Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
. 2006 Dec;1(3):271–274. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl040

Prior experience as a stimulus category confound: an example using facial expressions of emotion

Leah H Somerville 1, Paul J Whalen 1,
PMCID: PMC2555423  PMID: 18985113

Abstract

Facial expressions of emotion represent a stimulus set widely used to assess a broad range of psychological processes. However, a consideration of systematic differences between expression categories, other than differences relating to characteristics of the expressions themselves, has remained largely unaddressed. By collecting experience rankings in a large sample of undergraduates, we observed that the amount of reported experience individuals have had with different facial expressions of emotion systematically differed between all expression categories. These findings shed light on the potential for identifying confounds inherent to comparing some stimulus categories and, in this case, may aid in the interpretation of observed between-expression category findings.

Keywords: emotion, fMRI, facial expressions, novelty


Humans comprise an especially challenging subject pool precisely because we as experimenters lack the ability to control their prior experiences, a form of control that is more readily employed in animal studies. As Tolman put it, ‘rats live in cages; they do not go on binges the night before one has planned the experiment’ (Tolman, 1945, p. 166). Cognitive neuroscientists have devised clever experimental manipulations that have offered important insights into the neural substrates of everything from memory and attention to mood and moral reasoning. But given the experientially ‘tainted’ nature of our subjects of study, careful consideration should be given to whether prior experience differs systematically between any two experimental comparison conditions. To this end, the current report summarizes and discusses one such instance from the realm of affective neuroscience, namely, differential prior experience with primary emotional facial expression categories.

Images depicting facial expressions of emotion comprise standardized stimulus sets that are frequently employed to assess a wide range of psychological functions (e.g. Ekman and Friesen, 1976; Russell, 1994). While one can question the ecological validity of presenting subjects with static 2D images of expressions, a vast amount of useful and replicable data have been gleaned from these stimuli. However, a consideration of systematic differences between expression categories, other than differences relating to characteristics of the expressions themselves, has remained largely unaddressed.

Psychologists studying emotion have long pointed out that as important as fear states are, they are relatively rare events (Cannon, 1927). William James summarized the issue as follows: ‘The progress from brute to man is characterized by nothing so much as by the decrease in frequency of proper occasions for fear’ (James, 1890, p. 415). If it is true that we experience the feeling of fear less often than other emotions, then it follows that we would see fearful expressions on the faces of others less often than other expressions over the course of our lifetime (Bond and Siddle, 1996; Whalen, 1998). Such a familiarity bias would be an important caveat to consider when interpreting dependent measurements associated with responses to fearful facial expressions in comparison to other more frequently encountered expressions. Accordingly, the current report characterizes the past experience individuals report having with distinct primary expression categories in their lifetimes.

METHODS

Subjects were 1537 undergraduate students tested in groups of ∼200. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the Institutional Review Board of the University of Wisconsin. After excluding improperly ranked surveys, 1393 usable subjects remained, consisting of 774 females and 619 males. Subjects were presented with a list of the six primary facial expression labels (Ekman and Friesen, 1976) and the neutral expression label, and were instructed to rank order these seven expression labels based upon the frequency with which they believed they had encountered these expressions in their lifetimes (1 = seen the most, 7 = seen the least). To increase clarity, two words were presented together to describe each expression category.

RESULTS

Label, mean ranking and standard deviation (in parentheses) of rankings for each expression label were as follows: happy/smiling: 1.86 (1.26); neutral/expressionless: 2.27 (1.75); sad/unhappy: 3.65 (1.25); angry/mad: 4.03 (1.29); surprised/startled: 4.89 (1.52); disgusted/yuck: 5.37 (1.40) and fearful/afraid: 5.93 (1.33). A Friedman rank test for related samples yielded a significant main effect for expression, indicating that the distributions of ranks differed across expression XF2(6) = 4218.26, P < 0.001, r = 0.87). These ranking distributions are presented as a frequency histogram by expression in Figure 1. Given the significant omnibus effect, Wilcoxon signed rank tests were performed on each pair of neighboring expressions to determine whether the distributions of ranks differed significantly from one another. Reported results are corrected for multiple comparisons, and effect size (r) is denoted after significance values. The distributions of all neighboring pairs differed significantly from one another: happy/neutral: Z = 4.92, P < 0.0001, r = 0.132; neutral/sad: Z = 19.15, P < 0.0001, r = 0.51; sad/angry: Z = 7.85, P < 0.0001, r = 0.21; angry/surprise: Z = 13.28, P < 0.0001, r = 0.36; surprise/disgust: Z = 8.09, P < 0.0001, r = 0.22; disgust/fear: Z = 85.45, P < 0.0001, r = 0.29. Separate analyses of male and female subjects produced the same ranked order of expressions.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Distributions of experience rankings with seven facial expressions of emotion. Distributions are ordered from left to right according to frequency ranking (1 = seen the most, 7 = seen the least). Y-axis denotes frequency.

DISCUSSION

Here we define the pattern of reported differential prior experience with distinct facial expressions of emotion in a large sample of undergraduates. Rank position was significantly different between all adjacently ranked expression categories, with fearful expressions consistently reported as having been seen the least in one's lifetime, and happy expressions being seen the most. These data offer a caution for behavioral and neuroimaging studies seeking to compare responses to one expression with that of another, and more generally to any experimental paradigm involving conditions where stimuli fundamentally differ on the dimension of prior experience.

Here we consider the specific example of amygdala reactivity to human faces. In human neuroimaging studies, fearful facial expressions evoke greater responsivity in the amygdala compared to neutral or happy expressions (Morris et al., 1996; Whalen et al., 1998), consistent with the notion that the amygdala responds to fearful faces more because they have predicted negative outcomes in the past. The present report raises the possibility that greater amygdala reactivity to fearful faces might also reflect a heightened response to an environmental stimulus that has been encountered less frequently in one's lifetime. Such a notion would be consistent with experimental findings showing that the amygdala is more responsive to novel compared to familiar faces (DuBois et al., 1999; Schwartz et al., 2003; Wright et al., 2003).

While the present results have specific implications for interpreting data that vary across facial expression categories, they imply that for any measured difference between facial categories, one should consider the possible confound of prior life experience. For example, greater amygdala activity has been reported in Caucasian-American subjects while viewing unfamiliar African-American faces than unfamiliar Caucasian-American faces (Hart et al., 2000; Phelps et al., 2000; Cunningham et al., 2004), which could be attributed to the relative novelty of African-American faces to Caucasian-American subjects (but see Lieberman et al., 2005). Supporting this possibility is a study by Phelps et al. (2000) that attempted to equate novelty of different race faces by using only pictures of highly familiar, famous Caucasian-American and African-Americans, and found comparable amygdala activity to the two stimulus conditions (Phelps et al., 2000). Further, while amygdala activation to African-American faces has been shown to vary with scores on the Implicit Association Task (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998), a purported measure of implicit racism, it is possible to interpret IAT effects in terms of familiarity, where reaction time biases track categories that have been encountered more frequently (Rothermund and Wentura, 2004; Kinoshita and Peek-O’Leary, 2005).

Within the specific example of facial expression categories, some experimental designs have eliminated this alternative explanation by utilizing paradigms that manipulated other aspects of the face presentation, such as eye gaze (Adams and Kleck, 2003), while holding facial expression itself constant. For example, a neuroimaging study by Kim et al. (2004) used a paradigm employing only surprised faces while manipulating the valence of contextual information describing these faces. The resulting neural activity observed reflected differences in the interpretation of valence associated with these expressions (as a function of context) and could not be explained by the differences in experience reported here (see also Adams et al., 2003).

In addition, differential prior experience with facial expression categories would likely influence recognition difficulty. For example, labeling accuracy for the facial expressions depicted in the Pictures of Facial Affect (Ekman and Friesen, 1976), a commonly used facial expression stimulus set, is best for happy expressions (95–98% agreement) and worst for fearful expressions (77–87% agreement) (Ekman and Friesen, 1976; Russell, 1994). Although cross-cultural studies indicate that accuracy decreases when judging emotions of faces in other cultures, happiness and fear are still the most and the least accurately identified, respectively (for a review, see Elfenbein and Ambady, 2002). These data resonate with expression labelling accuracy rates observed in brain-damaged patients, in that negative expressions are consistently labelled less accurately than positive expressions (Braun et al., 2005; Broks et al., 1998; Rapcsak et al., 2000; Rosen et al., 2006).

It should be noted that these data rely on subjects’ ability to accurately quantify this facet of past experience. These findings would be supplemented by additional studies assessing the incidence of observed expressions across a number of real-life social settings (see Bond and Siddle, 1996 for preliminary data that address this issue). In addition, it would be useful to determine whether the same ranked order would be found using different eliciting stimuli (e.g. different category labels, using faces rather than verbal labels as items to rank), and other formats of evaluation such as use of a Likert scale or free response format. Indeed, a free response format (rather than the forced choice format) might reveal other experienced expression categories not considered here, such as social emotions like embarrassment (Keltner and Buswell, 1997).

In the future, the employment of this scale could provide insight into the psychological state of the individual, or group, of interest. An individual's rank ordering, if abnormal with respect to an overall sample, could be related to differences in one's past experiences and/or be predictive of certain personality traits or tendencies toward psychopathology. For example, a small minority of our sample rated sad faces as being seen the most. Future studies could aim to show that such a response relates to symptoms of depression, since prior research has demonstrated a tendency for depressed individuals to over-ascribe sad emotion to faces (Gur et al., 1992). A similarly small number of subjects rated angry faces as being seen the most. Such a response could be shown to be related to experiences during one's upbringing, given that individuals raised in an abusive or neglectful environment ascribe anger in a presented face at a lower threshold (Pollak and Sinha, 2002).

Numerous studies have demonstrated robust differences in behavioral and psychophysiological response to these primary facial expression categories. The present demonstration that these categories differ based upon reported past experience further complicates the interpretation of observed between-category findings. Our hope is that further research on this issue will lead to an improved understanding of these ubiquitously employed stimuli, as well as our processing of these primary expression categories. More generally, these data suggest that differential past experience will be an important variable to consider when comparing different stimulus categories within cognitive neuroscience research.

Acknowledgments

We thank George Wolford for statistical advice. This work was supported by NIMH01866 and NIMH069315.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

REFERENCES

  1. Adams RB, Gordon HL, Baird AA, Ambady N, Kleck RE. Effects of gaze on amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. Science. 2003;300:1536. doi: 10.1126/science.1082244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adams RB, Kleck RE. Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science. 2003;14:644–7. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1479.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bond NW, Siddle DAT. The preparedness account of social phobia:Some data and alternative explanations. In: Rapee RM, editor. Current Controversies in the Anxiety Disorders, New York: Guilford Press; 1996. pp. 291–316. [Google Scholar]
  4. Braun M, Traue HC, Frisch S, Deighton RM, Kessler H. Emotion recognition in stroke patients with left and right hemispheric lesion: Results with a new instrument – the FEEL test. Brain and Cognition. 2005;58:193–201. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Broks P, Young AW, Maratos EJ, et al. Face processing impairments after encephalitis: Amygdala damage and recognition of fear. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:59–70. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00105-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cannon WB. The James-Lange theory of emotion: A critical examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology. 1927;39:10–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Raye CL, Gatenby CJ, Gore JC, Banaji MR. Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science. 2004;15:806–13. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00760.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. DuBois S, Rossion B, Schlitz C, et al. Effect of familiarity on the processing of human faces. Neuroimage. 1999;9:278–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Pictures of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  10. Elfenbein HA, Ambady N. On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:203–35. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JKL. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1464–80. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gur RC, Erwin RJ, Gur RE, Zwil AS, Heimberg C, Kraemer HC. Facial emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings from depression. Psychiatry Research. 1992;42:241–51. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(92)90116-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hart AJ, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Fisher H, Rauch SL. Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. Neuroreport. 2000;11:2351–5. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008030-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. James W. The Principles of Psychology. Vol. II. New York: Henry Holt; 1890. [Google Scholar]
  15. Keltner D, Buswell BN. Embarrassment: Its distinct form and appeasement functions. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;122:250–70. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.122.3.250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kim H, Somerville LH, Johnstone T, et al. Contextual modulation of fMRI responsivity to surprised faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:1730–45. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kinoshita S, Peek-O'Leary M. Does the compatibility effect in the race Implicit Association Test reflect familiarity or affect? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2005;12:442–52. doi: 10.3758/bf03193786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lieberman MD, Hariri A, Jarcho JM, Eisenberger NI, Bookheimer SY. An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African-American and Caucasian-American individuals. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:720–2. doi: 10.1038/nn1465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, et al. A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature. 1996;383:812–5. doi: 10.1038/383812a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Phelps EA, O'Connor KJ, Cunningham WA, et al. Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;125:729–38. doi: 10.1162/089892900562552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Pollak SD, Sinha P. Effects of early experience on children's recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:784–91. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Rapcsak SZ, Galper SR, Comer JF, et al. Fear recognitions after focal brain damage: A cautionary note. Neurology. 2000;54:575–81. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.3.575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Rosen HJ, Wilson MR, Schauer GF, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired recognition of emotion in dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:365–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Rothermund K, Wentura D. Underlying processes in the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Dissociating effects of salience and valence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004;133:139–65. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Russell JA. Is there a universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;118:379–91. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Schwartz CE, Wright CI, Shin LM, et al. Differential amygdalar response to novel versus newly familiar neutral faces: a functional MRI probe developed for studying inhibited temperament. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;53:854–62. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01906-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Tolman EC. A stimulus-expectancy need-cathexis psychology. Science. 1945;101:160–6. doi: 10.1126/science.101.2616.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Whalen PJ. Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1998;7:177–88. [Google Scholar]
  29. Whalen PJ, Rauch SI, Etcoff NL, McInerney SC, Lee MB, Jenike MA. Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:411–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00411.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Wright CI, Martis B, Schwartz CE, Shin LM, Fisher HH, McMullin K. Novelty responses and differential effects of order in the amygdala, substantia innominata, and inferior temporal cortex. Neuroimage. 2003;18:660–9. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00037-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social cognitive and affective neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES