Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
. 2006 Dec;1(3):235–241. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl029

Music and mirror neurons: from motion to ’e’motion

Istvan Molnar-Szakacs 1,2,3,, Katie Overy 4
PMCID: PMC2555420  PMID: 18985111

Abstract

The ability to create and enjoy music is a universal human trait and plays an important role in the daily life of most cultures. Music has a unique ability to trigger memories, awaken emotions and to intensify our social experiences. We do not need to be trained in music performance or appreciation to be able to reap its benefits—already as infants, we relate to it spontaneously and effortlessly. There has been a recent surge in neuroimaging investigations of the neural basis of musical experience, but the way in which the abstract shapes and patterns of musical sound can have such profound meaning to us remains elusive. Here we review recent neuroimaging evidence and suggest that music, like language, involves an intimate coupling between the perception and production of hierarchically organized sequential information, the structure of which has the ability to communicate meaning and emotion. We propose that these aspects of musical experience may be mediated by the human mirror neuron system.

Keywords: language, communication, hierarchy, Brodmann Area 44, insula


It is a common experience to be transported back through time when we hear one of our favorite songs: to the summer vacation with our best friend; to the school disco; to that cross-country drive; to our first teenage crush. Music is a universal human trait, offering the mind a unique mode of communication, a means of evoking or stimulating different emotions and, most importantly, the kind of experience that can unite and define social groups, generations and cultures. People around the world use song and dance to tell stories, to conduct rituals, to teach children about their history and culture, to entertain and to relax. We relate to music spontaneously and effortlessly, and often with an emotional response. Yet the nature of such musical experiences is extremely complex; the processing and representation of music involves a multitude of perceptual and cognitive mechanisms that have yet to be fully described. It has recently been proposed that music is best understood as a form of communication in which acoustic patterns and their auditory representations elicit a variety of conscious experiences (Bharucha et al., 2006). Here we review some recent evidence on the neural basis of musical processing in relation to two other modes of communication, language and action, both of which have been described as supported by the human mirror neuron system. We hypothesize that the powerful affective responses that can be provoked by apparently abstract musical sounds are supported by this human mirror neuron system, which may subserve similar computations during the processing of music, action and linguistic information.

The mirror neuron system has been proposed as a mechanism allowing an individual to understand the meaning and intention of a communicative signal by evoking a representation of that signal in the perceiver's own brain. Neurons with these ‘mirror’ properties have been described in both area F5 of the premotor cortex and in parietal area PF of the macaque brain (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). These visuomotor neurons discharge both when the monkey performs an action and when it observes another individual perform a similar action (di Pellegrino et al., 1992; Gallese et al., 1996; Fogassi et al., 2005). It has been suggested that parietal mirror neurons have the special property of coding motor acts as belonging to an action sequence, predicting the intended goal of a complex action (Fogassi et al., 2005). In addition, subsets of premotor mirror neurons have been shown to have audiovisual properties, and are able to represent actions independently of whether they are performed, heard or seen (Kohler et al., 2002). Thus, area F5 of the ventral premotor cortex, and area PF of the inferior parietal lobule in the monkey are considered to form a fronto-parietal mirror neuron system critical to action understanding and intention attribution (Rizzolatti et al., 2001; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Fogassi et al., 2005).

A similar fronto-parietal network, including the posterior inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44), adjacent ventral premotor cortex and the inferior parietal lobule (BA 40), appears to subserve related functions in the human brain (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). A large number of neuroimaging studies have now shown that such a human fronto-parietal mirror neuron system is engaged during action observation and imitation (Fadiga et al., 1995; Hari et al., 1998; Iacoboni et al., 1999; Johnson-Frey et al., 2003; Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2005, 2006; Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2006;). The proposed frontal mirror neuron region has also been shown to be involved in understanding the intentions behind the actions of others (Iacoboni et al., 2005). In addition, there is some recent evidence to suggest that, as in the monkey, the human mirror neuron system shows sensitivity to auditory stimuli related to actions (Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2004; Buccino et al., 2005). Thus, a range of current evidence suggests that a human fronto-parietal mirror neuron system shows properties consistent with the ability to represent the actions and intentions of others, across modalities, by recruiting one's own motor system. For the purposes of this review, we will refer to this action observation/execution matching system as the ‘human mirror neuron system’.

To date, parallels between the activity of mirror neurons recorded in the monkey and human neuroimaging findings are established primarily in the domain of action observation/execution. However, functions of this neural system have recently been linked to several high-level human cognitive functions such as empathy (Carr et al., 2003; Gallese, 2003b; Dapretto et al., 2006), theory-of-mind (Williams et al., 2001, 2006) and self-other discrimination (Uddin et al., 2005, 2006). A notion of shared representations for production and perception of speech has been previously proposed and formalized in the Motor Theory of Speech Perception (Studdert-Kennedy et al., 1970; Liberman and Mattingly, 1985; Liberman and Whalen, 2000). This theory holds that two-way communication is based on shared representation and occurs when sender and perceiver co-activate this representation. Mirror neurons may provide the neural basis of this shared representation between sender and perceiver, which Liberman postulated as the necessary prerequisite for any type of communication (Liberman and Mattingly, 1985; Rizzolatti and Arbib, 1998). Gallese (2003a) has also proposed that this perception-action link is supported by an automatic and non-conscious simulation mechanism, whereby one uses the same neural resources to represent and understand the actions of others as to perform one's own actions (Gallese, 2003a). Such a neural system allows one, in essence, to experience the mind of the other, or as the expression would have it, to ‘walk in another's shoes’.

How might this system for action representation be involved in the experience of music? Until the recent advance of recorded music and synthesized sounds (relative to human evolution), music has always been associated with motor activity. From drumming to singing to virtuosic sitar playing, the production of music involves well-coordinated motor actions that produce the physical vibrations of sound. The experience of music thus involves the perception of purposeful, intentional and organized sequences of motor acts as the cause of temporally synchronous auditory information. Thus, according to the simulation mechanism implemented by the human mirror neuron system, a similar or equivalent motor network is engaged by someone listening to singing/drumming as the motor network engaged by the actual singer/drummer; from the large-scale movements of different notes to the tiny, subtle movements of different timbres. This allows for co-representation of the musical experience, emerging out of the shared and temporally synchronous recruitment of similar neural mechanisms in the sender and the perceiver of the musical message. This shared musical representation has a similar potential for communication as shared language or action.

The connection between music and motor function is evident in all aspects of musical activity—we dance to music, we move our bodies to play musical instruments, we move our mouths and larynx to sing. A number of recent neuroimaging studies have shown that specific musical experience or expertise can modulate the activity within the fronto-parietal mirror neuron system (Haslinger et al., 2005; Bangert et al., 2006), as can dancing experience (Cross et al., 2006) and music-related motor learning (Buccino et al., 2004; Calvo-Merino et al., 2004). Other recent studies have relied on the coupling of perception and action in musical experience to investigate the neural organization of such complex behaviors as sequence learning and temporal production (Janata and Grafton, 2003).

Music, of course, is a communicative signal comprised of patterns whose performance and perception are governed by combinatorial rules, or a sort of musical grammar (Sloboda, 1985); the auditory signal is not simply organized in consecutive sequential elements, but involves hierarchical relationships. Hierarchical organization is the process of integrating lower-level units to form more complex higher-level units and in the case of music this involves combinations of both sequential and simultaneous elements such as notes, rhythms, phrases, chords, chord progressions and keys to form an overall musical structure (Lerdahl and Jackendoff, 1983). Human language is a communicative signal with a similar hierarchical structure, in which phonemes are combined to form words, phrases and sentences up to the discourse level of speech structure (Hockett, 1960). Such principles of hierarchical organization also underlie other complex abilities such as problem-solving (Newell and Simon, 1972) and tool-use (Greenfield, 1991; Greenfield et al., 2000).

Hierarchical processing in action and in linguistic grammar have been found to show both behavioral and neural similarities in developmental investigations, psycholinguistic research, cross-species comparison and neuroscientific studies (1978, 1991, 2005). Grossman (1980) used evidence from aphasic patients to suggest that Broca's area is the common neural substrate for processing hierarchy in both language and action. He found that Broca's aphasics who lack hierarchical organization in their syntactic production were also impaired in recreating hierarchically organized tree structures used by Greenfield and Schneider (1977). In contrast, fluent aphasics, who have hierarchically organized (but semantically empty) speech were able to reproduce the hierarchical structure of the models (Grossman, 1980). In the musical domain, damage to this area of the posterior inferior frontal gyrus can lead to the conjoint impairments of aphasia and amusia—a selective problem with perceiving and interpreting music (Alajouanine, 1948). Recent evidence has also shown that aphasic patients with syntactic comprehension difficulties in language exhibit similar syntactic difficulties in the domain of musical harmony (Patel, 2005).

Neuroimaging studies of language function and studies of sensory-motor integration have shown evidence of an overlap between the brain regions involved in linguistic processing and regions comprising the human mirror neuron system (Rizzolatti and Arbib, 1998; Arbib, 2005). Recent neuroimaging studies have also implicated Broca's area and its right hemisphere homologue in the perception and representation of hierarchically organized human behavior (Koechlin and Jubault, 2006; Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2006). Furthermore, it has been proposed that parallel functional segregation within Broca's area during language and motor tasks may reflect similar computations used in both language and motor control (Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2005). In accordance with these findings, neuroimaging studies have shown that Broca's area and its right-hemisphere homologue supports the processing of syntax in both language (Dapretto and Bookheimer, 1999; Friederici et al., 2000a, 2000b) and music (Patel et al., 1998; Maess et al., 2001; Koelsch et al., 2002; Patel, 2003; Tillmann et al., 2003; Koelsch and Siebel, 2005); see Figure 1. In parallel with the developmental literature on action and language, infants also seem to show implicit knowledge of principles of hierarchical organization for music, for example they are able to distinguish different scales and show preferences for consonant over dissonant tonal combinations (Trehub, 2003).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Model of the possible involvement of the human mirror neuron system in representing meaning and affective responses to music. One aspect of the experience of music involves the perception of intentional, hierarchically organized sequences of motor acts with temporally synchronous auditory information. Auditory features of the musical signal are processed primarily in the superior temporal gyrus (STG) and combined with synchronous structural features of the ‘motion’ information conveyed by the musical signal in the posterior inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44) and adjacent premotor cortex. The anterior insula forms a neural conduit between the mirror neuron system and the limbic system, allowing this information to be evaluated in relation to one's own autonomic and emotional state contributing to a complex affective response mediated by the limbic system. Possible feedback mechanisms may influence the subsequent processing of the musical signal at the immediate and more long-term timescales. The shared recruitment of this neural mechanism in both the sender and the perceiver of the musical message allows for co-representation and sharing of the musical experience. Music notes from ‘The Lady Sings the Blues’ by Billie Holiday and Herbie Nichols.

The proposal of a common neural substrate for music, language and motor functions is supported by evidence from studies of language disorders. For example, it has been shown that children with dyslexia exhibit specific timing difficulties in the domain of music (Overy et al., 2003), motor control (Fawcett and Nicolson, 1995; Wolff, 2002) and language (Tallal et al., 1993; Goswami et al., 2002) and that music lessons with dyslexic children can lead to improvements in language skills (Overy, 2003). It has also been found that patients with severe non-fluent aphasia can benefit from Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT), a highly imitative speech therapy technique based on singing. The technique has been shown to lead to speech improvements (Sparks et al., 1974), coupled with changes in the neural resources recruited during speech (Belin et al., 1996; Overy et al., 2005).

The role of the human mirror neuron system, Broca's area in particular, in mediating the sensory-motor transformations underlying imitation is already well-established (Iacoboni et al., 1999; Koski et al., 2003, 2002; Heiser et al., 2003; Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2005). The success of music/speech therapy methods such as MIT might thus be due, at least in part, to the fact that their imitative elements involve a direct transfer of sensory information to a motor plan, leading to a strong recruitment and co-activation of brain regions involved in the perception and production of both music and language.

The range of research findings discussed so far lends support to the hypothesis that the perception of action, language and music recruit shared neural resources, which appear to be located in brain regions comprising the human mirror neuron system. Based on this evidence, we propose that humans may comprehend all communicative signals, whether visual or auditory, linguistic or musical, in terms of their understanding of the motor action behind that signal, and furthermore, in terms of the intention behind that motor action. The expressive nature of any human action or vocalisation sends a signal of the intentional and emotional state of the executor, such that even footsteps can be correctly interpreted as conveying simple emotions (such as sad, happy, angry or stressed) (de Gelder, 2006). Thus, as a sentence or a musical phrase can be used to express an individual's semantic intention or emotional state, a listener can understand the intended expression of the sentence or melody, via the perceived ‘motion’ of the signal. Since the acoustic nature of music can convey pure, non-referential ‘motion’ in pitch-space and time, it can thereby convey complex and subtle qualities of human ‘e’motion, using varying complexities of structural hierarchy.

Indeed, one of the defining features of music is its ability to induce an emotional response in listeners (Gabrielsson, 2001) and one of the main reasons people give for listening to music is to experience or modulate their emotional state (Sloboda and O'Neill, 2001). Emotional responses to music are present in early life and across cultures (Balkwill and Thompson, 1999), indicating that the ability to perceive emotions in music may be innate (Zentner and Kagan, 1996; Trevarthen, 1999). Numerous measures of autonomic arousal have been used to investigate the emotion-inducing effects of music. Skin conductance responses appear to be useful indicators of musically induced emotional arousal (VanderArk and Ely, 1992, 1993), and ‘chills’ (goosebumps) can be elicited when participants are allowed to select music they find arousing (Panksepp, 1995; Gabrielsson, 2001). Neuroimaging studies of affective responses to music have revealed the involvement of a network of paralimbic and neocortical regions, including frontal pole, orbitofrontal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus/sulcus, cingulate and the precuneus (Blood et al., 1999; Blood and Zatorre, 2001; Koelsch et al., 2005; Menon and Levitin, 2005; Koelsch et al., 2006). These regions correspond well to brain regions previously associated with processing emotional states and evaluating reward, particularly in socially relevant cognitions (Adolphs, 1999, 2001, 2003; Adolphs et al., 2000).

Emotion, especially as communicated by the face, the body and the voice is an active motor process. Emotion and action are intertwined on several levels, and this motor-affective coupling may provide the neural basis of empathy (Carr et al., 2003; Leslie et al., 2004)—especially the aspect of empathy that requires no intermediary cognitive process, but rather, is our automatic and immediate ‘motor identification’ or inner imitation of the actions of others (Lipps, 1903; Gallese, 2003a). The chameleon effect, whereby empathic individuals exhibit non-conscious mimicry of the postures, mannerisms and facial expressions of others provides strong support in favor of this theory (Chartrand and Bargh, 1999). There is also recent neuroimaging evidence that the anterior insula, the right amygdala and mirror neuron areas in the posterior inferior frontal gyrus, show enhanced activity during imitation of emotional facial expressions vs simple observation, providing additional support for the role of sensorimotor-affective coupling in understanding the emotions of others (Carr et al., 2003). In addition, Adolphs and colleagues have shown that in a group of lesion patients, those with a lesion of the amygdala and the sensorimotor cortex performed the worst when asked to name or rate facial expression of emotion (Adolphs et al., 2000). Thus, it appears that the mirror neuron system is involved not only in the intersubjective representations of actions but also in emotion—representations that allow us to feel connected with other agents.

It has been suggested that the perception of emotion in music may arise in part from its relation to physical posture and gesture (Davies, 1994; Jackendoff and Lerdahl, 2006). It has also been shown that expressive music can induce subliminal facial expressions in listeners (Witvliet and Vrana, 1996), and these in turn may induce subjective and physiological emotional expressions (Ekman et al., 1983). As posture, gesture and facial expressions are important implicit cues in social communication, one can easily imagine that ‘musical gesture’ can have similar effects in communicating emotions. Two regions, including the posterior inferior frontal gyrus and the anterior insula are commonly activated during musically evoked emotional states (Koelsch et al., 2005, 2006; Menon and Levitin, 2005). These two structures may hold the key to understanding how the brain uses a simulation mechanism to represent emotional states evoked by musical experience. As described above, the posterior inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44) is the frontal component of the human fronto-parietal mirror neuron system. With its ability to link perceptual and behavioral representations of a stimulus during the perception of emotionally arousing music, the mirror neuron system may simulate an emotional state in the listener (Gridley and Hoff, 2006).

The anterior insula may also support neural representations for subjective autonomic states, including bodily states such as pain and hunger, as well as more subtle states such as perception of heart rate and emotional awareness (Craig, 2002, 2003, 2004; Critchley et al., 2004). Anatomical data shows that the insular lobe has reciprocal connections with the limbic system as well as with posterior parietal, inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex (Augustine, 1996). Through its connection to regions of motor significance, the anterior insula has been proposed to serve as the neural relay station between the human mirror neuron system linking perception and action and the limbic system involved in processing emotions (Carr et al., 2003). Thus, in the case of music, the human mirror neuron system and the limbic system may communicate through the insula to provide an automatic representation of the musical stimulus (Figure 1).

In conclusion, we propose here that in its ability to integrate and represent cross-modal information, the mirror neuron system may provide a domain-general neural mechanism for processing combinatorial rules common to language, action and music, which in turn can communicate meaning and human affect. Although it is yet to be determined which specific aspects of processing linguistic, musical or motor syntax may recruit frontal mirror neuron regions, the emerging picture from the literature suggests that the mirror neuron system provides a neural substrate for representing infinite combinations of hierarchical structures, a computation that may underlie more general cognitive abilities. There is also evidence that perhaps this region may be the source of predictive models of upcoming events in sequential processing, a feature also common to language, music and action (Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2005; Zatorre and McGill, 2005).

While the evolutionary advantage of musical ability is still under debate (Hauser and McDermott, 2003), there is growing evidence that music plays an role in cognitive development, emotion regulation and social interaction (Trevarthen, 1999; Juslin and Sloboda, 2001). We propose that the human mirror neuron system may subserve some of these effects, linking music perception, cognition and emotion via an experiential rather than a representational mechanism. A review of the literature on musically induced emotions provides support for our proposal that music can invoke motor representations of emotions by recruiting the insula, a neural relay between the limbic and motor systems. Action, language and music appear to share neural resources, and we have proposed that common features governing the use and function of these means of communication may be represented within the fronto-parietal mirror neuron system. Given that music, language and action (i) show specific and relatively fixed developmental time courses (Trehub, 2001; Greenfield, 2005); (ii) are ubiquitous means of social communication in all human societies (Brown, 1991; Fiske, 2004); and (iii) share overlapping neural resources, it follows that these human abilities may be related evolutionarily. Ultimately, as further research tests some of the hypotheses presented here, we will perhaps gain valuable insight into a most fascinating aspect of humanity: the ability to express oneself creatively.

You are the music, while the music lasts.”

       - T.S. Elliot.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ralph Adolphs and an anonymous reviewer for comments on the manuscript. We also express our appreciation to the organizers and participants of The Cold Spring Harbor Workshop on the Biology of Social Cognition, July 14-20, 2006. Financial support to attend this conference was provided to KO by the Medical Research Council, UK and to IMS by the Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and the Brain Mind Institute at the Ecole Polytechnique Federale de Lausanne (EPFL).

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

REFERENCES

  1. Adolphs R. Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Science. 1999;3:469–79. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01399-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adolphs R. The neurobiology of social cognition. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2001;11:231–9. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00202-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adolphs R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Review of Neurosciences. 2003;4:165–78. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Adolphs R, Damasio H, Tranel D, Cooper G, Damasio AR. A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:2683–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-07-02683.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alajouanine T. Aphasia and artistic realization. Brain. 1948;71:229–41. doi: 10.1093/brain/71.3.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Arbib MA. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. The Behavioral and brain sciences. 2005;28:105–124. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x05000038. discussion 125–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Augustine JR. Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans. Brain research. Brain research reviews. 1996;22:229–44. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(96)00011-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Aziz-Zadeh L, Iacoboni M, Zaidel E, Wilson S, Mazziotta J. Left hemisphere motor facilitation in response to manual action sounds. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;19:2609–12. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03348.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Aziz-Zadeh L, Koski L, Zaidel E, Mazziotta J, Iacoboni M. Lateralization of the human mirror neuron system. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:2964–70. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2921-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Balkwill L-L, Thompson WF. A cross-cultural investigation of the perception of emotion in music: psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Perception. 1999;17:43–64. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bangert M, Peschel T, Schlaug G, et al. Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: evidence from fMRI conjunction. Neuroimage. 2006;30:917–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Belin P, Van Eeckhout P, Zilbovicius M, et al. Recovery from nonfluent aphasia after melodic intonation therapy: a PET study. Neurology. 1996;47:1504–11. doi: 10.1212/wnl.47.6.1504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bharucha JJ, Curtis M, Paroo K. Varieties of musical experience. Cognition. 2006;100:131–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001;98:11818–23. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ, Bermudez P, Evans AC. Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:382–7. doi: 10.1038/7299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brown DE. Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  17. Buccino G, Riggio L, Melli G, Binkofski F, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Brain research. Cognitive brain research. 2005;24:355–63. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Buccino G, Vogt S, Ritzl A, et al. Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 2004;42:323–34. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00181-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Calvo-Merino B, Glaser DE, Grezes J, Passingham R, Haggard P. 2004. Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau MC, Mazziotta JC, Lenzi GL. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2003;100:5497–502. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0935845100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chartrand T, Bargh J. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1999;76:893–910. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Review of Neuroscience. 2002;3:655–66. doi: 10.1038/nrn894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Craig AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2003;13:500–5. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Craig AD. Human feelings: why are some more aware than others? Trends in Cognitive Science. 2004;8:239–41. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:189–95. doi: 10.1038/nn1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cross ES, Hamilton AFDC, Grafton ST. Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. NeuroImage. 2006;31:1257–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dapretto M, Bookheimer SY. Form and content: dissociating syntax and semantics in sentence comprehension. Neuron. 1999;24:427–32. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80855-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dapretto M, Davies MS, Pfeifer JH, et al. Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:28–30. doi: 10.1038/nn1611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Davies S. Musical meaning and expression. Ithaca and London: Cornell University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  30. de Gelder B. Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language. Nature Review of Neuroscience. 2006;7:242–9. doi: 10.1038/nrn1872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. di Pellegrino G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental brain research. 1992;91:176–80. doi: 10.1007/BF00230027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ekman P, Levenson RW, Friesen WV. Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science. 1983;221:1208–10. doi: 10.1126/science.6612338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Pavesi G, Rizzolatti G. Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. Journal of neurophysiology. 1995;73:2608–11. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fawcett AJ, Nicolson RI. Persisitent deficits in motor skill of children with dyslexia. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1995;27:235–40. [Google Scholar]
  35. Fiske AP. Relational Models Theory: A Contemporary Overview. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2004. Four Modes of Constituting Relationships: Consubstantial Assimilation; Space, Magnitude, Time and Force; Concrete Procedures; Abstract Symbolism. In; pp. 61–146. [Google Scholar]
  36. Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, Rozzi S, Chersi F, Rizzolatti G. Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science. 2005;308:662–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1106138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Friederici AD, Meyer M, von Cramon DY. Auditory language comprehension: an event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and language. 2000a;74:289–300. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Friederici AD, Opitz B, von Cramon DY. Segregating semantic and syntactic aspects of processing in the human brain: an fMRI investigation of different word types. Cerebral Cortex. 2000b;10:698–705. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.7.698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gabrielsson A. Music and emotion: Theory and research. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2001. Emotions in strong experiences with music. In: Juslin, P., Sloboda, J.A., editors; pp. 431–449. [Google Scholar]
  40. Gallese V. The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. (Series B: Biological sciences).Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. 2003a;358:517–28. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gallese V. The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology. 2003b;36:171–80. doi: 10.1159/000072786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain. 1996;119:593–609. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Goswami U, Thomson J, Richardson U, et al. Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: a new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2002;99:10911–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.122368599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Greenfield P. Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. London: Academic Press; 1978. Structural parallels between language and action in development. In: Lock, A., editor. [Google Scholar]
  45. Greenfield P. Language, tools and brain: the ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1991;14:531–95. [Google Scholar]
  46. Greenfield P. Action to Language via the Mirror Neuron System. Cambridge University Press; 2005. Implications of Mirror Neurons for the Ontogeny and Phylogeny of Cultural Processes: the Examples of Tools and Language. In: (Arbib, M.A., editor) [Google Scholar]
  47. Greenfield P, Schneider P. Developmental Psychology. Vol. 13. 1977. Building a tree structure: the development of hierarchical complexity and interrupted strategies in children's construction activity; pp. 299–313. [Google Scholar]
  48. Greenfield P, Maynard A, Boehm C, Schmidtling E. Biology, brains & behavior. Santa Fe: SAR Press; 2000. Cultural apprenticeship and cultural change: Tool learning and imitation in chimpanzees and humans. In. [Google Scholar]
  49. Gridley MC, Hoff R. Do mirror neurons explain misattribution of emotions in music? Perceptual and motor skills. 2006;102:600–2. doi: 10.2466/pms.102.2.600-602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Grossman M. A central processor for hierarchically-structured material: evidence from Broca's aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 1980;18:299–308. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(80)90125-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hari R, Forss N, Avikainen S, Kirveskari E, Salenius S, Rizzolatti G. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1998;95:15061–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Haslinger B, Erhard P, Altenmuller E, Schroeder U, Boecker H, Ceballos–Baumann AO. Transmodal sensorimotor networks during action observation in professional pianists. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2005;17:282–93. doi: 10.1162/0898929053124893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hauser MD, McDermott J. The evolution of the music faculty: a comparative perspective. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:663–8. doi: 10.1038/nn1080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Heiser M, Iacoboni M, Maeda F, Marcus J, Mazziotta JC. The essential role of Broca's area in imitation. The European journal of neuroscience. 2003;17:1123–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hockett CD. The origin of speech. Scientific American. 1960;203:89–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science. 1999;286:2526–8. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5449.2526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G. Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e79. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Jackendoff R, Lerdahl F. The capacity for music: what is it, and what's special about it? Cognition. 2006;100:33–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Janata P, Grafton ST. Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:682–7. doi: 10.1038/nn1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Juslin P, Sloboda JA, editors. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2001. Music and emotion: theory and research. [Google Scholar]
  61. Johnson-Frey SH, Maloof FR, Newman-Norlund R, Farrer C, Inati S, Grafton ST. Actions or hand-object interactions? Human inferior frontal cortex and action observation. Neuron. 2003;39:1053–8. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00524-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Koechlin E, Jubault T. Broca's area and the hierarchical organization of human behavior. Neuron. 2006;50:963–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Koelsch S, Siebel WA. Towards a neural basis of music perception. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2005;9:578–84. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Koelsch S, Fritz T, Schulze K, Alsop D, Schlaug G. Adults and children processing music: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2005;25:1068–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Koelsch S, Fritz T, von Cramon DY, Muller K, Friederici AD. Investigating emotion with music: an fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping. 2006;27:239–50. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Koelsch S, Gunter TC, von Cramon DY, Zysset S, Lohmann G, Friederici AD. Bach speaks: a cortical “language-network” serves the processing of music. Neuroimage. 2002;17:956–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kohler E, Keysers C, Umilta MA, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science. 2002;297:846–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1070311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Koski L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau MC, Woods RP, Mazziotta JC. Modulation of cortical activity during different imitative behaviors. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89:460–71. doi: 10.1152/jn.00248.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Koski L, Wohlschlager A, Bekkering H, et al. Modulation of motor and premotor activity during imitation of target-directed actions. Cerebral Cortex. 2002;12:847–55. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lerdahl F, Jackendoff R. A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  71. Leslie KR, Johnson-Frey SH, Grafton ST. Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy. Neuroimage. 2004;21:601–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Liberman AM, Mattingly IG. The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition. 1985;21:1–36. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90021-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Liberman AM, Whalen DH. On the relation of speech to language. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2000;4:187–96. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01471-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Lipps T. Leipzig: Engelmann, W; 1903. Einfühlung, innere nachahmung und organenempfindung. [Google Scholar]
  75. Maess B, Koelsch S, Gunter TC, Friederici AD. Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:540–5. doi: 10.1038/87502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Menon V, Levitin DJ. The rewards of music listening: response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. Neuroimage. 2005;28:175–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Molnar-Szakacs I, Iacoboni M, Koski L, Mazziotta JC. Functional Segregation within Pars Opercularis of the Inferior Frontal Gyrus: Evidence from fMRI Studies of Imitation and Action Observation. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15:986–94. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Molnar-Szakacs I, Kaplan JT, Greenfield PM, Iacoboni M. 2006. Observing Complex Action Sequences: The Role of the Fronto-Parietal Mirror Neuron System. NeuroImage doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Newell A, Simon H. Prentice-Hall; 1972. Human Problem Solving. [Google Scholar]
  80. Overy K. Dyslexia and music. From timing deficits to musical intervention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2003;999:497–505. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Overy K, Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ, Clarke EF. Dyslexia and music: measuring musical timing skills. Dyslexia. 2003;9:18–36. doi: 10.1002/dys.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Overy K, Norton A, Ozdemir E, Helm-Estabrooks N, Schlaug G. Activation of left inferior frontal gyrus after melodic intonation therapy in a Broca's aphasia patient. Washington, DC: Society for Neuroscience; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  83. Panksepp J. The emotional sources of “chills” induced by music. Music Perception. 1995;13:171–207. [Google Scholar]
  84. Patel AD. Language, music, syntax and the brain. Natural Neuroscience. 2003;6:674–81. doi: 10.1038/nn1082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Patel AD. The relationship of music to the melody of speech and to syntactic processing disorders in aphasia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2005;1060:59–70. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Patel AD, Gibson E, Ratner J, Besson M, Holcomb PJ. Processing syntactic relations in language and music: an event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1998;10:717–33. doi: 10.1162/089892998563121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Rizzolatti G, Arbib M. Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences. 1998;21:188–94. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(98)01260-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annual review of neuroscience. 2004;27:169–92. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:661–70. doi: 10.1038/35090060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Sloboda JA. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1985. The musical mind. [Google Scholar]
  91. Sloboda JA, O'Neill SA. Music and emotion: Theory and research. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2001. Emotions in everyday listening to music. In. Juslin, P., Sloboda, J.A., editors. [Google Scholar]
  92. Sparks R, Helm N, Albert M. Aphasia rehabilitation resulting from melodic intonation therapy. Cortex. 1974;10:303–16. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(74)80024-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Studdert-Kennedy M, Liberman A, Harris K, Cooper F. Motor theory of speech perception: a reply to Lane's critical review. Psychological Review. 1970;77:234–49. doi: 10.1037/h0029078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Tallal P, Miller S, Fitch RH. Neurobiological basis of speech: a case for the preeminence of temporal processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1993;682:27–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1993.tb22957.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Tillmann B, Janata P, Bharucha JJ. Activation of the inferior frontal cortex in musical priming. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2003;999:209–11. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Trehub SE. Musical predispositions in infancy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;930:1–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05721.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Trehub SE. The developmental origins of musicality. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:669–73. doi: 10.1038/nn1084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Trevarthen C. Musicae Scientiae, 1999-2000. 1999. Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. In “Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human commuincation”; pp. 157–213. [Google Scholar]
  99. Uddin LQ, Molnar-Szakacs I, Zaidel E, Iacoboni M. rTMS to the right inferior parietal lobule disrupts self-other discrimination. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2006;1:65–71. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Uddin LQ, Kaplan JT, Molnar-Szakacs I, Zaidel E, Iacoboni M. Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2005;25:926–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. VanderArk SD, Ely D. Biochemical and galvanic skin responses to music stimuli by college students in biology and music. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1992;74:1079–90. doi: 10.2466/pms.1992.74.3c.1079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. VanderArk SD, Ely D. Cortisol, biochemical, and galvanic skin responses to music stimuli of different preference values by college students in biology and music. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1993;77:227–34. doi: 10.2466/pms.1993.77.1.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Williams JH, Whiten A, Suddendorf T, Perrett DI. Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2001;25:287–95. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00014-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Williams JH, Waiter GD, Gilchrist A, Perrett DI, Murray AD, Whiten A. Neural mechanisms of imitation and ‘mirror neuron’ functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:610–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Witvliet C, Vrana S. The emotional impact of instrumental music on affect ratings, facial EMG, autonomic response, and the startle reflex: Effects of valence and arousal. Psychophysiology, Supplement. 1996:91. [Google Scholar]
  106. Wolff PH. Timing precision and rhythm in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing. 2002;15:179–206. [Google Scholar]
  107. Zatorre R, McGill J. Music, the food of neuroscience? Nature. 2005;434:312–5. doi: 10.1038/434312a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Zentner MR, Kagan J. Perception of music by infants. Nature. 1996;383:29. doi: 10.1038/383029a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social cognitive and affective neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES