Skip to main content
Journal of Neurophysiology logoLink to Journal of Neurophysiology
. 2008 Jul 9;100(3):1160–1168. doi: 10.1152/jn.90592.2008

Behavioral States, Network States, and Sensory Response Variability

Alfredo Fontanini 1,2, Donald B Katz 1
PMCID: PMC2544460  PMID: 18614753

Abstract

We review data demonstrating that single-neuron sensory responses change with the states of the neural networks (indexed in terms of spectral properties of local field potentials) in which those neurons are embedded. We start with broad network changes—different levels of anesthesia and sleep—and then move to studies demonstrating that the sensory response plasticity associated with attention and experience can also be conceptualized as functions of network state changes. This leads naturally to the recent data that can be interpreted to suggest that even brief experience can change sensory responses via changes in network states and that trial-to-trial variability in sensory responses is a nonrandom function of network fluctuations, as well. We suggest that the CNS may have evolved specifically to deal with stimulus variability and that the coupling with network states may be central to sensory processing.

INTRODUCTION

The primary goal of the sensory neuroscientist is to describe the brain's reaction to specific environmental stimuli and to relate these characterizations to perceptual function. To this end, neurophysiologists typically present a stimulus multiple times while recording the spiking activity of the subject's sensory neurons. The fact that these single-neuron responses are affected by the firing of other neurons to which they are connected—that each sensory neuron is extensively connected to others within local and distributed networks, such that activity in the network will affect the probability of firing in others—is not a subject of much debate.

The network activity in question can be summarized in terms of “states” of balanced inhibition and excitation (Brunel 2000; MacLean et al. 2005; Vogels et al. 2005) and monitored in “spontaneous” (i.e., not driven by any specific stimulus) local field potential (LFP) recordings, a measure of the average electrical activity of local networks (see Buzsáki 2006); the term “network state”, then, serves as shorthand for emergent, coherent structure (typically oscillatory) in the activity of multiple interconnected single neurons. Theoretical analyses predict that when these network states change, the changes should have significant, nonrandom impacts on single-neuron processing of inputs (Chance et al. 2002; Jaeger and Bower 1999; Santamaria et al. 2002). Thus it is reasonably clear that sensory responses should be studied in the context of the current states of networks in which the neurons reside.

In this review, we explore the implications of this innocuous-seeming claim. After first reviewing data demonstrating that large-scale state changes in unconscious animals—across levels of anesthesia and sleep—do indeed significantly influence sensory function, we relate these effects to a series of successively more subtle phenomena in awake animals. We discuss well-studied behavioral state changes known to modulate sensory responsiveness—switches from consciousness to unconsciousness and from attentiveness to inattentiveness—and show that they are associated with (and perhaps synonymous with) neural network changes. We then turn to learning-related behavioral modifications, suggesting that the sensory response changes typically linked to learning-related synaptic changes may in fact be more directly related to network reconfigurations. Finally, we extend this analysis to include sensory variability occurring across one to three trials; this extension leads us ultimately to suggest that trial-to-trial variability in sensory responses may not be meaningless noise, as is commonly believed, but may instead be a function of behavioral and network state changes.

If this last point is correct—if network states shift on a moment-to-moment basis, such that sensory responses change from trial to trial—then the nervous system does not act on reliable sensory representations; thus the innocuous-seeming link between network states and sensory responses may in fact represent a challenge to current thinking about sensory processing, which is based on the idea that objects are represented in the brain by stationary (plus random noise) sensory codes.

RESPONSES CHANGE ACROSS LEVELS OF ANESTHESIA AND SLEEP

At first glance, it appears that brain function should be easily described in an unconscious animal. The animal is simply anesthetized, knocked out, dead to the world. A closer look, of course, makes it clear that this description is not strictly correct; physiological and behavioral parameters frequently change during surgery as a function of anesthesia level: reflexes disappear and reappear, muscular tone increases and decreases, and heart rate, respiratory frequency, and core temperature change. With these changes come dramatic shifts in neural network states (Friedberg et al. 1999), measured in terms of shifts in the characteristic frequencies in single LFPs, or in the coupling between multiple LFPs or spike trains. During periods of lighter anesthesia, spontaneous patterns of cortical activity are reminiscent of the “fast oscillations” observed in animals during periods of active waking or rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. Activity patterns of a deeply anesthetized animal, meanwhile, are more reminiscent of “slow-wave sleep” (Adrian 1950; Fontanini and Bower 2005; Friedberg et al. 1999; Murakami et al. 2005). These phenomena have been explicitly linked to changes in thalamocortical network functioning (Destexhe and Sejnowski 2001) and to modulations of the balance between intrinsic excitatory and inhibitory conductances (Bazhenov et al. 2002; Hill and Tononi 2005).

Sensory responses change with these sudden, anesthesia-linked transitions from one LFP regime to another. In the olfactory cortex of rats, for example, odors evoke smaller and shorter-lasting responses when the animal is in sleep-like states than when it is in waking-like states (Murakami et al. 2005; Fig. 1A). Similar results have been noted in auditory cortex (Kisley and Gerstein 1999) and in somatosensory thalamus and cortex (Aguilar and Castro-Alamancos 2005; Castro-Alamancos 2004). A series of analogous results from visual cortex—coupled changes in spontaneous patterns of background activity and receptive field shapes—have been successfully modeled in terms of specific shifts in the balance between excitation and inhibition in simulated networks (Worgotter et al. 1998).

FIG. 1.

FIG. 1.

Macroscopic states modulate neural responsiveness. A: responses to odors in the olfactory cortex of rats are larger during periods dominated by fast electroencephalographic (EEG) waves (right) than during periods of slow waves (left). From top to bottom: EEG recorded from neocortex; a single-trial, single-neuron olfactory cortical response histogram and associated spike train (SU); simultaneously recorded record of artificial inhalation (AI); and, finally, an across-trial peristimulus time histogram (PSTH). B: average firing rate (y-axis) across time (x-axis) of a murine olfactory bulb mitral cell in response to amyl acetate (red) and citral (blue) during anesthesia and wakefulness. Note that the excitatory response to amyl acetate turns inhibitory during wakefulness. Gray shadow: period of odor presentation. Colored bars: average response amplitude across the last half of the stimulus period. Black line in the prestimulus period: average spontaneous firing frequency. C: spread of activity following magnetic stimulation of the cortex in human subjects during nonrapid eye movement (NREM) sleep (left) and wakefulness (right). Traces on the left of each panel are global mean field powers (yellow line: significance level). On the right of each panel, brain diagrams showing the location of current sources, color coded according to the latency of activation (blue = 0 ms; red = 300 ms). The yellow cross marks the location of the stimulus. Panels modified with permission from Murakami et al. (2005) (A), Rinberg et al. (2006) (B), and Massimini et al. (2005) (C).

Given these results from studies of anesthesia (Steriade 2000), it is natural to ask whether sensory responses change across different sleep stages—each of which is associated with distinctive patterns of LFP, physiology, and behavior—themselves. In fact, responses to a particular stimulus are different in each of the different network states that distinguish stages of sleep. In auditory thalamus (Edeline et al. 2000), for instance, sensory responses recorded during slow-wave sleep differ in several ways from those recorded during REM sleep: neurons recorded during REM sleep typically respond to larger frequency ranges, in a less homogeneous fashion, than do neurons recorded during slow-wave sleep.

In summary, even under apparently monolithic conditions (i.e., situations in which the animal is immobile and not producing any obviously visible movement), sensory responses depend on subtle distinctions in the animal's physiology, distinctions that are synonymous with local and distributed neural network states.

RESPONSES CHANGE ACROSS THE BOUNDARY OF CONSCIOUSNESS

Given that coupled plasticity in network activity and sensory responses is observed within periods of time in which an animal remains still and unconscious, it comes as no surprise that the transition between consciousness and unconsciousness is associated with large differences in both network states and sensory function. The spectral character of LFPs in the olfactory bulb changes as an animal falls under (or climbs out of) the sway of anesthetic (Adrian 1950). Furthermore, odor responses in olfactory bulb neurons are much sparser in awake animals than in anesthetized animals. Approximately three fourths of the neurons that can be identified as odor responsive in the anesthetized state become less responsive as the animal wakes and some of the remaining responses change from excitatory to inhibitory (Rinberg et al. 2006; Fig. 1B). These network and sensory phenomena are likely related to the fact that, in awake animals, olfactory bulb responses are involved more in task-related functions than in olfactory coding itself (Kay and Laurent 1999), via strong feedback connectivity from piriform cortex (Martin et al. 2007).

Although such studies are scarce, data from other sensory systems match those observed in the olfactory bulb. The auditory cortical tonotopy, for instance, also changes across the consciousness boundary. Primary auditory cortex (A1) tone responses increase in latency, decrease in number and amplitude, and even change in best frequency under equithesin anesthesia (Gaese and Ostwald 2001). Between waking to slow-wave sleep the changes are more variable but no less striking (Edeline et al. 2001). Network function changes across the sleep–wake divide and this change has major implications for single-neuron processing.

Research from Tononi and colleagues (Massimini et al. 2005) puts these findings in a larger systems context and provides additional evidence for the link between network organization and response changes seen during the transition from wakefulness to slow-wave sleep. These researchers used high-density scalp electrodes to record the spread of transcranial stimulation in human subjects during sleep (specifically during periods of nonrapid eye movement sleep characterized by slow oscillations) and wakefulness. The initial stimulus-induced activity was stronger, but the spread of activation was greatly reduced in both temporal and spatial extent, in sleeping compared with waking subjects (Fig. 1C). The processing of activity (in this case, electrically induced) was greatly curtailed during sleep because of greatly increased damping of the effective network.

NEURAL RESPONSES CHANGE WITH ATTENTION

The value of working with awake animals lies in the fact that the researcher has the opportunity to explore the interaction between cognitive variables, typically thought of as top-down influences on sensory processing, and the bottom-up sensory neural responses themselves. The most obvious and well studied of these cognitive state shifts is the one between attentiveness and inattentiveness. Here we suggest that shifts in attention are analogous to the variations in anesthesia and sleep levels described earlier. In each case, reconfigurations of the network underlie changes in sensory responsiveness.

Sizable, meaningful changes in animal behavior accompany sudden shifts of an animal's focus toward particular features or modalities of its stimulus environment. Attention affects performance on stimulus detection and discrimination tasks and, in general, is a central variable controlling sensory processing (for review, see Posner 1980). Furthermore, evidence tightly links changes in attention to modulations of neural response magnitudes, both in higher-order (Mitchell et al. 2007; Reynolds and Chelazzi 2004) and primary sensory cortices—including visual (Li et al. 2006) and auditory (Fritz et al. 2005, 2007) cortices—and even in sensory thalamus (Kastner et al. 2006; Wunderlich et al. 2005).

More recently, it has become apparent that attention is, like sleep and anesthesia level, intimately related to specific network states (Buschman and Miller 2007; Saalmann et al. 2007; Womelsdorf and Fries 2007; Womelsdorf et al. 2006). Specifically, attention acutely changes the spectral content of LFPs, enhancing activity in multiple frequency ranges, most notably θ (4–7 Hz) and γ (∼30–80 Hz; Ekstrom et al. 2005; Engel et al. 2001; Fan et al. 2007; Womelsdorf and Fries 2007). Such enhancements are positively correlated with performance on attention-intensive behavioral tasks (Womelsdorf et al. 2006; Fig. 2A).

FIG. 2.

FIG. 2.

Relationship between behavioral and network states. A, top and middle panels: time course of average relative power in the gamma band of a visual cortex local field potential (LFP, y-axis) across trials (x-axis) in which a monkey showed fast and slow reactions to a sudden visual change taking place within the receptive field of neurons simultaneously recorded from the same electrode. The 0 time point in the x-axis shows when the stimulus change occurred. The top panel shows the relative LFP power when the changing stimulus was the focus of the monkey's attention, whereas the middle panel represents the power induced when it was meant to be ignored. In each, the solid line shows LFP power for trials with fast reaction times (i.e., when attention was allocated most effectively) and the dashed line shows power for trials with slow reaction times (those when attention was less effective); the gray-shaded area shows significant differences between those two trial types. Gamma power predicts faster reactions to attended stimuli and slower reactions to unattended stimuli. Bottom: Z-score of correlation coefficients across monkeys and recording sites for LFP power vs. reaction times, showing that higher gamma power predicts faster performance (i.e., smaller reaction times). B: correlation between μ oscillations and reaction times in rat engaged in a tasting session. Top trace: session-long recording trace of a single cortical LFP, showing a general change at 71 min into the session; below, two 2-s-long traces, one from early in the session and one from late, showing in detail the emergence of μ rhythms late in the session. Bottom: the 2 bars show the amount of μ rhythms in and after the 1st h; the points overlaid on the bars show the reaction times of the first purposeful orofacial movement in response to a taste stimulus. Note the correlation with μ rhythms. A modified with permission from Womelsdorf et al. (2006); B from Fontanini and Katz (2005).

Whereas θ and γ rhythms become enhanced in the LFP of an attentive animal, intermediate rhythms—7–12 Hz (α or μ)—become enhanced specifically when an animal stops attending (Fig. 2B); in the extreme, enhancement of neural activity in this frequency range evidences itself as complete attentional withdrawal and the onset of “absence seizures” (Shaw 2004, 2007). The sudden withdrawal of attention and the attendant enhancement of μ and α rhythms can happen spontaneously and transiently and, when it happens, performance on behavioral tasks suffers (Fontanini and Katz 2005; Molle et al. 2002; Slobounov et al. 2000). In essence, the prominence of θ and γ (or of α and μ)—that is, specific measures of network state—can serve as a continuous measure of an animal's level of attention (or inattention).

Neural sensory responses in awake animals prove sensitive to such changes in concurrently recorded LFPs, even when those changes appear to happen “spontaneously” in relation to general levels of attention and arousal. Investigations on the visual, auditory, and somatosensory systems demonstrate that response magnitudes rise as a function of γ power and fall as a function of α and μ power (Bezdudnaya et al. 2006; Cano et al. 2006; Fanselow and Nicolelis 1999; Womelsdorf et al. 2006). Although this phenomenon has been explicitly referred to as an active “gate” that prevents stimuli from activating the forebrain of inattentive animals (Shepherd 2005), responses to stimuli with emotional valence (such as taste stimuli) are not “gated out” during μ-related inattention; rather, inattention alters responses to these stimuli, emphasizing hedonics and palatability (the emotional content of the stimulus) over actual stimulus identity (Fontanini and Katz 2006).

Regardless of the specifics, it is clear that the sensory impact of attentional changes can be directly related to network function, which changes across a session, sometimes as the intended result of an experimenter's task manipulations and sometimes despite (or even because of) an experimenter's lack of manipulation. Attention can in fact be reconceptualized in terms of network function, which shapes the way stimuli are processed in meaningful and reasonable ways, enhancing or suppressing particular aspects of the neural responses. Thus it is important to track changes in network states, to avoid treating data collected under different conditions as differing only in random ways.

NEURAL RESPONSES CHANGE WITH LEARNING

As described earlier, attention pertains to an animal's focus on a sensory modality that has particular relevance at that point in time. Of course, in natural conditions, and in most laboratory tasks, stimulus relevance is a learned quality. It is thus reasonable to suspect that learning and learning-related response changes might themselves be explainable in terms of network states (Gilbert and Sigman 2007). Indeed, when an animal learns the relationships between stimuli and particular rewards (Maunsell 2004; Schoenbaum et al. 1998), this learning is accompanied by modifications in network states: not only does the spectral content of spontaneous (i.e., prestimulus) activity in primary sensory areas vary in ways that are similar to those caused by attention; the within- and between-region functional connectivities change over the course of learning as well (Bressler 2004; Kay and Freeman 1998; Schoenbaum et al. 2000; see Fig. 3A). Learning is in fact a network change.

FIG. 3.

FIG. 3.

Learning and multisensory integration modulate intra- and interareal connectivity. A: changes in spontaneous functional connectivity: representative cross-correlograms between pairs of neurons within amygdala (A1) and orbitofrontal cortex (A2) during different phases of the learning and reversal of a 2-odor go/no-go discrimination task. Cross-correlations can occur for some neurons on a fast (top row of each panel) timescale and for others can be slower (bottom row). Cross-correlation peaks are modulated upward or downward depending on the different phases of learning. B: multisensory integration between ecologically meaningful stimuli modulates the functional coupling between auditory cortex and superior temporal sulcus. Top panels: cross-spectogram between electrodes positioned in each of the 2 areas following the presentation of a stimulus representing the face of a monkey performing a call (left) or a monkey face plus the sound of the corresponding call (right). Bottom, left: cross-spectra for different frequencies for 300 ms after stimulus onset, showing that gamma power in these auditory regions is significantly higher with both face and voice than with voice or face alone (or for voice matched with a dynamic disc stimulus mimicking mouth opening). Right: the average normalized cross-spectra in the gamma band (55–95 Hz). A adapted from Schoenbaum et al. (2000); B adapted from Ghazanfar et al. (2008).

Of course, learning has long been known to modulate neural responses to task-related stimuli. In studies on human subjects, stimuli are perceived more according to expectation than reality and these perceptions are tracked by neural activity (functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI] blood oxygenation level–dependent signals) in sensory areas (de Araujo et al. 2005; Nitschke et al. 2006). It is not only the coding of expected stimuli that is changed by learning, however: at least in the task context, the neural response to the cue stimulus itself changes, as well, according to its learned predictive value (Schoenbaum and Roesch 2005). In the olfactory cortex, for instance, neurons alter their responsiveness to two different odors as the animal learns within an experimental session the predicted outcome of each of them; furthermore, responses reverse if and when the odor cue-outcome association is reversed (Calu et al. 2007; Roesch et al. 2007). A similar phenomenon has also been observed in the olfactory bulb, where spiking responses of neurons can be ascribed as much to the predictive value of a stimulus as to its sensory quality (Kay and Laurent 1999). It is reasonable to suggest that these changes in responding to stimuli with learned relevance are a direct function of the spontaneous and driven network state changes described in the preceding paragraph—a possibility for which direct evidence has recently been collected, in a study showing that taste learning is accompanied by changes in amygdalo–cortical interactions (Grossman et al. 2008).

Even when reward and punishment do not drive explicit learning—when features of the environment naturally co-occur, for instance—sensory responses change in tight conjunction with alterations of network states. Primates and rodents are in fact exquisitely sensitive to the coherent statistics of the environment, even when the interstimulus relationships are unrelated to the attainment of reward (and even when the subjects cannot consciously identify these relationships; see Arcediano and Miller 2002; Orban et al. 2008; Saffran et al. 1996). Particularly clear examples of this phenomenon can be found in studies of multimodal stimuli, which occur together by virtue of being part of the same real-world event. Human infants quickly learn the importance of multimodal coherence (Bahrick et al. 2004); such multimodal interactions appear to modulate single-unit responses in such a way that responses to each unimodal stimulus are altered by the presence of the other (Ghazanfar and Schroeder 2006; Ghazanfar et al. 2005). Although no network changes were specifically observed in the background activity preceding the presentation of multimodal stimuli in these studies, a more recent paper demonstrates that the spectral signature of LFP responses in A1 is modified by the simultaneous presentation of the visual stimulus with the auditory stimulus (Ghazanfar et al. 2008; Fig. 3B).

In summary, learning-related response changes that are usually discussed as a function of plasticity at discrete and specific synapses may in fact be better thought of as secondary to distributed changes in network and neural function (Maffei and Turrigiano 2008; Maffei et al. 2004)—modifications that are themselves a function of more spatially diffuse patterns of plasticity at multiple levels of the circuit or (in the case of attention) of more transient phenomena such as phasic activation of neuromodulatory nuclei (Bouret and Sara 2005). Learning is just another example of network function driving sensory function, with synaptic plasticity serving to shift the network into new states such that handling of incoming stimuli is consistently updated by experience.

SENSORY RESPONSES CHANGE FROM MOMENT TO MOMENT

The work described in previous sections demonstrates that experience and context—even subtle experience and ephemeral and/or internal context—can have a powerful impact on the states of sensory networks and thus on the processing of specific sensory stimuli. Carried to their logical extreme, these findings have implications that are both exciting and worrying. Since experience accrues gradually and since internal and external contexts can effectively change from moment to moment, the possibility arises that nonrandom network state shifts may develop quickly and capriciously and that sensory function may therefore be subject to trial-by-trial variations that are not appropriately described as random noise.

An ideal paradigm in which to look for such effects would be one in which the impact of experience accrues gradually across a session. The ongoing sensory experience during feeding is just such a paradigm because an animal's internal homeostasis gradually changes as the animal approaches satiety (de Araujo et al. 2006). With decreasing appetite comes changes in the animal's behavioral and neural responses to the specific food on which it is being fed (Corbit et al. 2007; Rolls et al. 1989), which relate to profound modifications of spontaneous ensemble activity across the many areas involved in feeding behaviors (orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, lateral hypothalamus, and gustatory cortex; de Araujo et al. 2006).

Even the development of familiarity is associated with modulations in the spontaneous activity of brain networks (Bouret and Sara 2005; Harley 2007), with neural plasticity (Berman and Dudai 2001), and with modified responses to stimuli: work on olfaction demonstrates that one to two odor presentations suffice to change a rat's behavioral responses to formerly novel tastes or odors (Verhagen et al. 2007); early neural responses change on the same timescales (Bazhenov et al. 2005; Stopfer and Laurent 1999; Verhagen et al. 2007). Similar findings have recently been reported in visual cortex (Yao et al. 2007).

Most surprisingly, the motivated processing of familiar stimuli may be modulated from trial to trial. It has long been assumed that sensory responses are “noisy” (Shadlen and Newsome 1998), but recent evidence suggests that this seemingly random variability may in fact reflect meaningful variation in stimulus processing: in many parts of the brain, there appear to be multiple possible responses to stimulation, one member of which is evoked in each individual trial (Fellous et al. 2004; Fig. 4A). Put another way, the difference between one response to a particular stimulus and the next may not be caused by noise, but instead may be related to some real aspect of brain function.

FIG. 4.

FIG. 4.

Trial-to-trial variability in neuronal responses. A, top: raster plot of a neuron in monkey's middle temporal area (MT) responding to multiple presentations of a moving Gabor stimulus. Each tick mark is an action potential. The middle panels show in detail the spikes occurring in the dotted boxes. The bottom panels are representations of the same trials as in the middle panels, but reordered according to a clustering algorithm. Note that trials are now grouped into subtypes according to similarity of responses. B: population responses to tastes in the rat gustatory cortex go through stimulus-specific sequences of coherent network states. Top: raster plots showing the responses of 4 gustatory cortical neurons to multiple presentations of citric acid (9 trials). y-axis: trials; x-axis: time. Bottom: 4 representative citric acid trials showing the taste- and trial-specific coherent activity of the ensemble of simultaneously recorded neurons (the neurons represented in the top are the first 4 starting from the bottom). The overlaid shadowing represents network states characterized in terms of specific patterns of network activity. A adapted from Fellous et al. (2004); B adapted from Jones et al. (2007).

The evidence suggests that, like the response modulations discussed in previous sections, the source of this trial-to-trial variability of sensory responses may be spontaneously generated network states. fMRI imaging of human subjects demonstrates that large, distributed neural systems fluctuate in coherent fashion from moment to moment during periods between stimulus presentations (Fox et al. 2006, 2007; Raichle et al. 2001). Similar fluctuations, observed in intrinsic signal images of cortex in anesthetized primates (Kenet et al. 2003; Vincent et al. 2007), account for a great deal of the variability in ensemble responses themselves (Arieli et al. 1996) and, in fact, spontaneously attain states that look remarkably similar to stimulus responses themselves (Kenet et al. 2003). Individual sensory responses appear to have as much to do with the coherent activity of the active networks as with the stimulus itself (see also Fiser et al. 2004).

Recent data from our lab demonstrate that the trial-to-trial fluctuations in sensory responses are not simply a by-product of network activity—they in fact represent the very essence of network dynamics in action (Fig. 4B). We have shown that trial-to-trial differences in single-neuron taste responses in cortex—differences that appear at first blush to be noise—are actually coupled across neurons in simultaneously recorded ensembles. When the activity of each neuron is related to that of each other neuron in the ensemble, rather than to trial onset, this network activity relays more information about the stimulus above and beyond that available in across-trial averages, even for the entire ensemble (Jones et al. 2007).

These ensemble data “close the loop” between network and sensory activity, directly arguing for network characterizations of sensory responses. Furthermore, they point the way out of the conundrum that develops when changes in network state are recognized to be a moment-to-moment phenomenon: by explicitly considering networks as a formative factor in sensory responses, information need not be lost in across-trial averages; when the importance of network processing is recognized and accounted for, the observed variance in sensory responses decreases. This work takes account of the fact that response variability is related to variability in network function, by quantifying sensory activity in terms of explicit network measures, rather than in terms of across-trial averages of single-neuron activity.

CONCLUDING COMMENTS: WHY VARIABILITY?

In the preceding, we suggest that a wide range of phenomena typically considered in isolation from each other—depth of anesthesia, phases of sleep, trial-to-trial variations in levels of consciousness, attention, expectation, and learning—can be reconceptualized as examples of a single overarching process whereby network preconfiguration shapes neural processing (Fig. 5). An essential component of this view is the reconceptualization of synaptic plasticity as not an end in and of itself (e.g., as a “neural trace”) but rather as a tool in the service of network configuration; the functionally active synaptic circuitry of a sensory area, which ultimately manifests itself in specific patterns of spontaneous activity and promotes a particular responsiveness to sensory stimuli, is the result of sensory context, environmental contingencies, and experience.

FIG. 5.

FIG. 5.

Dependence of sensory responses on anatomical and functional connectivity. A: schematic representing evoked activity as depending only on anatomical connectivity: the stimulus activates the cells directly connected to the periphery that, in turn, activate those to which they are more strongly connected. In this case repeated presentations of sensory stimuli always evoke the same responses. B: cartoon summarizing the view emerging from this review: the state of the network varies in parallel with the behavioral state of the animal. Functional connectivity and background activity of the neurons composing the network determine the shape of the evoked response.

If it is true that sensory responsiveness is exquisitely sensitive to network fluctuations at work on the moment-to-moment timescale, however, an obvious question arises: How are we to interpret these results from an organismal perspective? Could it be that sensory variability is an epiphenomen, a useless (in fact, a seemingly counterproductive) result of networks fluctuating for no specific reason other than its organization?

A more satisfying explanation emerges within an ecological framework (Gibson 1986), from which an animal's brain is studied specifically in relation to the natural environment that surrounds the organism. In that natural environment of odors that ebb and flow, foods that vary in texture and flavor, and moving stimuli that cause ever-changing acoustic and visual experiences, stimulus variability is the name of the game. Just as it is impossible, as Heraclitus pointed out, to step into the same river twice, it is also highly unlikely that an unrestrained perceiver will apprehend exactly the same stimulus more than once. Even if the very same stimulus is presented multiple times to immobile subjects, however, there is no reason to believe that each presentation will be experienced in the same way (Lutz and Thompson 2003; Lutz et al. 2002). The same sensory stimulus has multiple dimensions and meanings, each of which will be differentially evoked dependent on the internal milieu—where the subject's “head is at.” The experience of stimulus invariance is simply not something that brains have evolved to handle; capturing stimulus variability, on the other hand, has survival value because it maximizes the experiential content of repeated sensory presentations.

Immersed in this dynamic environment, the subject itself adds variability by engaging in complex and time-varying motor behaviors (see examples from the realm of searching, foraging, and feeding; Edwards et al. 2007; Garcia et al. 2007; Lum et al. 2005), a tendency that is adaptively advantageous in situations of environmental unpredictability (Belanger and Willis 1996) and that in fact matches the variability of motor output to the variability of sensory input (Bernstein 1967; Churchland et al. 2007).

Irrespective of the value of this speculation, however, one thing is clear: the fact that single-neuron responses are a function of network states is not merely a “footnote” or “wrinkle” on sensory processing. It is, instead, an important determinant of sensory processing and of the matching of that sensory processing to the specifics of context. It is, quite possibly, the most important aspect of the nature of sensory processing, equal in importance to the sensory input itself.

GRANTS

This work was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grants DC-006666 and DC-007102 to D. B. Katz and DC-008885 to A. Fontanini. A. Fontanini was also supported by the Sloan–Swartz Center for Theoretical Neuroscience at Brandeis.

Acknowledgments

We thank Drs. Arianna Maffei, Asif Ghazanfar, and Benjamin Rubin for helpful comments.

The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

REFERENCES

  • Adrian 1950.Adrian ED The electrical activity of the mammalian olfactory bulb. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 2: 377–388, 1950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Aguilar and Castro-Alamancos 2005.Aguilar JR, Castro-Alamancos MA. Spatiotemporal gating of sensory inputs in thalamus during quiescent and activated states. J Neurosci 25: 10990–11002, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Arcediano and Miller 2002.Arcediano F, Miller RR. Some constraints for models of timing: a temporal coding hypothesis perspective. Learn Motiv 33: 105–123, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • Arieli et al. 1996.Arieli A, Sterkin A, Grinvald A, Aertsen A. Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science 273: 1868–1871, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bahrick et al. 2004.Bahrick LE, Lickliter R, Flom R. Intersensory redundancy guides the development of selective attention, perception, and cognition in infancy. Curr Direct Psychol Sci 13: 99–102, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • Bazhenov et al. 2005.Bazhenov M, Stopfer M, Sejnowski TJ, Laurent G. Fast odor learning improves reliability of odor responses in the locust antennal lobe. Neuron 46: 483–492, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bazhenov et al. 2002.Bazhenov M, Timofeev I, Steriade M, Sejnowski TJ. Model of thalamocortical slow-wave sleep oscillations and transitions to activated states. J Neurosci 22: 8691–8704, 2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Belanger and Willis 1996.Belanger JH, Willis MA. Adaptive control of odor-guided locomotion: behavioral flexibility as an antidote to environmental unpredictability. Adapt Behav 4: 217–253, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • Berman and Dudai 2001.Berman DE, Dudai Y. Memory extinction, learning anew, and learning the new: dissociations in the molecular machinery of learning in cortex. Science 291: 2417–2419, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bernstein 1967.Bernstein NA The Coordination and Regulation of Movements. London: Pergamon, 1967.
  • Bezdudnaya et al. 2006.Bezdudnaya T, Cano M, Bereshpolova Y, Stoelzel CR, Alonso JM, Swadlow HA. Thalamic burst mode and inattention in the awake LGNd. Neuron 49: 421–432, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bouret and Sara 2005.Bouret S, Sara SJ. Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function. Trends Neurosci 28: 574–582, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bressler 2004.Bressler SL Inferential constraint sets in the organization of visual expectation. Neuroinformatics 2: 227–238, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Brunel 2000.Brunel N Dynamics of networks of randomly connected excitatory and inhibitory spiking neurons. J Physiol (Paris) 94: 445–463, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Buschman and Miller 2007.Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315: 1860–1862, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Buzsáki 2006.Buzsáki G Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press, 2006.
  • Calu et al. 2007.Calu DJ, Roesch MR, Stalnaker TA, Schoenbaum G. Associative encoding in posterior piriform cortex during odor discrimination and reversal learning. Cereb Cortex 17: 1342–1349, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cano et al. 2006.Cano M, Bezdudnaya T, Swadlow HA, Alonso JM. Brain state and contrast sensitivity in the awake visual thalamus. Nat Neurosci 9: 1240–1242, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Castro-Alamancos 2004.Castro-Alamancos MA Absence of rapid sensory adaptation in neocortex during information processing states. Neuron 41: 455–464, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Chance et al. 2002.Chance FS, Abbott LF, Reyes AD. Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron 35: 773–782, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Churchland et al. 2007.Churchland MM, Yu BM, Sahani M, Shenoy KV. Techniques for extracting single-trial activity patterns from large-scale neural recordings. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17: 609–618, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Corbit et al. 2007.Corbit LH, Janak PH, Balleine BW. General and outcome-specific forms of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer: the effect of shifts in motivational state and inactivation of the ventral tegmental area. Eur J Neurosci 26: 3141–3149, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • de Araujo et al. 2006.de Araujo IE, Gutiérrez R, Oliveira-Maia AJ, Pereira A Jr, Nicolelis MA, Simon SA. Neural ensemble coding of satiety states. Neuron 51: 483–494, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • de Araujo et al. 2005.de Araujo IE, Rolls ET, Velazco MI, Margot C, Cayeux I. Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron 46: 671–679, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Destexhe and Sejnowski 2001.Destexhe A, Sejnowski TJ. Thalamocortical Assemblies. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press, 2001.
  • Edeline et al. 2001.Edeline JM, Dutrieux G, Manunta Y, Hennevin E. Diversity of receptive field changes in auditory cortex during natural sleep. Eur J Neurosci 14: 1865–1880, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Edeline et al. 2000.Edeline JM, Manunta Y, Hennevin E. Auditory thalamus neurons during sleep: changes in frequency selectivity, threshold, and receptive field size. J Neurophysiol 84: 934–952, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Edwards et al. 2007.Edwards AM, Phillips RA, Watkins NW, Freeman MP, Murphy EJ, Afanasyev V, Buldyrev SV, da Luz MG, Raposo EP, Stanley HE, Viswanathan GM. Revisiting Levy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer. Nature 449: 1044–1048, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ekstrom et al. 2005.Ekstrom AD, Caplan JB, Ho E, Shattuck K, Fried I, Kahana MJ. Human hippocampal theta activity during virtual navigation. Hippocampus 15: 881–889, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Engel et al. 2001.Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 704–716, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fan et al. 2007.Fan J, Byrne J, Worden MS, Guise KG, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Posner MI. The relation of brain oscillations to attentional networks. J Neurosci 27: 6197–6206, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fanselow and Nicolelis 1999.Fanselow EE, Nicolelis MA. Behavioral modulation of tactile responses in the rat somatosensory system. J Neurosci 19: 7603–7616, 1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fellous et al. 2004.Fellous JM, Tiesinga PH, Thomas PJ, Sejnowski TJ. Discovering spike patterns in neuronal responses. J Neurosci 24: 2989–3001, 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fiser et al. 2004.Fiser J, Chiu C, Weliky M. Small modulation of ongoing cortical dynamics by sensory input during natural vision. Nature 431: 573–578, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fontanini and Bower 2005.Fontanini A, Bower JM. Variable coupling between olfactory system activity and respiration in ketamine/xylazine anesthetized rats. J Neurophysiol 93: 3573–3581, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fontanini and Katz 2005.Fontanini A, Katz DB. 7 to 12 Hz activity in rat gustatory cortex reflects disengagement from a fluid self-administration task. J Neurophysiol 93: 2832–2840, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fontanini and Katz 2006.Fontanini A, Katz DB. State-dependent modulation of time-varying gustatory responses. J Neurophysiol 96: 3183–3193, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fox et al. 2007.Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. Intrinsic fluctuations within cortical systems account for intertrial variability in human behavior. Neuron 56: 171–184, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fox et al. 2006.Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Zacks JM, Raichle ME. Coherent spontaneous activity accounts for trial-to-trial variability in human evoked brain responses. Nat Neurosci 9: 23–25, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Friedberg et al. 1999.Friedberg MH, Lee SM, Ebner FF. Modulation of receptive field properties of thalamic somatosensory neurons by the depth of anesthesia. J Neurophysiol 81: 2243–2252, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fritz et al. 2005.Fritz JB, Elhilali M, Shamma SA. Differential dynamic plasticity of A1 receptive fields during multiple spectral tasks. J Neurosci 25: 7623–7635, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fritz et al. 2007.Fritz JB, Elhilali M, Shamma SA. Adaptive changes in cortical receptive fields induced by attention to complex sounds. J Neurophysiol 98: 2337–2346, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gaese and Ostwald 2001.Gaese BH, Ostwald J. Anesthesia changes frequency tuning of neurons in the rat primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 86: 1062–1066, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Garcia et al. 2007.Garcia R, Moss F, Nihongi A, Strickler JR, Goller S, Erdmann U, Schimansky-Geier L, Sokolov IM. Optimal foraging by zooplankton within patches: the case of Daphnia. Math Biosci 207: 165–188, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ghazanfar et al. 2008.Ghazanfar AA, Chandrasekaran C, Logothetis NK. Interactions between the superior temporal sulcus and auditory cortex mediate dynamic face/voice integration in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci 28: 4457–4469, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ghazanfar et al. 2005.Ghazanfar AA, Maier JX, Hoffman KL, Logothetis NK. Multisensory integration of dynamic faces and voices in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. J Neurosci 25: 5004–5012, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ghazanfar and Schroeder 2006.Ghazanfar AA, Schroeder CE. Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn Sci 10: 278–285, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gibson 1986.Gibson JJ The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1986.
  • Gilbert and Sigman 2007.Gilbert CD, Sigman M. Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron 54: 677–696, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Grossman et al. 2008.Grossman SA, Fontanini A, Weiskopf JS, Katz DB. Learning-related plasticity of temporal coding in simultaneously-recorded amygdala-cortical ensembles. J Neurosci 28: 2864–2873, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harley 2007.Harley CW Norepinephrine and the dentate gyrus. Prog Brain Res 163: 299–318, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hill and Tononi 2005.Hill S, Tononi G. Modeling sleep and wakefulness in the thalamocortical system. J Neurophysiol 93: 1671–1698, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Jaeger and Bower 1999.Jaeger D, Bower JM. Synaptic control of spiking in cerebellar Purkinje cells: dynamic current clamp based on model conductances. J Neurosci 19: 6090–6101, 1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Jones et al. 2007.Jones LM, Fontanini A, Sadacca BF, Miller P, Katz DB. Natural stimuli evoke dynamic sequences of states in sensory cortical ensembles. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 18772–18777, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kastner et al. 2006.Kastner S, Schneider KA, Wunderlich K. Beyond a relay nucleus: neuroimaging views on the human LGN. Prog Brain Res 155: 125–143, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kay and Freeman 1998.Kay LM, Freeman WJ. Bidirectional processing in the olfactory-limbic axis during olfactory behavior. Behav Neurosci 112: 541–553, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kay and Laurent 1999.Kay LM, Laurent G. Odor- and context-dependent modulation of mitral cell activity in behaving rats. Nat Neurosci 2: 1003–1009, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kenet et al. 2003.Kenet T, Bibitchkov D, Tsodyks M, Grinvald A, Arieli A. Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes. Nature 425: 954–956, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kisley and Gerstein 1999.Kisley MA, Gerstein GL. Trial-to-trial variability and state-dependent modulation of auditory-evoked responses in cortex. J Neurosci 19: 10451–10460, 1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Li et al. 2006.Li W, Piech V, Gilbert CD. Contour saliency in primary visual cortex. Neuron 50: 951–962, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lum et al. 2005.Lum CS, Zhurov Y, Cropper EC, Weiss KR, Brezina V. Variability of swallowing performance in intact, freely feeding aplysia. J Neurophysiol 94: 2427–2446, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lutz et al. 2002.Lutz A, Lachaux JP, Martinerie J, Varela FJ. Guiding the study of brain dynamics by using first-person data: synchrony patterns correlate with ongoing conscious states during a simple visual task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99: 1586–1591, 2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lutz and Thompson 2003.Lutz A, Thompson E. Neurophenomenology: integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. J Conscious Stud 10: 31–52, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • MacLean et al. 2005.MacLean JN, Watson BO, Aaron GB, Yuste R. Internal dynamics determine the cortical response to thalamic stimulation. Neuron 48: 811–823, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Maffei et al. 2004.Maffei A, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG. Selective reconfiguration of layer 4 visual cortical circuitry by visual deprivation. Nat Neurosci 7: 1353–1359, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Maffei and Turrigiano 2008.Maffei A, Turrigiano G. Multiple modes of network homeostasis in visual cortical layer 2/3. J Neurosci 28: 4377–4384, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Martin et al. 2007.Martin C, Beshel J, Kay LM. An olfacto-hippocampal network is dynamically involved in odor-discrimination learning. J Neurophysiol 98: 2196–2205, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Massimini et al. 2005.Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Huber R, Esser SK, Singh H, Tononi G. Breakdown of cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Science 309: 2228–2232, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Maunsell 2004.Maunsell JH Neuronal representations of cognitive state: reward or attention? Trends Cogn Sci 8: 261–265, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Mitchell et al. 2007.Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA, Reynolds JH. Differential attention-dependent response modulation across cell classes in macaque visual area V4. Neuron 55: 131–141, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Molle et al. 2002.Molle M, Marshall L, Fehm HL, Born J. EEG theta synchronization conjoined with alpha desynchronization indicate intentional encoding. Eur J Neurosci 15: 923–928, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Murakami et al. 2005.Murakami M, Kashiwadani H, Kirino Y, Mori K. State-dependent sensory gating in olfactory cortex. Neuron 46: 285–296, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nitschke et al. 2006.Nitschke JB, Dixon GE, Sarinopoulos I, Short SJ, Cohen JD, Smith EE, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Davidson RJ. Altering expectancy dampens neural response to aversive taste in primary taste cortex. Nat Neurosci 9: 435–442, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Orban et al. 2008.Orban G, Fiser J, Aslin RN, Lengyel M. Bayesian learning of visual chunks by human observers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105: 2745–2750, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Posner 1980.Posner MI Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32: 3–25, 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Raichle et al. 2001.Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 676–682, 2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Reynolds and Chelazzi 2004.Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L. Attentional modulation of visual processing. Annu Rev Neurosci 27: 611–647, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rinberg et al. 2006.Rinberg D, Koulakov A, Gelperin A. Sparse odor coding in awake behaving mice. J Neurosci 26: 8857–8865, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Roesch et al. 2007.Roesch MR, Stalnaker TA, Schoenbaum G. Associative encoding in anterior piriform cortex versus orbitofrontal cortex during odor discrimination and reversal learning. Cereb Cortex 17: 643–652, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rolls et al. 1989.Rolls ET, Sienkiewicz ZJ, Yaxley S. Hunger modulates the responses to gustatory stimuli of single neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex of the macaque monkey. Eur J Neurosci 1: 53–60, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Saalmann et al. 2007.Saalmann YB, Pigarev IN, Vidyasagar TR. Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science 316: 1612–1615, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Saffran et al. 1996.Saffran JR, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274: 1926–1928, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Santamaria et al. 2002.Santamaria F, Jaeger D, De Schutter E, Bower JM. Modulatory effects of parallel fiber and molecular layer interneuron synaptic activity on Purkinje cell responses to ascending segment input: a modeling study. J Comput Neurosci 13: 217–235, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schoenbaum et al. 1998.Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, Gallagher M. Orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala encode expected outcomes during learning. Nat Neurosci 1: 155–159, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schoenbaum et al. 2000.Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, Gallagher M. Changes in functional connectivity in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during learning and reversal training. J Neurosci 20: 5179–5189, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schoenbaum and Roesch 2005.Schoenbaum G, Roesch M. Orbitofrontal cortex, associative learning, and expectancies. Neuron 47: 633–636, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shadlen and Newsome 1998.Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J Neurosci 18: 3870–3896, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shaw 2004.Shaw F-Z Is spontaneous high-voltage rhythmic spike discharge in Long Evans rats an absence-like seizure activity? J Neurophysiol 91: 63–77, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shaw 2007.Shaw F-Z 7–12 Hz high-voltage rhythmic spike discharges in rats evaluated by antiepileptic drugs and flicker stimulation. J Neurophysiol 97: 238–247, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shepherd 2005.Shepherd GM Perception without a thalamus: how does olfaction do it? Neuron 46: 166–168, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Slobounov et al. 2000.Slobounov SM, Fukada K, Simon R, Rearick M, Ray W. Neurophysiological and behavioral indices of time pressure effects on visuomotor task performance. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 9: 287–298, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Steriade 2000.Steriade M Corticothalamic resonance, states of vigilance and mentation. Neuroscience 101: 243–276, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Stopfer and Laurent 1999.Stopfer M, Laurent G. Short-term memory in olfactory network dynamics. Nature 402: 664–668, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Verhagen et al. 2007.Verhagen JV, Wesson DW, Netoff TI, White JA, Wachowiak M. Sniffing controls an adaptive filter of sensory input to the olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 10: 631–639, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vincent et al. 2007.Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, Zempel JM, Snyder LH, Corbetta M, Raichle ME. Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447: 83–86, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vogels et al. 2005.Vogels TP, Rajan K, Abbott LF. Neural network dynamics. Annu Rev Neurosci 28: 357–376, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Womelsdorf and Fries 2007.Womelsdorf T, Fries P. The role of neuronal synchronization in selective attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17: 154–160, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Womelsdorf et al. 2006.Womelsdorf T, Fries P, Mitra PP, Desimone R. Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection. Nature 439: 733–736, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Worgotter et al. 1998.Worgotter F, Suder K, Zhao Y, Kerscher N, Eysel UT, Funke K. State-dependent receptive-field restructuring in the visual cortex. Nature 396: 165–168, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wunderlich et al. 2005.Wunderlich K, Schneider KA, Kastner S. Neural correlates of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nat Neurosci 8: 1595–1602, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Yao et al. 2007.Yao H, Shi L, Han F, Gao H, Dan Y. Rapid learning in cortical coding of visual scenes. Nat Neurosci 10: 772–778, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neurophysiology are provided here courtesy of American Physiological Society

RESOURCES