Skip to main content
Journal of General Internal Medicine logoLink to Journal of General Internal Medicine
. 2008 Jul 30;23(10):1708–1712. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0719-5

Affective Forecasting: An Unrecognized Challenge in Making Serious Health Decisions

Jodi Halpern 1,, Robert M Arnold 2,3
PMCID: PMC2533375  PMID: 18665428

Abstract

Patients facing medical decisions that will impact quality of life make assumptions about how they will adjust emotionally to living with health declines and disability. Despite abundant research on decision-making, we have no direct research on how accurately patients envision their future well-being and how this influences their decisions. Outside medicine, psychological research on “affective forecasting” consistently shows that people poorly predict their future ability to adapt to adversity. This finding is important for medicine, since many serious health decisions hinge on quality-of-life judgments. We describe three specific mechanisms for affective forecasting errors that may influence health decisions: focalism, in which people focus more on what will change than on what will stay the same; immune neglect, in which they fail to envision how their own coping skills will lessen their unhappiness; and failure to predict adaptation, in which people fail to envision shifts in what they value. We discuss emotional and social factors that interact with these cognitive biases. We describe how caregivers can recognize these biases in the clinical setting and suggest interventions to help patients recognize and address affective forecasting errors.

KEY WORDS: decision-making, communication, patient preferences, doctor–patient relationship, quality of life


When facing medical decisions, patients make assumptions about how they will adjust emotionally to living with disabilities and declines in health. Yet we know little about how accurately patients predict their future well-being and the role of such predictions in their actual decisions.13 Outside medicine, a burgeoning field of psychology—“affective forecasting”—consistently shows that people are poor predictors of their future well-being. Specifically, people overestimate the impact and duration of negative emotions in response to loss. People without a given disability rate their expected quality of life significantly lower than those actually living with that disability. Researchers have demonstrated this gap for paraplegia, visual impairment, heart disease, asthma, dialysis, or living with a colostomy.1,46 The overarching conclusion is that people fail to envision their own capacities to adapt to declines in health.

Why should this concern physicians? Patients need to be able to form realistic beliefs about their future quality of life to make adequately informed decisions. Patients often choose among treatment options with similar impacts on mortality, but very different effects on their lives.79 Patients need to form realistic beliefs about their future quality of life to make decisions ranging from whether to undergo screening to whether to pursue aggressive treatment at the end of life.1012 Beyond the clinic, the difficulties people have predicting their future well-being also appear to contribute to a wide range of faltering public health initiatives ranging from long-term disease prevention13,14 to advance directives.15

Current models of shared medical decision-making presuppose a division of labor in which doctors are experts about the medical facts and patients supply values or preferences regarding outcomes.1618 This model fails to include the beliefs patients hold about their ability to adapt emotionally, which, along with their values, determine their decisions. In this article, we develop a framework to describe how cognitive, emotional and social biases influence patients’ beliefs about their future well-being. Much more clinical research is needed to illuminate how affective forecasting affects patients’ decision-making and to identify interventions to help patients make more informed choices.1924 As a start, we offer descriptions meant to help physicians identify affective forecasting biases that influence patients’ views of their future quality of life in various states of illness and disability.

We begin with three case examples.

CASES

Mr. Wishaw A highly independent police officer in his late fifties requires a second above-the-knee amputation as a result of type II diabetes mellitus. He was admitted to the hospital for dry gangrene in the affected leg. Despite antibiotics, the infection is progressing and the surgeons are worried that without surgery he may die. He understands the medical facts, yet refuses surgery because he can’t imagine his life using a wheelchair. He hates the idea of “not being able to run around with my grandchildren” and being so dependent on his wife. His wife, who loves him, is distraught, but says she will go along with whatever he decides.

Dr. Charania An intellectually gifted, independent and socially active 70-year-old university professor has a left middle cerebral artery stroke with hemiplegia and an expressive aphasia. With an intensive period of rehabilitation, she may be able to live independently. However, she is unlikely to move back to her home (too many stairs), nor is she likely to be able to return to teaching. The team communicates with her through a letter board and she shows that she understands her situation. They suggest that after rehabilitation she move into assisted living where she can receive more support. While not clinically depressed, she hates this idea, because she believes that even with rehabilitation, her deficits will prevent her from connecting intellectually or socially. She refuses to participate in rehabilitation and tells her children that she hopes God will take her soon because it is “her time.”

Mr. Thompson A 67-year-old man receives a diagnosis of localized, low Gleason score prostate cancer. He urgently wants the cancer removed surgically, even though he understands that watchful waiting is considered a preferable strategy with fewer side effects. Mr. Thompson is unhappy about the possibility of facing impotence and urinary incontinence, but he and his wife feel that they can deal with it. He prefers the possibility of these side effects to feeling overwhelming anxiety about having “cancer inside,” which he believes will continue unabated and destroy his peace of mind for the ongoing future. His doctor thinks that over time the incontinence will bother Mr. Thompson more than the anxiety, but he keeps quiet, assuming that the patient knows himself best.

All three of these patients are competent, show a high level of health literacy, and understand treatment risks and benefits. They can reason and make decisions based on their long-standing values. However, they are either not well-informed about, or not appreciating, an important element of their decision. The first two do not seem to know or even consider the fact that most people adapt to disabilities much better than able-bodied people predict that they will.1,5,25 And Mr. Thompson does not seem to grasp accurately the relative impact of anxiety versus surgical side effects on his future quality of life.8,26

Their clinicians view these patients’ decisions as tragic but believe they have nothing else to offer, having informed these patients about the medical facts. To further question them seems like meddling with personal values. Yet for these patients, it is not their values, but their beliefs that drive their decisions. Specifically, they believe that their current feelings of despair and anxiety will continue unabated into the future. For example, both Mr. Wishaw and Ms. Charania want to live and enjoy their lives, but they no longer believe that this is possible. Mr. Wishaw believes that he will be too disabled to play with and enjoy his grandchildren. Ms. Charania would opt to extend her life if she thought that she could maintain her social and intellectual engagement. Mr. Thompson values his quality of life highly, but believes that incontinence will be more tolerable than his anxiety.

Thus while their doctors conscientiously meet current standards for informing patients, these patients are actually not appreciating important aspects of their future quality of life. What should doctors and patients know about barriers to predicting quality of life?

PROBLEMS ENVISIONING FUTURE STATES

Cognitive Distortions in Affective Forecasting

Affective forecasting researchers have identified consistent gaps between healthy and ill populations’ ratings of their well-being with disability or illness.1,46,25,2730 A recent review concluded that this “disability paradox” is not due to exaggeration by disabled individuals, but rather to the mispredictions of non-disabled individuals.1 What specifically do people mispredict? Research shows that people usually predict the valence and specific emotion type accurately, but they mispredict the magnitude and duration of their emotional shifts from baseline. This occurs in three different ways: people focus more on what they will lose than on what will stay the same (focalism), they fail to envision how their own coping skills will lessen their unhappiness (immune neglect), and they fail to envision how they might develop new values (adaptation).3137

Focalism Focalism refers to focusing narrowly on what will change in one’s life while ignoring how much of what one enjoys daily can still be continued.4,32,38 Thus, Mr. Wishaw imagines himself constantly depressed because he cannot walk or run, and he does not think about enjoyable experiences that he can still have, like meals with his grandchildren.

Focalism is the most easily recognizable problem in affective forecasting and appears straightforward to address—just ask Mr. Wishaw to list all the things he enjoys doing and then note that he will still be able to continue many of these. Yet while such defocusing exercises have worked in some non-medical situations,38 they have not helped to reduce focalism about disabilities.4,30 There also appear to be cultural differences in baseline focalism, which may give us important clues for designing interventions.39

Immune Neglect People generally fail to recognize the extent to which their defense, or coping, mechanisms will buffer them (provide “immunity”) from emotional suffering.33 Defense mechanisms are largely unconscious processes, ranging from developmentally early ways of shielding the self (which can be problematic in adult life)—such as denial and projection—to more mature coping processes, including rationalization, humor, intellectualization and compartmentalization.4043 Using such defense mechanisms truly helps people feel better,44 yet we are largely unaware of these processes as they occur.33,37

Importantly, while some developmentally early defense mechanisms, like denial, are more powerful if kept unconscious and not challenged by reality, others need not be unconscious to work. Compartmentalization, humor, intellectualization, and sublimation are all coping mechanisms that we can consciously use. Further, recent research suggests that people who are more conscious of how they use coping mechanisms are also significantly better affective forecasters.45 By asking patients about what helped them get through past difficult times, physicians can remind patients that they have many conscious tools for coping—for example, keeping busy, redefining options, or becoming more socially involved.46 These interventions are already used to help patients envision coping with cancer.47,48 Further, cognitive behavioral therapies (CBT) that help patients consciously experience and build their coping skills could be studied as ways of addressing immune neglect.4952

Failure to Predict Genuine Adaptation Research consistently shows that people fail to predict adaptation,30,44 despite findings suggesting that, over time, most people are highly adaptive to states of disability.1,25 By adaptation we mean the process of forming new values, replacing lost sources of meaning with new ones.53 For example, imagine that after her stroke, Dr. Charania, who had never listened to music, becomes an avid music lover. We need more research on how people adapt to particular medical problems over time, and we need to communicate these experiences to patients. For example, Mr. Thompson’s physician could have let him know that research shows that anxiety lessens for most people over time, but that incontinence often causes an ongoing depreciation in quality of life.8,26

Recent research suggests that it is possible to help people better envision adaptation.30 Support groups for cancer survivors have been shown to engender optimism and active coping.54,55 Knowing people who have adapted to a similar disability or health condition seems to help patients envision better quality of life if living with a disability.30 And even the stories of virtual others appear to be quite valuable, as evidenced by patients, avid use of Internet sites for sharing their experiences.56 In clinics, interactive decision aids with narratives have been shown to help patients better understand treatment risks and benefits and experience less decisional conflict.5760 However, there are also ethical risks with using rich anecdotes to inform. A recent study showed that while 20% of participants said that they desired life-prolonging treatment if they had advanced Alzheimer’s disease, after seeing a video of a patient and family’s struggles, not one desired life-prolonging treatment.61 Thus, the powerful impact of narratives might call for giving patients a time gap or debriefing between experiencing them and making their decisions.

Emotional and Social Influences on Affective Forecasting

In addition to the above cognitive biases (which occur even in calm states), emotional distress also influences how one appraises one’s future prospects. Research shows that people make different projections about their future well-being when they are in positive versus negative emotional moods.2,3,34 For example, fear can rivet attention on the most frightening aspects of a situation (worsening “focalism”) or convince a person that a possible threat is inevitable.53,62 Distress can block memories of better times, limiting one’s ability to form more hopeful beliefs about the future.63,64 Patients who are afraid and upset project these intense feelings onto the future,65 and anxiety can undermine the reflectiveness needed to recognize such projections and address them.65,66 For example, Mr. Thompson is distressed enough that it is difficult for him to imagine anything but his current anxiety continuing on into the future.

Physicians often need to address patients’ emotional distress before they can address cognitive biases. Empathic listening, cognitive reframing,49,51 gathering social supports, and encouraging peer support groups54,67 are all effective ways to help alleviate patients’ distress. However, when patients are upset, physicians sometimes respond in problematic ways, ranging from avoidance, to anger, to using stereotypes as a form of distancing.6870 And studies show that physicians’ specific professional biases may lessen their ability to see patients’ situations clearly, for example, when they are overly influenced by the memory of a rare bad outcome.65,71 Therefore, physicians are likely to better address patients’ emotional needs if they incorporate tools for reflective self-awareness into their practices.7274

Finally, stigma forms a powerful barrier to envisioning adapting to disability. Stigmatizing images of illness and disability depict broken and unfulfilling lives.75 Importantly, able-bodied people who know disabled people view living with disabilities more positively—perhaps because they gain realistic examples to imagine how they themselves could cope.30 Healthcare professionals could make a difference by calling for measures to address the societal basis of exclusion and stigma, which would help make the real lives of disabled people more visible.76

IMPLICATIONS FOR FURTHER RESEARCH

We need research on the incidence of forecasting biases in patients, and on the impact of such biases on actual medical decisions. Much of the research on affective forecasting has taken place in experimental psychology or behavioral economics labs with college students. We currently have almost no longitudinal data showing how individual patients actually adapt emotionally compared to their predictions, or about how patients vary in their abilities to predict future well-being.28,77,78 A crucial first step would be to establish empirical and clinical ways of identifying when decision-making is being disrupted by affective forecasting problems.

This research agenda also would benefit from crossing disciplinary boundaries, bringing together methods from social and cognitive psychology, behavioral economics, clinical research, and neuroscience. For example, clinical research shows that health state preferences change as patients experience serious health declines, suggesting the importance of longitudinal data.79 An fMRI study might identify brain differences corresponding to these preference changes in illness-experienced subjects, and cognitive psychology methods might look at differences in patients’ use of automatic versus deliberative processing depending on illness severity.

Finally, we need to study the impact of decisional supports on affective forecasting.8083 Research is needed on how such aids can incorporate forecasting interventions, such as helping people recall previous coping30 and providing narratives about how people respond differently to health challenges. Interestingly, patients, rather than clinicians, may lead the way in developing effective interventions, for example, through developing websites about coping with illness and disability.56 Physicians who educate themselves and their patients about forecasting biases and stay abreast of the diverse ways that patients learn about living with illness and disability will be better equipped to help patients make informed decisions.

Acknowledgment

Financial support: Dr. Halpern’s work was supported by the Greenwall Foundation Faculty Scholar’s Program, the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, and the National Institute of Mental Health. Dr. Arnold was supported by the Greenwall Foundation, Ladies Hospital Aid Society of Western Pennsylvania, the Jewish Health Care Foundation, the National Center for Palliative Care Research, and the LAS Trust Foundation.

Acknowledgement for Katie Hasson and Deb Seltzer: “For excellent editorial assistance.” And Anthony Back and Peter Ubel: “For useful comments about how to improve paper.”

Conflicts of Interest None disclosed.

References

  • 1.Ubel PA, Loewenstein G, Schwarz N, Smith D. Misimagining the unimaginable: the disability paradox and health care decision making. Health Psychol. 2005;24(4 (Suppl.)):S57–62, July. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 2.Loewenstein G. Hot-cold empathy gaps and medical decision making. Health Psychol. 2005;24(4 (Suppl.)):S49–56, July. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 3.Loewenstein G. Projection bias in medical decision making. Med Decis Making. 2005; 25(1):96–105. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 4.Ubel PA, Loewenstein G, Hershey J, et al. Do nonpatients underestimate the quality of life associated with chronic health conditions because of a focusing illusion? Med Decis Making. 2001;21(3):190–9, May-Jun. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 5.Riis J, Loewenstein G, Baron J, Jepson C, Fagerlin A, Ubel PA. Ignorance of hedonic adaptation to hemodialysis: a study using ecological momentary assessment. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2005;134(1):3–9, Feb. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 6.Baron J, Asch DA, Fagerlin A, et al. Effect of assessment method on the discrepancy between judgments of health disorders people have and do not have: a web study. Med Decis Making. 2003;23(5):422–34. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 7.Litwin MS, Gore JL, Kwan L, et al. Quality of life after surgery, external beam irradiation, or brachytherapy for early-stage prostate cancer. Cancer. 2007;109:2239–2247. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 8.Zeliadt SB, Ramsey SD, Penson DF, et al. Why do men choose one treatment over another?: a review of patient decision making for localized prostate cancer. Cancer. 2006;106(9):1865–74, May 1. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 9.Weeks JC, Cook EF, O’Day SJ, et al. Relationship between cancer patients’ predictions of prognosis and their treatment preferences. JAMA. 1998;279(21):1709–14, June 3, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 10.Maguire P, Faulkner A. Communicate with cancer patients: 2 Handling uncertainty, collusion, and denial. Br Med J. 1988;297:972–974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 11.Travis K. For phase I studies, ethical and practical concerns abound. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2004;96(18):1354–5, Sep 15. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 12.Tymstra T. The imperative character of medical technology and the meaning of ‘anticipated decision regret. Int J Technol Assess Health Care. 1989;5:207–213. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 13.Selwyn PA, Forstein M. Overcoming the false dichotomy of curative vs palliative care for late-stage HIV/AIDS: “Let me live the way I want to live, until I can’t". JAMA. 2003;290(6):806–14 August 13, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 14.Moyad MA, Carroll PR. Lifestyle recommendations to prevent prostate cancer, part II: time to redirect our attention? Urol Clin North Am. 2004;31(2):301–11. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 15.Lo B, Steinbrook R. Resuscitating advance directives. Arch Intern Med. 2004;164:1501–1506. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 16.Carrese JA. Refusal of care: patients’ well-being and physicians’ ethical obligations: “But doctor, I want to go home”. JAMA. 2006;296(6):375–376. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 17.Beauchamp TL, Childress JF. Principles of Biomedical Ethics. 5New York, N.Y.: Oxford University Press; 2001.
  • 18.Snyder L, Leffler C, for the Ethics and Human Rights Committee ACoP. Ethics Manual. 5th ed. Ann Intern Med. 2005;142(7):560–82. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 19.Appelbaum P, Grisso T. Assessing patients’ capacities to consent to treatment. N Engl J Med. 1988;319(25):1635–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 20.Grisso T, Appelbaum P. Assessing Competence to Consent to Treatment: A Guide to Physicians and Other Health Care Professionals. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1998.
  • 21.Grisso T, Appelbaum PS. Comparison of standards for assessing patients’ capacities to make treatment decisions. Am J Psychiatr. 1995;152(7):1033–7, Jul. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 22.Bursztajn H, Harding H, Gutheil T, Brodsky A. Beyond cognition: the role of disordered affective states in impairing competence to consent to treatment. Bull Am Acad Psychiatry Law. 1991;19(4):383–8. [PubMed]
  • 23.Buchanan AE, Brock DW. Deciding for Others: The Ethics of Surrogate Decision Making. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1989.
  • 24.Silverman H. The role of emotions in decisional competence, standards of competency, and altruistic acts. J Clin Ethics. 1997;8(2):171–5. [PubMed]
  • 25.Brickman P, Coates D, Janoff-Bulman R. Lottery winners and accident victims: is happiness relative? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1978;36(8):917–27, Aug. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 26.Denberg TD, Melhado TV, Steiner JF. Patient treatment preferences in localized prostate carcinoma: the influence of emotion, misconception, and anecdote. Cancer. 2006;107(3):620–30, Aug 1. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 27.Buick DL, Petrie KJ. “I know just how you feel": The validity of healthy women’s perceptions of breast-cancer patients receiving treatment. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2002;32(1):110–23, Jan. [DOI]
  • 28.Lacey HP, Fagerlin A, Loewenstein G, Smith DM, Riis J, Ubel PA. It must be awful for them: Perspectives and task context affects ratings for health conditions. Judgment and Decision Making. 2006;1(2):146–152, Nov.
  • 29.Smith DM, Sherriff RL, Damschroder L, Loewenstein G, Ubel PA. Misremembering colostomies? Former patients give lower utility ratings than do current patients. Health Psychol. 2006;25(6):688–95, Nov. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 30.Ubel PA, Loewenstein G, Jepson C. Disability and sunshine: can hedonic predictions be improved by drawing attention to focusing illusions or emotional adaptation? J Exp Psychol Appl. 2005;11(2):111–23, Jun. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 31.Schwarz N, Clore G. Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In: Higgins ET, Kruglanski A, eds. Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. New York: The Guilford Press; 1996.
  • 32.Gilbert D, Gill M, Wilson T. The future is now: temporal correction in affective forecasting. Org Behav Human Decis Process. 2002;88(1):430–44. [DOI]
  • 33.Gilbert D, Pinel E, Wilson T, Blumberg S, Wheatley T. Immune neglect: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;75:617–638. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 34.Schwarz N. Emotion, cognition, and decision making. Cogn Emot. 2000;14(4):433–40. [DOI]
  • 35.Schwarz N, Clore G. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;45(3):513–23. [DOI]
  • 36.Wilson T, Gilbert D. The pleasures of uncertainty: prolonging positive moods in ways people do not anticipate. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88(1):5–21. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 37.Wilson T, Gilbert D. Lessons from the past: do people learn from experience that emotional reactions are short-lived? Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2001;27(12):1648–61. [DOI]
  • 38.Wilson TD, Wheatley T, Meyers JM, Gilbert DT, Axsom D. Focalism: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(5):821–36, May. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 39.Lam KC, Buehler R, McFarland C, Ross M, Cheung I. Cultural differences in affective forecasting: the role of focalism. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2005;31(9):1296–309, Sep. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 40.Brenner C. The Mind in Conflict. New York: International Universities Press; 1982.
  • 41.Mitchell SA. Contemporary perspectives on self: toward an integration. Psychoanal Dialogues. 1991;1(2):121–47.
  • 42.Winnicott DW. The Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment; Studies in the Theory of Emotional Development. London: Hogarth; 1965.
  • 43.Kaplan HI, Sadock BJ. Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry/VI. 6Baltimore, MD: Wiliams & Wilkins; 1995.
  • 44.Wilson T, Gilbert D. Affective forecasting: knowing what to want. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2005;14(3):131–4. [DOI]
  • 45.Dunn EW, Brackett MA, Ashton-James C, Schneiderman E, Salovey P. On emotionally intelligent time travel: individual differences in affective forecasting ability. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2007;33(1):85–93, Jan. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 46.Weisman A. The Coping Capacity. New York, NY: Human Sciences Press; 1984.
  • 47.Nezu AM. Helping Cancer Patients Cope: A Problem-solving Approach. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1998.
  • 48.Block SD. Psychological issues in end-of-life care. J Palliat Med. 2006;9(3):751–72, Jun. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 49.Craske MG. Anxiety Disorders: Psychological Approaches to Theory and Treatment. Boulder: CO: Westview Press; 1999.
  • 50.Beck AT. The current state of cognitive therapy: a 40-year retrospective. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(9):953–9, Sep. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 51.Persons JB. Cognitive Therapy in Practice: A Case Formulation Approach. 1New York: Norton; 1989.
  • 52.Academy of Cognitive Therapy. Training certification as a cognitive therapist. www.academyofct.org.
  • 53.Elster J. Alchemies of the Mind: Rationality and the Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999.
  • 54.Ussher J, Kirsten L, Butow P, Sandovala M. What do cancer support groups provide which other supportive relationships do not? The experience of peer support groups for people with cancer. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62:2565–2576. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 55.Goodwin P. Support groups in advanced breast cancer. Cancer. 2005;104(11 Suppl):2596–601. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 56.patientslikeme.com [homepage on the Internet]. September 27, 2007; www.patientslikeme.com. Accessed September 27, 2007.
  • 57.O’Connor A, Rostom A, Fiset V, et al. Decision aids for paitents facing health treatment or screening decisions: systematic review. Br Med J. 1999;319:731–734. September 18, 1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 58.Murray E, Davis H, Tai SS, Coulter A, Gray A, Haines A. Randomised controlled trial of an interactive multimedia decision aid on hormone replacement therapy in primary care. BMJ. 2001;323(7311):490–3, Sep 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 59.Murray E, Davis H, Tai SS, Coulter A, Gray A, Haines A. Randomised controlled trial of an interactive multimedia decision aid on benign prostatic hypertrophy in primary care. BMJ. 2001;323(7311):493–6, Sep 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 60.Morgan MW, Deber RB, Llewellyn-Thomas HA, et al. Randomized, controlled trial of an interactive videodisc decision aid for patients with ischemic heart disease. J Gen Intern Med. 2000;15(10):685–93, Oct. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 61.Volandes AE, Lehmann LS, Cook EF, Shaykevich S, Abbo ED, Gillick MR. Using video images of dementia in advance care planning. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167(8):828–33, April 23, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 62.Buer N, Linton S. Fear-avoidance beliefs and catastrophizing: occurence and risk factor in back pain and ADL in the general population. Pain. 2002;99:485–491. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 63.Eich E, Forgas JP. Mood, cognition, and memory. In: Weiner IB, Freedheim DK, Schinka JA, Velicer WF, Lerner RM, eds. Handbook of Psychology, Vol 4. New York: Wiley; 2003:61–83.
  • 64.Philippot P, Schaefer A. Emotion and memory. In: Mayne, TJ, Bonanno, GA, eds. Emotions: Current Issues and Future Directions. New York, NY: Guiliford Press; 2001:82–122.
  • 65.Halpern J. From Detached Concern to Empathy: Humanizing Medical Practice. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press; 2001.
  • 66.LeDoux J. Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. New York: Penguin; 2003.
  • 67.Goodwin PJ. Support groups in advanced breast cancer. Cancer. 2005;104(11 Suppl):2596–601, Dec 1. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 68.Burgess D, van Ryn M, Dovidio J, Saha S. Reducing racial bias among health care providers: lessons from social-cognitive psychology. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(6):882–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 69.Burgess DJ, van Ryn M, Crowley-Matoka M, Malat J. Understanding the provider contribution to race/ethnicity disparities in pain treatment: insights from dual process models of stereotyping. Pain Med. 2006;7(2):119–34. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 70.Halpern J. Empathy and patient-physician conflicts. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(5):696–700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 71.Christakis NA, Asch DA. Biases in how physicians choose to withdraw life support. The Lancet. 1993;342(8872):642–6. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 72.Epstein RM, Siegel DJ, Silberman J. Self-monitoring in clinical practice: a challenge for medical educators. J Contin Educ Health Prof. 2008;28(1):5–13, Winter. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 73.Novack DH, Suchman AL, Clark W, Epstein RM, Najberg E, Kaplan C. Calibrating the physician. Personal awareness and effective patient care. Working group on promoting physician personal awareness, american academy on physician and patient. Jama. 1997;278(6):502–9, Aug 13. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 74.Meier DE, Back AL, Morrison RS. The inner life of physicians and care of the seriously ill. Jama. 2001;286(23):3007–14, Dec 19. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 75.Wang CC. Portraying stigmatized conditions: disabling images in public health. J Health Commun. 1998;3(2):149–59, Apr-Jun. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 76.Heijnders M, Van der Meij S. The fight against stigma: an overview of stigma-reduction strategies and interventions. Psychol Health Med. 2006;11(3):353–63. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 77.Fried TR, O’Leary J, Van Ness P, Fraenkel L. Inconsistency over time in the preferences of older persons with advanced illness for life-sustaining treatment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55(7):1007–1014, Jul. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 78.Fried TR, Van Ness PH, Byers AL, Towle VR, O’Leary JR, Dubin JA. Changes in preferences for life-sustaining treatment among older persons with advanced illness. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(4):495–501, Apr. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 79.Fried TR, Byers AL, Gallo WT, et al. Prospective study of health status preferences and changes in preferences over time in older adults. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(8):890–5, April 24, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 80.O’Connor AM, Drake ER, Wells GA, Tugwell P, Laupacis A, Elmslie T. A survey of the decision-making needs of Canadians faced with complex health decisions. Health Expect. 2003;6(2):97–109, Jun. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 81.Butow PN, Maclean M, Dunn SM, Tattersall MH, Boyer MJ. The dynamics of change: cancer patients’ preferences for information, involvement and support. Ann Oncol. 1997;8(9):857–863, Sep. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 82.Gattellari M, Butow P, Tattersall M. Sharing decisions in cancer care. Soc Sci Med. 2001;52:1865–1878. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 83.O’Connor A, Rostom A, Fiset V, et al. Decision aids for patients facing health treatment or screening decisions: systematic review. Br Med J. 1999;319:731–734. September 18, 1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Articles from Journal of General Internal Medicine are provided here courtesy of Society of General Internal Medicine

RESOURCES