10.1126/science.3059497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Häusser M, Spruston N, Stuart GJ. Diversity and dynamics of dendritic signaling. Science. 2000;290:739–744. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bartos M, Vida I, Jonas P. “Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks”. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:45–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Konig P, Engel AK, Singer W. Integrator or coincidence detector? The role of the cortical neuron revisited. TINS. 1996;19:130–137. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)80019-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Delgado-Lezama R, Hounsgaard J. Adapting motoneurons for motor behavior. Progress in Brain Research. 1999;23:57–63. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)62844-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hounsgaard J, Kiehn O, Mintz I. Response properties of motoneurones in a slice preparation of the turtle spinal cord. J Physiol. 1988;398:575–589. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Brownstone RM, Jordan LM, Kriellaars DJ, Noga BR, Shefchyk SJ. On the regulation of repetitive firing in lumbar motoneurones during fictive locomotion in the cat. Exp Brain Res. 1992;90:441–455. doi: 10.1007/BF00230927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Schmidt BJ. Afterhyperpolarization modulation in lumbar motoneurons during locomotor-like rhythmic activity in the neonatal rat spinal cord in vitro. Exp Brain Res. 1994;99:214–222. doi: 10.1007/BF00239588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Parkis MA, Dong X-W, Feldman JL, Funk GD. Concurrent inhibition and excitation of phrenic motoneurons during inspiration: Phase-specific control of excitation. J Neuroscience. 1999;19:2368–2380. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-06-02368.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Del Negro CA, Morgado-Valle C, Feldman JL. Respiratory rhythm: an emergent network property? Neuron. 2002;34:821–830. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00712-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Feldman JL, Del Negro CA. Looking for inspiration: New perspectives on repiratory rhythm”. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:232–241. doi: 10.1038/nrn1871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Granit R, Kernell D, Lamarre Y. Algebraic summation in synaptic activation of motoneurones firing in the “primary range” to injected currents. J Physiol. 1966;187:379–399. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Powers RK, Binder MD. Summation of effective synaptic currents and firing rate modulation in cat spinal motoneurons. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:483–500. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.1.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Chance FS, Abbott LF, Reyes AD. Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron. 2002;35:773–782. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00820-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: Implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J Neurosci. 1998;18:3870–3896. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03870.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Stein RB, Gossen ER, Jones KE. Neuronal variability: Noise or part of the signal? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;25:4985–4995. doi: 10.1038/nrn1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Prinz AA, Abbott LF, Marder E. The dynamical clamp comes of age. TINS. 2004;27:218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Czurko A, Mamiya A, Buzsaki G. Oscillatory coupling of hippocampal pyramidal cells and interneurons in the behaving rat. J Neurosci. 1999;19:274–287. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00274.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Haider B, Duque A, Hasenstaub AR, McCormick DA. Neocortical network activity in vivo is generated through a dynamical balance of excitation and inhibition. J Neurosci. 2006;26:4535–4545. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5297-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wehr M, Zador AM. Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory. Nature. 2003;426:442–446. doi: 10.1038/nature02116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Baca SM, Marin-Burgin A, Wagenaar DA, Kristan WB., Jr Widespread inhibition proportional to excitation controls the gain of a leech behavioral circuit. Neuron. 2008;57:276–289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Monier C, Fournier J, Fregnac Y. In vitro and in vivo measures of evoked excitatory and inhibitory conductance dynamics in sensory cortices. J Neurosci Meth. 2008;169:323–365. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kapfer C, Glickfeld LL, Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Supralinear increase of recurrent inhibition during sparse activity in the somatosensory cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:743–753. doi: 10.1038/nn1909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Silberberg G, Markram H. Disynaptic inhibition between neocortical pyramidal cells mediated by Martinotti cells. Neuron. 2007;53:735–746. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Monier C, Chavane F, Baudot P, Graham LJ, Fregnac Y. Orientation and direction selectivity of synaptic inputs in visual cortical neurons: a diversity of combinations produces spike tuning. Neuron. 2003;37:663–680. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00064-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Marino J, Schummers J, Lyon DC, Schwabe L, Beck O, et al. Invariant computations in local cortical networks with balanced excitation and inhibition. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:194–201. doi: 10.1038/nn1391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ruigrok TJ, Crowe A, Donkelaar HJ. Morphology of lumbar motoneurons innervating hindlimb muscles in the turtle Pseudemys scripta elegans: an intracellular horseradish peroxidase study. J Comp Neurol. 1984;230:413–425. doi: 10.1002/cne.902300309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Chmykhova NM, Adanina VO, Karamian OA, Kozhanov VM, Vesselkin NP, et al. Comparative study of spinal motoneuron axon collaterals. Brain Research Bulletin. 2005;66:381–386. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hyngstrom AS, Johnson MD, Miller JF, Heckman CJ. Intrinsic electrical properties of spinal motoneurons vary with joint angle. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:363–369. doi: 10.1038/nn1852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Berkowitz A. “Broadly tuned spinal neurons for each form of fictive scratching in spinal turtles”. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:1017–1026. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Press WH, Teukolsky SA, Vetterling WT, Flannery BP. Numerical recipes in fortran-The art of scientific computing. 2nd edition. Cambridge University Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Taylor JR. An introduction to error analysis-The study of uncertainties in physical measurements. California: University Science books; 1982. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.