Skip to main content
Injury Prevention logoLink to Injury Prevention
editorial
. 2006 Apr;12(2):71–73. doi: 10.1136/ip.2005.008839

Would society pay more attention to injuries if the injury control community paid more attention to risk communication science?

D C Girasek
PMCID: PMC2564451  PMID: 16595419

Short abstract

“If a disease were killing our children at the rate unintentional injuries are, the public would be outraged and demand that this killer be stopped.” Former US Surgeon General, C Everett Koop

Keywords: injury, risk communication, public, social


Whenever injury control professionals gather, it doesn't take long for the conversation to turn to one topic: “Why is it that more attention and resources aren't devoted to injuries, the leading cause of death for children and young adults?” One early hypothesis—that people think injuries cannot be prevented—has not held up to scientific scrutiny.1,2,3,4,5,6 Another possible explanation, however, may be found in the risk communication literature.

Since the 1980s, corporate and government officials have turned to risk communication experts for help with a problem contrary to our own: why do communities “overreact” to hazards that pose minimal risk from an epidemiological perspective?7 In such situations, environmental engineers and safety experts characterize lay people's views as irrational or ignorant.8 For example, in a national (US) survey, more than twice as many parents worried about their children being kidnapped than being involved in an “automobile accident”.2 Many professionals assumed that if they shared more data with the public, enlightenment would follow. The risk communication literature suggests, however, that scientists may also benefit from enlightenment.8

Most Americans know that injuries are a leading cause of death,9,10 yet they display little concern for injury prevention. In trying to explain this disconnect between vital statistics and public support, psychometric researchers have identified qualitative dimensions of risk not captured by epidemiologic methods. Sandman conceptualizes Risk, as it is perceived by the public, as a function of Hazard + Outrage.8 His “hazard” refers to the statistical calculation of risk that professionals generally rely on. “Outrage”, on the other hand, is more emotional and predicts an issue's ability to evoke community response. In the past few decades, numerous authors have described the components shared by threats which tend to spark public outrage. In this commentary I have drawn selectively from the work of Sandman8,11 and Bennett.12 Readers interested in a more comprehensive summary should consult Chapman.13

Qualities that have been associated with outrage

The first quality that I will discuss in relation to injury is voluntariness. Studies have shown that if two situations carry the same statistical risk, people will be more upset about hazards that are thrust upon them rather than those over which they have a choice. Most injury producing events in developed countries are unlikely to feel coercive to the general public, hence they would not generate much “outrage”.

The next factor reflects equitability: whether some members of society benefit from a hazard while others suffer from it? One example is firearms because gun control advocates argue that people who endorse easy access to weapons for self‐defense put the rest of society at greater risk. Dangers judged to pose this sort of “unfair” risk are associated with more distress.

The third factor applies to threats that an individual cannot avoid by taking personal action. It differs from “voluntariness” in that it relates to who is operating the hazardous agent, not who exposes you to it. At least one (US) study has found that most adults believe they control their own destiny when it comes to avoiding injury.14 The opposite perception would be associated with increased concern.

The next component contrasts manmade with natural risks. According to this line of reasoning, fires caused by arsonists or poor wiring would generate more distress than those caused by lightning.

Another important quality of a risk is whether it evokes memories for the public, either because of past experiences or media portrayals. It is akin to the availability heuristic. Tragedies that are easy to envision (for example, a plane crashing into a skyscraper) receive more attention.

Threats that are unusual or exotic (for example, killer bees) are more likely to arouse concern than those we encounter every day (for example, driving to work).

Socially “dreaded” consequences are perceived as riskier than other hazards. This is reflected in the fact that people will pay three times more to prevent a cancer death, for example, than a crash death.15 This may be because traumatic deaths are thought of as quick (“at least he didn't suffer”) or romantic (“he died doing what he loved”).

Catastrophic threats cause more concern than threats that occur apart from each other in time and space. Again, fatal injuries are at a disadvantage because they are relatively rare and usually kill few people in one community on any given day. Exceptions to this rule, like mining accidents and hotel fires, generally receive international attention.

Unknowable risks are those that are difficult for scientists to quantify. The public finds it unsettling when experts cannot agree about whether something is harmful (for example, electromagnetic fields). The damage caused by injury hazards is generally irrefutable, which makes their toll more predictable and less frightening.

The public can become concerned to the point of outrage when confronted with issues that are viewed as “morally relevant”. Sandman contends that slavery and child molestation, for example, are so repugnant that officials would never discuss them in terms of “tradeoffs”.8,11 Injuries can result when public officials and/or corporate executives value profits over human lives. Such calculations are rarely made public, however.

Hazards that pose delayed or hidden threats (for example, carcinogens in drinking water) are usually taken more seriously than threats that act acutely. Injuries generally fall into the latter category. While some events may result in drawn‐out declines that ultimately prove fatal, such deaths usually occur in long term care facilities and do not make headlines.

The next issue asks whether the threat affects vulnerable populations. Injury advocates have learned that legislative remedies are more readily accepted when they are billed as saving children's lives rather than adults'.

Those seeking public support also know that it is easier to rally concern for identifiable victims than for people who are represented only by statistics. This phenomenon is well documented as influencing the amount of money we are willing to spend to save lives.16 Many types of injury victims, however, are rarely shown or named in our society (for example, elderly fall victims).

When reacting to risks, the public takes into account whether a hazard carries substantial benefits. No one is calling for a ban on cars, for example, although they are associated with more injury deaths than any other product. In this domain, fires associated with cigarettes, or deaths associated with firearms might lend themselves to activist efforts because the public could perceive their mere possession as a “foolish risk”.

Finally, we need to consider whether opportunities exist for collective action. Injury again falls short on this dimension because no well recognized voluntary organization exists with a general focus on “our” public health problem. This issue has been raised by parents of fatally injured children seeking avenues for injury advocacy.17

“You would be amazed. I mean these people [who raise funds in their child's name] beat the bushes looking for injury prevention programs to give money to, and we can't find any. Pretty scary, huh?” [unpublished interview excerpt]

This (albeit subjective) analysis illustrates that few of the major causes of injury are likely to be linked—in the minds of the public—to characteristics that have been shown to evoke outrage or activism. In fact, when a table was created to cross tabulate all of Sandman's primary and secondary “outrage components” (n = 20) with the leading causes of injury death (n = 8), 82% of the cells in the table were left unchecked.18 In other words, the challenge we face in raising the profile of injury prevention was revealed.

Implications for practice and advocacy

Although this commentary focuses mainly on perceptions of the lay public, its implications are relevant to how we communicate with policy makers and the media as well. We must be strategic in crafting our messages. For example, when a less hazardous product—like firesafe cigarettes—becomes technically feasible but is not even sold in most jurisdictions, we could emphasize the involuntary nature of this risk scenario. Housefires might be framed as resulting from “negligent manufacturing” rather than “careless smoking”. We could point out that radar detectors benefit their owners and manufacturers while putting others at “unfair” risk. We could emphasize scientific findings that reveal teenagers to be developmentally vulnerable, rather than irresponsibly reckless.19,20 People might dread injuries more if they were exposed to the experiences of bereaved parents or those of people who face recovery from disabling or disfiguring injuries. Such campaigns can be disrespectful and insensitive,21,22 however, so they should be approached with caution.

It is critical that all attempts to communicate about injury prevention be evaluated for intended and unintended consequences. We may find, for example, that approaches that “move” communities decrease the likelihood of individuals taking actions to protect themselves. Researchers should devote particular attention to what happens when we stress how preventable injuries are. There are red flags in the literature about negative consequences that could result from such campaigns. Risk communication findings, for example, suggest that the public is less concerned about preventable health problems, and less likely to support funding for such causes.23,24,25 Subjects have demonstrated diminished sympathy for individuals who were disabled by “controllable” causes.26 Furthermore, optimistic bias (an erroneous perception of low personal risk) has been shown to operate more strongly in relation to preventable health problems.27 Finally, concerns have been raised about whether parents become less vigilant once they have adopted measures that are billed as preventing (as opposed to reducing) childhood injuries.28,29

Perhaps we should consider expounding on the refrain “injuries are preventable”. If we don't, our message may be interpreted as operating solely at the level of the individual. Injury control professionals know, conversely, that most of our progress can be traced to population level interventions. A majority of Americans believe that allocating additional government funds to injury control would yield no benefit, and that “scientists and other experts” are unlikely to find ways to reduce the injury threat.15 This may be because a distressingly large proportion of individuals still attribute injuries to “carelessness” and “stupidity”.9,30

Our field might also benefit from calling attention to the many injury problems that remain poorly understood.31,32 Although we have made monumental advances in recent decades, our evidence base is still quite young. We also need to call attention to the injuries that continue to take lives, despite the fact that solid solutions for them have been published in our scientific journals. We need research on translating study findings into public action. Epidemiology and engineering remain central to the field of injury control. We must look to the social and behavioral sciences, however, if we hope to overcome the political and cognitive barriers that impede our advancement.

Acknowledgements

The views expressed are those of the author, who is not writing on behalf of any government agency.

Footnotes

Competing interests: none.

References

  • 1.Duan L. A survey of unintentional childhood injury pattern and awareness in three cities in China. Presented at the 7th World Conference on Injury Prevention and Safety Promotion in Vienna, Austria on June 8 2004
  • 2.Eichelberger M R, Gotschall C S, Feely H B.et al Parental attitudes and knowledge of child safety. AJDC 1990144714–720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hu X, Wesson D, Parkin P.et al Pediatric injuries: parental knowledge, attitudes and needs. Can J Public Health 199687101–105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Girasek D C. How members of the public interpret the word accident. Inj Prev 1999519–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hooper R, Coggan C A, Adams B. Injury prevention attitudes and awareness in New Zealand. Inj Prev 2003942–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Vincenten J A, Sector M J, Rogmans W.et al Parents' perceptions, attitudes and behaviours towards child safety: a study in 14 European countries. Int J Inj Contr Saf Promot 200512183–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Covello V T, McCallum D B, Pavlova M. Principles and guidelines for improving risk communication. In: Covello VT, McCallum DB, Pavlova M, eds. Effective risk communication: the role and responsibility of government and nongovernment organizations. New York, NY: Plenum, 1989, 3–4, 27
  • 8.Sandman P M.Responding to community outrage: strategies for effective communication. Fairfax, VA: American Industrial Hygiene Association, 19934–75.
  • 9. Americans aware that injury is leading killer of young people. (A public opinion study for Research!America). Spring 2005. Available at http://www.safeusa.org/publications/injury_poll_report.pdf (accessed January 2006)
  • 10.Coalition for American Trauma Care The American public's views of and support for trauma systems: a congressional briefing. March 2, 2005. Available at http://www.aast.org/PPTs/CATC.05.HarrisPoll.HTFINAL.2.28.05.ppt (accessed January 2006)
  • 11.Sandman P M. Risk = Hazard & Outrage: a formula for effective risk communication (1991). A video distributed by the American Industrial Hygiene Association: Fairfax, VA,
  • 12.Bennett P. Understanding responses to risk: some basic findings. In: Bennett P, Calman K, eds. Risk communication and public health. New York, NY: Oxford University Press 1999, 6–7, 16
  • 13.Chapman S, Wutzke S. Not in our backyard: media coverage of community opposition to mobile phone towers—an application of Sandman's outrage model of risk perception. Aust N Z J Public Health 199721614–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.O'Connor R E, Bord R J, Fisher A. Rating threat mitigators: faith in experts, governments, and individuals themselves to create a safer world. Risk Anal 199818547–556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jones‐Lee M W, Hammerton M, Philips P R. The value of safety: results of a national sample survey. The Economic Journal 19859549–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jenni K E, Loewenstein G. Explaining the “Identifiable Victim Effect.” J Risk Uncertain 199714235–257. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Girasek D C. Parents of fatally injured children discuss taking part in prevention campaigns: an exploratory study. Death Stud 200327929–937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Girasek D C. Health risk communication. In: Gielen AC, Sleet DA, DiClemente RJ, eds. Injury and violence prevention: behavioral science theories, methods, and applications. San Francisco, CA: Jossey‐Bass, 2006
  • 19.Giedd J N. Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Ann NY Acad Sci 2004102177–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Arnett J J, Irwin C E, Jr, Halpern‐Fisher B L. Developmental sources of crash risk in young drivers. Inj Prev 2002817–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wang C. Culture, meaning and disability: injury prevention campaigns and the production of stigma. Soc Sci Med 1992351093–1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Girasek D C. Advice from bereaved parents: on forming partnerships for injury prevention. Health Promot Pract 20056207–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Beggs S D. Diverse Risks and the Relative Worth of Government Health and Safety Programs: an Experimental Survey. Economic Analysis Division. U.S. Environmental Protection Agency Report No 700. 23, June 1984
  • 24.Mendeloff J M, Kaplan R M. Are large differences in “lifesaving” costs justified? A psychometric study of the relative value place on preventing deaths. Risk Anal 19899349–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.National Research Council Improving risk communication. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 198935
  • 26.Menec V H, Perry R P. Reactions to stigmas. The effect of targets' age and controllability of stigmas. J Aging Health 19957365–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Weinstein N D. What does it mean to understand a risk? Evaluating risk comprehension. J Natl Cancer Inst 19992515–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Morrongiello B A, Major K. Influence of safety gear in parental perceptions of injury risk and tolerance for children's risk‐taking. Inj Prev 2002827–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Smith G S. Drowning prevention in children: the need for new strategies. Inj Prev 19951216–217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Girasek D C. Injury's perceived association with stupidity. Presented at the 5th World Conference on Injury Prevention and Safety Promotion in Delhi, India 2000
  • 31.Philippakis A, Hemenway D, Alexe D M.et al A quantification of preventable unintentional childhood injury mortality in the United States. Inj Prev 20041079–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rivara F P, Grossman D C. Prevention of traumatic deaths to children in the United States: how far have we come and where do we need to go? Pediatrics 97791–797. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Injury Prevention are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES