Abstract
Background
We hypothesized that time of day of surgery would influence the incidence of anesthetic adverse events (AEs).
Methods
Clinical observations reported in a quality improvement database were categorized into different AEs that reflected (1) error, (2) harm, and (3) other AEs (error or harm could not be determined) and were analyzed for effects related to start hour of care.
Results
As expected, there were differences in the rate of AEs depending on start hour of care. Compared with a reference start hour of 7 am, other AEs were more frequent for cases starting during the 3 pm and 4 pm hours (p<0.0001). Post hoc inspection of data revealed that the predicted probability increased from a low of 1.0% at 9 am to a high of 4.2% at 4 pm. The two most common event types (pain management and postoperative nausea and vomiting) may be primary determinants of these effects.
Conclusions
Our results indicate that clinical outcomes may be different for patients anesthetized at the end of the work day compared with the beginning of the day. Although this may result from patient related factors, medical care delivery factors such as case load, fatigue, and care transitions may also be influencing the rate of anesthetic AEs for cases that start in the late afternoon.
Keywords: fatigue, work schedules, time of day, adverse events, transitions
Health care is a 24 hour a day operation. Factors such as time on the job, effects of circadian rhythms, and issues related to demand, scheduling, and staffing may all have an effect on patient care over the course of a day. Research has revealed that human performance is adversely affected by sleep deficit, circadian rhythm disruption, and long work hours, leading to decrements in cognitive and psychomotor performance and increased risk of accidents.1,2,3,4,5,6 Fatigue is believed to be a greater problem in transportation accidents than drugs and alcohol combined, contributing to 15–20% of all transportation accidents.6 Surveys, laboratory and simulator studies, and evaluation of clinical data have revealed damaging effects of fatigue, both for the patient and the healthcare worker.7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14 Decisions about scheduling, demand, and staffing can result in variations in workload over the course of the day that may be reflected in care. In addition, staffing and scheduling decisions may create specific times of day that are associated with potentially risky events such as care transitions.15,16,17,18
Research evaluating relationships between time of day and clinical performance, as defined by the occurrence of adverse events (AEs) or patient outcomes, is limited. In anesthesia, this type of research is partially hindered by a relatively low frequency of adverse outcomes.19 One exception is a prospective study of cases of unintended dural puncture in obstetric epidural anesthesia that identified a greater risk of unintended dural puncture for epidural placement performed at night than during daytime.20 There is also little research evaluating clinical performance over multiple times of day. Several studies have compared night performance with day performance7,20 and sleep deprived practitioners with well rested practitioners,9,10,11,12,13,21,22,23,24,25 but few have considered potential time of day effects such as the early morning and afternoon circadian troughs or start and end of shift.
We suspect that clinical performance may vary throughout the day due to effects of time on the job, circadian lows, and times of transition. Periods that include circadian lows (3–5 am, 3–5 pm) and transfers of patient care from one anesthesia team to another (7 am, 4–6 pm) are times of day that may be related to qualitative changes in operating room performance and the incidence of AEs. Identifying periods of relatively impaired operating room performance is an important step in applying human factors principles to the improvement of patient care in this environment.
The Duke University Medical Center Department of Anesthesiology maintains a perioperative database that serves as a patient record and as a tool for assessing and improving quality of care. Specific clinical and administrative events are documented in the database as “quality improvement” (QI) events. These data provide an opportunity for a retrospective analysis of the incidence of AEs with respect to time of day. The primary objective of this evaluation was to determine whether time of day affects the number of AEs that occur perioperatively. We hypothesized that time of day of surgery would influence the incidence of anesthetic adverse events (AEs).
Methods
The Saturn Information System is a perioperative database and charting tool through which anesthesia providers and perioperative nurses electronically record and track a patient's clinical progress. The Saturn database includes patient demographic data, surgical and anesthetic plans, and patient notes, including QI event descriptors and provider entered text. The selection of QI event descriptors and the addition of comments are under the discretion of the anesthesia care team (self‐reports) in the operating room and preoperative and postoperative care units.
This study is based on data from 130 912 operating room cases recorded in Saturn between 1 May 2000 and 4 August 2004. The data include all anesthetic procedures completed at Duke University Medical Center during that time, including both inpatient and ambulatory procedures for adult and pediatric patients. All data were de‐identified before transfer to the research team and were therefore exempt from institutional review.
The independent variable we selected to assess the effect of time of day on the incidence of AEs was the start hour of care. The following characteristics of the patient, procedure, and operating room environment were expected to affect the incidence of AEs and were included in our analyses as covariates:
Age of patient (in months).
Sex of patient.
Global assessment of patient health (American Society of Anesthesiologists (ASA) physical risk classification).
Emergency or non‐emergency/scheduled procedure.
Duration of anesthesia care (in minutes).
Complexity of the procedure (ASA base units—a measure of case difficulty used for billing purposes).
Activity level of the OR suite (number of operating rooms running).
Although of interest, details such as the experience level of the anesthesia provider, the case load of the attending anesthesiologist, and the makeup of the care team could not be determined from the de‐identified data set.
Response variables for our analyses were anesthetic AEs. These were determined from the self‐reported QI events recorded in the perioperative database. Based on Leape's definition of preventable and non‐preventable AEs,26 records that indicate an error or failure to adhere to a standard of care can be considered preventable AEs. We originally planned to separately analyze events which may be preventable, events which cause some harm to the patient, and non‐preventable events. However, due to the nature of the database, we were not able to definitively distinguish preventable and non‐preventable events. We were, however, able to separate the data into three categories of events. These included error (preventable events), harm (events which resulted in harm to patients), and other AEs. We included the category of “other AEs” to classify adverse events that could not be definitively described as preventable or causing harm based on the information available, but were likely to be associated with error, harm, or an increased risk of either error or harm. Detailed definitions of the categories used by the research team are provided in table 1.
Table 1 Definitions of event categories.
Error | An error is defined using an adaptation of the definition posed by The Australian Council for Safety and Quality in Health Care Shared Meanings project27 (Merry, personal communication, 2004): |
“The failure to complete an action as intended or the unintentional use of a wrong plan to achieve an aim.” | |
This definition includes both errors due to a deficiency in knowledge or a failure in judgment or decision making (e.g. “mistakes” or “errors of judgment”) and errors which are an incorrect execution of a correct action sequence (e.g. “slips” or “technical errors”). Errors may occur by doing the wrong thing (commission) or by failing to do the right thing (omission).27 | |
Because it is difficult to determine the intent of a practitioner, we will assume the “standard of care” as the underlying assumption of intent. Therefore, any incident that represents a deviation from “standard of care”, whether it is an error of judgment or a technical error, will be classified as an error. For our purposes, “standard of care” is defined in accordance with NC statutes as care that is “… in accordance with the standards of practice among members of the same health care profession with similar training and experience situated in the same or similar communities…” (1975, 2nd Sess, c 977, s 4). | |
Harm | Harm includes any untoward medical occurrence in the patient that is not reasonably expected or common, based on the procedure being conducted. The instance of harm may or may not have a causal relationship with treatment. Harm includes any unfavorable incident or unintended sign (including an abnormal laboratory finding), symptom, or disease temporally associated with the procedure for which the patient is receiving anesthetic care, whether or not considered related to the procedure. This includes emotional distress, psychological trauma, invasion of privacy, embarrassment, loss of social status or employment, or any economic impact considered related to the conduct of the procedure. This category does not include delays of an administrative nature (see “Delay” below). Our definition is similar to an incident of “Harm” or “Loss” for the patient, as defined by the Shared Meanings project:27 |
“Harm: Death, disease, injury, suffering, and/or disability experienced by a person” (see loss) | |
“Loss: Any negative consequence, including financial, experienced by a person(s) or organisations(s)”. | |
Other AE | This category is included for other perioperative events that may have some association with error or harm but do not provide sufficient evidence of either error or harm based on the above definitions. This category includes, for example, events that are sometimes due to error or sometimes result in harm; events that sometimes occur concurrently with either error or harm; or events that may be associated with an increased risk for error or harm. This category does not include delays of an administrative nature (see “Delay” below). |
Delay | This category is included to track administrative delays. These include events that delay the start of the procedure or the transfer of the patient. Examples include: late arrival of the surgeon, anesthesiologist, other care team members or the patient; readiness problems associated with equipment, operating room, or other care units; and lateness of laboratory results or other necessary information. This category is NOT intended to reflect changes in procedure length or anesthesia care associated with other perioperative events of the categories error, harm, or other AEs as defined above. |
When QI events are entered into the database at the point of care by a certified registered nurse anesthetist (CRNA), anesthesia resident, or staff anesthesiologist, the practitioner selects from a list of perioperative events and also has an opportunity to enter additional text describing the QI event. Five experienced anesthesiologists reviewed the QI event labels available in the database and came to a consensus (in a face to face meeting) regarding whether each QI event label represented error, harm, or other AE (table 2). The panel of reviewers also identified the QI events that represented administrative delays because we believed that there may be an association between delays and AEs. We validated the assignment of QI events into these categories by comparing the automated classification by event labels with manual classification by four experienced anesthesiologists (JT, JM, KG, MSS) using both the event labels chosen by the practitioner and associated text. Data from 148 cases showed that, for the largest portion of the events (83%), two or three of the reviewers made no change to the classification that would have been made based on the event label alone. In addition, the reviewers very rarely agreed to change from an adverse event category to no event or delay (four cases, 2.6%) and they never agreed to change from either the error or harm category to the less definitive other AE category. Based on these results, we concluded that an analysis of the data based on categorization by QI event labels was unlikely to attain different results than an analysis that included expert review of each case that contained text descriptions.
Table 2 QI events assigned to AE categories.
Error events | 122 Delayed recognition esophageal intubation126 Unintentional extubation133 Delayed recognition bronchial intubation | 173 Wrong medication/wrong dose177 Inadequate preoperative assessment |
Harm events | 114 Unplanned outpatient admission115 Unplanned admission to ICU121 Inability to intubate124 Trauma to airway125 Damage to teeth131 Significant hypoxemia134 Severe bronchospasm135 Pneumothorax136 Pulmonary aspiration146 Confirmed myocardial infarction147 Pulmonary edema/CHF148 Cardiac arrest152 Excessive block153 Adverse event following block155 Post dural puncture headache161 Prolonged sedation162 Prolonged neuromuscular blockade163 Central nervous system complication164 Peripheral neurologic deficit | 165 Patient awareness (unintentional)171 Significant hyperthermia172 Significant (unintended) hyperthermia174 Drug/transfusion reaction176 Prolonged nausea/vomiting191 Eye injury192 Skin/soft tissue injury193 Death195 Other injury/catastrophe204 Emergency tracheotomy or cricothyrotomy211 Wound infection212 Other infection/sepsis213 Deep vein thrombosis214 Pulmonary thromboembolism215 Postoperative oliguria/anuria216 New postoperative need for dialysis417 Postoperative nausea and vomiting |
Other adverse events | 123 Laryngospasm127 Unanticipated difficult intubation128 Other airway132 Significant hypercapnia137 Other respiratory141 Significant hypertension142 Significant hypotension143 Significant tachycardia144 Other major arrhythmia145 Suspected myocardial ischemia149 Other cardiovascular | 151 Failed/inadequate block154 Unintentional dural puncture156 Other regional166 Other neurological175 Problem with vascular access181 Equipment problem (describe)194 Staff injury (describe)217 Other postoperative complications416 Pain management418 Failed discharge criteria808 Unclassified (please describe) |
Delay | 301 No/incomplete surgical consent302 No green sheet (no preoperative assessment paperwork)303 Late arrival of patient304 Waiting for results305 Time for regional anesthesia306 OR not ready307 Surgeon unavailable308 Anesthesia unavailable309 Anesthesia detained in PACU310 Other reason for delay (specify)401 No assigned intermediate bed402 No assigned SD bed403 No assigned ICU bed | 404 Room not ready: occupied405 Room not ready: not clean406 No transporter407 Floor nurse unavailable408 PACU nurse admitting 2nd patient409 Waiting for surgeon: orders410 Waiting for surgeon: MO1B (waiting for surgeon to sign postoperative care unit patient release)411 Waiting for surgeon: RX (prescription)412 Waiting for surgeon: other413 Waiting for anesthesiologist414 Waiting for lab results415 X‐ray to be taken or read419 Delayed discharge: other |
ICU, intensive care unit; CHF, congestive heart failure; OR, operating room; PACU, postoperative care unit; SD, stepdown.
Analysis of data
Because of the binary nature of the AE response variables, the data were subjected to multiple logistic regression analysis.28 The multiple regression model included start hour of care as an independent variable with the patient and procedural factors included as covariates. In order to minimize the degrees of freedom in the statistical model, the ASA physical status classification was reduced to two categories of low risk (ASA classification 1 or 2) and high risk (ASA classification 3, 4, or 5). Hour of day was treated as a categorical variable as were ASA status, sex, and emergency (yes or no). Age in months, surgery duration in minutes, and surgery complexity in ASA base units were treated as continuous variables.
Since there were fewer night time cases, initial review of the data suggested that power would be maximized by grouping events into eight 3 hour intervals over a full 24 hour day. We also conducted a separate hour by hour comparison of the non‐emergency (scheduled) procedures over the 12 hours of the regular work day (6 am to 6 pm). In the 12 hour analysis, the category of error was excluded due to an insufficient number of observations.
The response variables for four separate analyses were (1) harm, (2) error, (3) other AEs, and (4) delays. A review of the frequency of specific event types revealed a proportionally high frequency of postoperative nausea and vomiting (PONV) in the harm category (35% of harm events) and a proportionally high frequency of pain management issues in the other AEs category (49% of other AEs). Post hoc analyses therefore included analyses of the response variables PONV and pain management. We also analyzed harm with PONV events excluded and other AEs with pain management events excluded. Our goal in this analysis was to understand the degree to which these specific event types may be driving the overall results for harm and other AEs.
To further describe significant time of day effects, comparisons were made using odds ratio estimates. We selected the start hour time period with the highest frequency of starts (7 am hour or 6–9 am time period) as the reference point for all comparisons.
Results
Cases with missing start or end times were excluded from the analysis dataset, reducing the sample from 130 912 to 107 620 cases. The frequency distribution and means of the covariates are shown in table 3, and the frequencies of all case start times for each start hour are shown in fig 1. The dataset was further reduced to 90 159 cases for the multiple regression analysis because of missing covariate data. From this dataset, the following frequencies were observed for the response measures:
Table 3 Covariate proportions (for categorical variables) and means (for continuous variables).
Variable | Proportion | Mean (SD) |
---|---|---|
Emergency | 8% emergency | |
Sex | 52% female | |
ASA physical status | 62% low risk (ASA 1 and 2) | |
Age | 48 (23) years | |
Duration | 2.7 (2.0) hours | |
Complexity | 6.7 (3.9) ASA base units | |
OR suite activity level | 34 (7) OR rooms in operation |
Error: 31
Harm (including PONV events): 667
Other AEs (including pain management events): 1995
Delays: 9497
PONV: 277
Pain management: 1102
As expected, the multiple logistic regression analysis revealed a number of significant effects of covariates on AEs and delays in both the 24 hour analysis of all cases and the work day analysis of non‐emergency cases. The results for the 24 hour analysis are shown in table 4.
Table 4 Effects of covariates on adverse events and delays in the 24 hour analysis of all surgical cases.
Increased probability of an AE associated with: | For the following event categories: |
---|---|
Higher patient age | Other AE†, Delay† |
Female sex | Harm†, Other AE† |
High ASA status | Harm*, Other AE†, Delay† |
Longer case duration | Error*, Harm†, Other AE†, Delay† |
Emergency cases | Delay† |
Higher complexity cases | Other AE†, Delay† |
Greater number of OR rooms in operation | Harm**, Other AE†, Delay† |
*p<0.05, **p<0.01, or †p<0.001 (Wald χ2 test).
In the multiple logistic regression analysis over a 24 hour day, the incidence of other AEs was significantly influenced by the 3 hour time period in which surgery began (p<0.0001). The predicted probability of other AEs (with all covariates held constant) is shown in fig 2. Visual inspection of the graph suggests a higher probability of AEs in the late afternoon and early evening hours than in morning and early afternoon cases. Odds ratio estimates confirm the afternoon effect, indicating that cases that began between 3–6 pm had a higher probability of other AEs than cases that started during the reference time of 6–9 am (point estimate 1.48, 95% Wald confidence limit 1.19 to 1.84). Odds ratio estimates also indicate that, compared with the reference time of 6–9 am, there was a lower probability of events for cases starting later in the morning (9 am–noon) (point estimate 0.68, confidence limit 0.68 to 0.85) or in the early afternoon (noon–3 pm) (point estimate 0.88, confidence limit 0.77 to 0.99).
Analysis of hourly starts of scheduled cases over the workday (6 am–5 pm) revealed significant effects of start time for harm (p<0.01) and other AEs (p<0.0001). Considering the effects on harm, odds ratio estimates revealed significant differences from the reference start time of 7 am for the 8 am hour only (point estimate 0.62, confidence limit 0.45 to 0.85). However, visual inspection of the predicted probability of harm (fig 3) reveals trends toward an increase in events for cases that start in the late afternoon. For example, the predicted probability of harm is three times higher for cases that start at 3 pm (1.0%) than for those starting at 8 am (0.3%). Compared with the 7 am reference time, odds ratio estimates suggest trends toward a greater number of events for the 2 pm and 3 pm start hours (2 pm point estimate 1.38, confidence limits 0.97 to 1.98; 3 pm point estimate 1.53, confidence limits 0.98 to 2.40).
Considering the effects on other AEs, the predicted probability of an event indicated an increased risk for cases starting in the late afternoon (fig 3). Odds ratio estimates revealed a higher probability of other AEs for cases beginning in the 3 pm and 4 pm hours than for those beginning at the reference hour of 7 am (3 pm point estimate 1.49, confidence limit 1.15 to 1.93; 4 pm point estimate 1.68, confidence limit 1.16 to 2.42). Odds ratio estimates also revealed that cases starting in the 6 am hour and all hours between 8 am and 1 pm had a lower probability of an event than cases starting in the 7am hour (with the strongest effect seen at 9 am with a point estimate of 0.58 and confidence limits of 0.48 to 0.70). In examining specific data points for the size of the effect, the predicted probability of other AEs increased from a low of 1.0% for cases starting at 9 am to a high of 4.2% for cases starting at 4 pm.
Because of the high proportion of pain management events in the other AEs data set and PONV events in the harm dataset, we analyzed these specific events for time of day effects as post hoc analyses. The analyses revealed significant effects for both pain management (p<0.0001) and PONV (p<0.0001) in both the 24 hour and work day analyses. For the most part, these effects mirrored the effects seen in the general analyses of other AEs and harm. There was an increased probability of pain management events for cases starting in the late afternoon (2, 3, and 4 pm) compared with 7 am. There was also a decreased probability of pain management for cases starting in the mid morning (8 am–noon) compared with 7 am. For PONV, there was an increased probability of events for cases starting at 2 pm compared with 7 am and a decreased probability of events in the late morning (8–10 am) compared with 7 am.
For the work day analysis, removal of pain management and PONV events from the other AEs and harm datasets reduced the strength of the time of day effects such that they were no longer significant (p = 0.13 for harm; p = 0.18 for other AEs. Time of day had a significant effect on other AEs over a 24 hour day (p<0.05). In this case, the midday probability of an event (9 am–noon and noon–3 pm) was lower than the reference time of 6–9 am. In general, the overall rates of occurrence with these high frequency events excluded were very low (around 0.2% for harm and 0.6% for other AEs) and the confidence intervals were very large (of the order of 1.5 to 2.5% with, for example, a point estimate of 1.00 and confidence limits of 0.59 to 1.71 at 6–9 pm).
In both the 24 hour analysis and the work day analysis, delays were significantly affected by hour of day (p<0.0001). The predicted probabilities of delays from both analyses are shown in fig 4. Delays appear to increase substantially over the work day. Predicted probability increases from just over 5% in the morning hours to approximately 30% in the late afternoon.
Discussion
The results of our analyses support our hypothesis that AEs are influenced by the time of day of surgery. We identified a small but significant increase in AEs in the early morning compared with late morning and early afternoon hours. This effect was robust throughout a number of different analyses and event types. We also identified a significant and sizable increase in AEs in the late afternoon compared with early morning. Post hoc analyses revealed that this effect may have been driven primarily by the most frequent events of PONV and pain management.
In addition to the significant effects of AEs, there was a significant and sizeable increase in administrative delays in the late afternoon. This suggests that there may be a relationship between administrative delays and AEs that requires further investigation.
There are a number of reasons why AEs may occur more often at the end of the day, including (1) end of day fatigue, (2) afternoon circadian lows, (3) care transitions, (4) change in makeup of the care team, (5) changes in case load, (6) physiological changes in the patient, or (7) other unrecognized factors. Some of these same factors might also be relevant as an explanation for the increased AE rate in the early morning.
These data were collected at our medical center where all anesthesia care is supervised by a faculty physician anesthesiologist and delivered primarily by either an anesthesia resident or a CRNA. The attending anesthesiologist may supervise up to four CRNAs or up to two residents in separate operating rooms. The work day generally begins between 6 and 7 am and ends between 3 and 6 pm for most attendings and residents. However, each day several attendings stay later to finish cases or remain on a late call schedule that ends between 6 and 9 pm. Both attendings and residents work a distinct night call schedule. CRNAs work 12 hour shifts of 7 am to 7 pm, 11 am to 11 pm, or 7 pm to 7 am. Scheduling details are visually depicted in fig 5.
Because of this scheduling system, times of transition in anesthesia care are most likely to occur around 6–8 am and 3–7 pm. Changes in the makeup of the care team may also occur during these times. In particular, a greater fraction of the case load may be covered by CRNAs rather than by residents during the 3–7 pm time frame. There also may be increases in case load per attending physician associated with the 3–7 pm time frame as supervision transitions to fewer late call anesthesiologists. Our finding of a substantial increase in delays in the late afternoon also suggests a potential problem of workload at this time. In contrast to the 6–8 am time of transition when well rested anesthesiologists arrive to begin the day, physicians supervising cases during the 3–7 pm transition times are usually physicians who are continuing cases, taking over new cases, and who began their work day between 6 and 7 am.
Arbous et al29 identified specific issues associated with both transitions and the makeup of the patient care team that affect postoperative mortality and coma. They found an increased risk of perioperative death associated with intraoperative change of one anesthesiologist by another. In addition, the risk of severe morbidity and mortality was reduced by (1) direct availability of an anesthesiologist (via intercom rather than phone or pager), (2) the presence of a full time (versus part time) anesthetic nurse, and (3) the presence of two individuals at emergence and termination of anesthesia. While there are some relevant differences in the model for anesthesia care in the United States and Europe—for example, differences in training and responsibilities of anesthesia nurses and requirements for anesthesiologists to be present at critical points such as induction and emergence—it is likely that, as case load increases, anesthesiologists may be faced with difficult choices about where their presence is most needed or when they should call for help.
This study has some limitations. Firstly, it is based on non‐anonymous self‐reports. Although the database is used as the official perioperative record which requires providers to be diligent in reporting significant events, they may be biased in how they document events or in their decisions whether or not to report minor incidents. They may be more likely to select QI event labels that describe the event more generally or are seen as patient related events rather than labels that suggest clinician error and indicate a cause or possible blame. There also may be an increase in documentation associated with cases in which there are transitions, either for purposes of providing key information to oncoming providers or for providing a clear historical record of whether events occurred before or after the transition.
Secondly, a reduction of over 30% in our sample size due to missing data calls into question the robustness of the database for the purposes of these analyses. Although we were unable to identify specific causes of missing data, we have no reason to believe that there are any systematic biases associated with missing data that would affect the results of our study.
Lastly, we were unable to clearly determine which events were preventable. This limits our ability to determine whether or not the time of day effects have underlying causes that can be controlled. For example, circadian effects on the patient such as changes in the sensitivity to pain30 or propensity for PONV could partly explain our results.
Most studies on the effects of fatigue on clinical outcomes have focused on sleep deprivation or disrupted circadian rhythms—for example, looking at post call effects15,21 or comparing night time with daytime performance.7,20 We are not aware of any other studies that have revealed decrements in clinical outcome associated with the beginning or end of the work day. This study presents evidence of a significant and sizeable increase in the incidence of anesthetic AEs in the late afternoon hours. It is unclear whether the increase in AEs was due to (1) problems related to increased case load and delays at these times, (2) effects of caregiver fatigue after many hours on the job, (3) problems that occur because of transitions, (4) increased reporting during times of transitions, or (5) other unidentified factors such as changes in the makeup of the anesthesia care team or physiological changes in the patients. Future research should focus on identifying the causes of increases in anesthetic AEs in the late afternoon. After these causes are identified, strategies to reduce or eliminate these events should be evaluated.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank the APSF Scientific Evaluation Committee for their input regarding our methods and ongoing enthusiasm for this project; Richard Adrian (Duke Health Technology Solutions Perioperative Information Systems Group) for his support in retrieving the perioperative data; and Terry Breen (Duke University Medical Center, Department of Anesthesiology) for his assistance in interpreting the quality improvement information and participation in categorizing QI events.
Abbreviations
AE - adverse event
PONV - postoperative nausea and vomiting
QI - quality improvement
Footnotes
This research was funded by a grant from the Anesthesia Patient Safety Foundation (APSF), Indianapolis, IN, USA.
Competing interests: none declared.
Preliminary results of this project were presented at the Healthcare Systems Ergonomics and Patient Safety Conference in Florence, Italy on 1 April 2005 (Wright MC, Andregg BC, Mark JB, et al. Effects of time of day and surgery duration on adverse events in anaesthesia. In: Tartaglia R, Bagnara S, Bellandi T, eds. International Conference on Healthcare Systems Ergonomics and Patient Safety. Taylor & Francis, 2005, 377–80).
References
- 1.Allied Pilots Association An overview of the scientific literature concerning fatigue, sleep, and the circadian cycle. Battelle Memorial Institute, JIL Information Systems 1998
- 2.Shingledecker C A, Holding D H. Risk and effort measure of fatigue. J Motor Behav 1974617–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Graeber R C. Air crew fatigue and circadian rhythmicity. Human factors in aviation. Academic Press 1988305–344.
- 4.Dawson D, Reid K. Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment. Nature 1997388235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mitler M M, Carskadon M A, Czeisler C A.et al Catastrophes, sleep, and public policy: consensus report. Sleep 198811100–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Akerstedt T. Consensus statement: Fatigue and accidents in transport operations. J Sleep Res 20009395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gold D R, Rogacz Z, Bock N. Rotating shift work, sleep, and accidents related to sleepiness in hospital nurses. Am J Public Health 1992821011–1014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Landrigan C P, Rothschild J M, Cronin J W.et al Effect on reducing intern work hours on serious medical errors in intensive care units. N Engl J Med 20043511838–1848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Denisco R A, Drummond J N, Gravenstein J S. The effect of fatigue on the performance of a simulated anesthetic monitoring task. J Clin Monit 1987322–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Smith‐Coggins R, Rosekind M R, Hurd S.et al Relationship of day versus night sleep to physician performance and mood. Ann Emerg Med 199424928–934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Friedman R C, Bigger J T, Kornfeld D S. The intern and sleep loss. N Engl J Med 1971285201–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Taffinder N J, McManus I C, Gul Y.et al Effect of sleep deprivation on surgeon's dexterity on laparoscopy simulator. Lancet 19983521191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Howard S K, Gaba D M, Smith B E.et al Simulation study of rested versus sleep‐deprived anesthesiologists. Anesthesiology 2003981345–1355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rogers A E, Hwang W ‐ T, Scott L D.et al The working hours of hospital staff nurses and patient safety. Health Aff 200423202–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Petersen L A, Brennan T A, O'Neil A C.et al Does housestaff discontinuity of care increase the risk for preventable adverse events? Ann Intern Med 1994121866–872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Laine C, Goldman L, Soukup J R.et al The impact of a regulation restricting medical house staff working hours on the quality of patient care. JAMA 1993261374–378. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cooper J B, Long C D, Newbower R S.et al Critical incidents associated with intraoperative exchanges of anesthesia personnel. Anesthesiology 198256456–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wears R L, Perry S J, Shapiro M.et al Shift changes among emergency physicians: Best of times, worst of times. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 47th Annual Meeting. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 2003
- 19.Gaba D M. Human error in dynamic medical domains. In: Bogner MS, ed. Human error in medicine. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence, Erlbaum Associates, 1994
- 20.Aya A G, Mangin R, Robert C.et al Increased risk of unintentional dural puncture in night‐time obstetric epidural anesthesia. Can J Anesthes 199946665–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Haynes D F, Schwedler M, Dyslin D C.et al Are postoperative complications related to resident sleep deprivation? South Med J 199588283–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Howard S K, Keshavacharya S, Smith B E.et al Behavioral evidence of fatigue during a simulator experiment. Anesthesiology 1998891236A [Google Scholar]
- 23.Howard S K, Healzer J M, Gaba D M. Sleep and work schedules of anesthesia residents: a national survey. Anesthesiology 199787932A [Google Scholar]
- 24.Howard S K, Smith B E, Gaba D M.et al Performance of well‐rested vs. highly‐fatigued residents: a simulator study. Anesthesiology 199787981A [Google Scholar]
- 25.Herndon C N, Weinger M B, Smith B E.et al Use of task analysis to evaluate the effects of fatigue on performance during simulated anesthesia cases. Anesthesiology 1998891180A [Google Scholar]
- 26.Leape L L. The preventability of medical injury. In: Bogner MS, ed. Human error in medicine. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 199413–25.
- 27.Australian Council for Safety and Quality in Health Care Shared meanings: consultation toward a common vocabulary of key health care safety terms. Available at http://www.safetyandquality.org/smhome.htm, 2000 (accessed 6 July 2004)
- 28.Neter J, Kutner M H, Nachtsheim C J.et alApplied linear statistical models. Boston: McGraw‐Hill, 1996
- 29.Arbous M S, Meursing A E E, vanKleef J W.et al Impact of anesthesia management characteristics on severe morbidity and mortality. Anesthesiology 2005102257–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Aya A G M, Vialles N, Mangin R.et al Chronobiology of labour pain perception: an observational study. Br J Anaesth 200493451–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]