Abstract
Background
Restrained worker honey bees are a valuable model for studying the behavioral and neural bases of olfactory plasticity. The proboscis extension response (PER; the proboscis is the mouthpart of honey bees) is released in response to sucrose stimulation. If sucrose stimulation is preceded one or a few times by an odor (forward pairing), the bee will form a memory for this association, and subsequent presentations of the odor alone are sufficient to elicit the PER. However, backward pairing between the two stimuli (sucrose, then odor) has not been studied to any great extent in bees, although the vertebrate literature indicates that it elicits a form of inhibitory plasticity.
Methodology/Principal Findings
If hungry bees are fed with sucrose, they will release a long lasting PER; however, this PER can be interrupted if an odor is presented 15 seconds (but not 7 or 30 seconds) after the sucrose (backward pairing). We refer to this previously unreported process as olfactory interference. Bees receiving this 15 second backward pairing show reduced performance after a subsequent single forward pairing (excitatory conditioning) trial. Analysis of the results supported a relationship between olfactory interference and a form of backward pairing-induced inhibitory learning/memory. Injecting the drug cimetidine into the deutocerebrum impaired olfactory interference.
Conclusions/Significance
Olfactory interference depends on the associative link between odor and PER, rather than between odor and sucrose. Furthermore, pairing an odor with sucrose can lead either to association of this odor to PER or to the inhibition of PER by this odor. Olfactory interference may provide insight into processes that gate how excitatory and inhibitory memories for odor-PER associations are formed.
Introduction
Insects have proven to be invaluable for studying not only the basic forms of learning but also for understanding how higher-order cognitive processes might be supported by their smaller, more accessible nervous systems [1]–[3]. Use of insects such as fruit flies and honey bees has clearly provided insight into the cellular and molecular events that underlie sophisticated behavioral plasticity in higher vertebrates [4], [5]. Likewise, well-conceived mechanistic studies of insect behavior can reveal components of the more complex cognitive phenomena found in mammals [6]–[11].
In particular, there is a rich history of using the honey bee for studying both non-associative and associative learning processes [1]–[3], [12]–[17] as well as cognitive processes such as choice behavior [18], [19], non-elemental learning [20], [21], the use of conceptual rules [9]–[11], spatial orientation [22], [23], visual categorization [24], delayed alternation [25], lateralization [26] and inter-individual communication about location of food via the ‘dance language’ [23], [27]. Non-associative and associative learning have been extensively studied in the laboratory with restrained bees using a well-defined behavioral response called the proboscis extension response (PER, the proboscis is the main mouthpart of the bee). PER is an appetitive response triggered when sucrose solution -the unconditioned stimulus (US)- is applied to the antennae and/or the proboscis. The animal then extends its proboscis to consume the sucrose solution. If an odor –the conditioned stimulus (CS)- precedes and slightly overlaps sucrose presentation (forward pairing), the bee will form an excitatory association between this odor and the sucrose (and/or the PER) by way of classical (Pavlovian) conditioning [28]. On the other hand, presentation of an odor 15 s after sucrose delivery (backward pairing) will produce inhibitory learning about this odor, i.e. bees will display poorer performance during subsequent training [29].
The PER olfactory conditioning protocol allows for the precise control of stimulation parameters in behavioral studies as well as for simultaneous linkage to neurophysiological and imaging analyses of brain activity [30]–[33]. Furthermore PER conditioning allows for pharmacological and molecular manipulation of identified modulatory pathways involved in processing stimuli, learning and reinforcement [34]–[48]. Recent studies have begun to extend PER conditioning to cognitive processes such as choice behavior [18], [19] and non-elemental learning [20], [21] using restrained bees (rather than free-flying bees) in the controlled conditions of a laboratory. These studies provide the opportunity for associating more complex kinds of learning with neurophysiological measurements and molecular manipulations.
Backward pairing of sucrose and odor has received relatively less attention [29]. Here we report on the observation of a new PER related phenomenon, which we call olfactory interference. During the olfactory interference protocol, a hungry bee consumes a small droplet of sucrose solution, which elicits PER that continues for several seconds after the sucrose has been consumed. After the end of the feeding, the proportion of bees displaying the ongoing PER decays smoothly. This ongoing PER can be abruptly interrupted by presentation of an odor stimulus (backward pairing), but only within a specific time frame. Olfactory interference elicits inhibitory conditioning as with other backward pairing stimuli [29]. We argue that studying olfactory interference will provide insight into an important process that gates how excitatory and inhibitory memories for odor-PER associations are formed in the brain. It is well-established that inhibitory neurotransmitters (such as histamine) are involved in olfactory processing in well defined areas of the honey bee brain [33], [37], [48], [49]. We show that olfactory interference can be disrupted by blockade of cimetidine-sensitive pathways specifically in the deuterocerebrum.
Results
Olfactory interference
In this first experiment, sucrose was presented to the bee to release PER. A 4 s odor pulse (either 1-nonanol or octanal) was presented 15 s after the onset of feeding. We used a delay of 15 s because previous work indicated that backward inhibitory conditioning occurs at this interval [29]. Two control groups of bees received either no stimulation (nothing group) or just air (i.e. the air was blown across an unscented filter paper inside the syringe). The behavior of the bees was videotaped to measure the duration of the initial PER (see materials and methods and supplementary Figure S1 for details). The results are presented Figure 1.
In the “nothing” and “air-only” groups, the probability of continuing with the initial PER declined with time. This is is the normal response to sucrose stimulation. The bees initially responded to sucrose stimulation with a PER lasting 3–7 s, which corresponds to the time needed to consume the sucrose. After that, the proportion of bees extending their proboscis declined at a fairly constant rate through 30–35 s, by which time most bees had stopped the PER. There were no statistically significant differences between the air-only group and the nothing group (Wald test: before the odor, 0–14 s: χ2 = 0.335, p = 0.553; during the odor, 15–19 s: χ2 = 1.019, p = 0.313; after the odor, 20–34 s: χ2 = 0.0002, p = 0.989). Therefore, the air alone did not have any effect on the probability of continuing the PER, although the performance in this group is slightly lower than in the untreated group.
Olfactory interference is clearly apparent in groups that received odor stimulation. In both groups that received an odor there was a sharp decline in PER at the onset of the odor relative to the rate of decline in the two control groups. This decline was evident relative to the “nothing” control during odor presentation (Wald test during odor, 15–19 s: χ2 = 8.297, p = 0.004) but not before or after (before the odor, 0–14 s: χ2 = 0.360, p = 0.548; after the odor, 20–34 s: χ2 = 2.417, p = 0.120). Similarly, the odor group was significantly different from the air-only group when the odor was presented, but not before or after odor presentation (Wald test; before the odor, 0–14 s: χ2 = 0.004, p = 0.952; during the odor, 15–19 s: χ2 = 4.460, p = 0.035; after the odor, 20–34 s: χ2 = 2.333, p = 0.127), indicating that the olfactory component of stimulation (rather than the mechanosensory component from air) is more salient for retraction of the proboscis. This is consistent with previous reports that the mechanical effect of air that carries the odorant is much less salient for the bee than the odor itself [15], [50]; we also observed that bees placed in a constant air flow still display olfactory interference (data not shown). In summary, an odor presented 15 s after sucrose feeding can terminate the ongoing PER: this is the olfactory interference effect (see supplementary Movie S1). The observation of olfactory interference has been replicated in supplementary Figure S2, which demonstrates its reliability. We also presented sucrose to the antennae alone (rather than on both antennae and proboscis) without feeding the bees. In that case, the bees quickly retracted the proboscis, which prevents the detection of olfactory interference (supplementary Figure S3).
Time dependency of olfactory interference
We evaluated the time frame for olfactory interference by stimulating different groups of bees with odor (1-nonanol or octanal,) 7, 15 or 30 s after onset of feeding (Figure 2). Consistent with the previous observation (compare to Figure 1), when the odor was presented 15 s after the onset of sucrose feeding olfactory interference was observed as a sharp decline in PER associated with odor stimulation. This group was significantly different from the control (no odor) group (Wald test; no odor group vs. odor at 15 s group, during the 15–19 s period: χ2 = 9.838, p = 0.002). On the other hand, olfactory interference was not found when the odor was presented 7 s or 30 s after the sucrose (Wald test; no odor group vs. odor at 7 s group, during the 7–11 s period: χ2 = 2.627, p = 0.105; no odor group vs. odor at 30 s group, during the 30–34 s period: χ2 = 0.236, p = 0.627). Therefore, olfactory interference occurs specifically at or around 15 s after the onset of feeding. The decrease observed when the odor was presented at 7 s was also observed in the control group, but the difference between the two groups was not significant. This decrease can be explained by the spontaneous retraction of the proboscis occurring in some bees after the end of feeding (i.e. not all the bees release a long-lasting PER).
Among bees that had stopped extending their proboscis before the odor onset, we rarely observed proboscis extension when odor was presented. That would have been an indication of “sensitization” documented in other studies of PER in flies and bees for odors presented 30 s after sucrose stimulation [15], [41], [47], [51]–[54]. Sensitization should lead to a transient rise in proportion of bees responding during or shortly after odor presentation at 30 s (or at the two other delays). Although we observed a small increase in the proportion of bees responding, it was not significantly different from the control group (10.3% of the bees resumed the PER at 30 s in the 30 s group vs. 2.5% in the control group; Fisher's exact test, p = 0.360).
Olfactory interference, sucrose sensitivity and sensitization
This experiment had two objectives. First, we wanted to confirm the absence of sensitization-induced odor responses by presenting the odor 30 s after the sucrose. Second, we wanted to assess whether well established genetic differences among bees in sucrose sensitivity would affect olfactory interference and the sensitization-induced odor response. Therefore, we used both nectar foragers and pollen foragers, as pollen foragers are more sensitive to sucrose than nectar foragers [55]; the previous experiments used only nectar foragers.
Bees in each foraging group (nectar or pollen) were either presented with 1-nonanol or octanal 15 s or 30 s after sucrose presentation, or they were left untreated (Figure 3). The results were the same for both pollen and nectar groups: animals that were presented with either odor abruptly retracted their proboscis when exposed to odor 15 s after the onset of feeding (Wald test; nectar foragers, no odor group vs. odor at 15 s group, during the 15–19 s period: χ2 = 11.463, p = 0.0007; pollen foragers, no odor group vs. odor at 15 s group, during the 15–19 s period: χ2 = 10.894, p = 0.00097). In addition, animals presented with an odor at 30 s did not resume the PER more often than the control group (nectar groups: 3.6% in the control group vs. 13% in the 30 s group, Fisher's exact test, p = 0.329; pollen groups: 4.3% in the control group vs. 5.6% in the 30 s group, Fisher's exact test, p = 1), so neither nectar nor pollen foragers displayed a sensitization-induced odor response in our experimental conditions. This led us to conclude that olfactory interference is equivalent in nectar and pollen foragers, which suggests that olfactory interference is not affected by sucrose sensitivity.
There was only a small difference between the pollen and nectar untreated groups: while they were not different in the 15–19 s time period (Wald test: χ2 = 0.198, p = 0.656), pollen foragers tended to retract their proboscis more often after this time (Wald test: χ2 = 5.374, p = 0.020). This indicates that pollen foragers tend to retract their proboscis slightly earlier than nectar foragers.
Olfactory interference is associated with backward inhibitory learning
Previous results have indicated that a 15 s backward pairing between sucrose and odor elicits inhibitory conditioning [29], which is revealed through subsequent attempts to condition the PER using forward pairing (excitatory conditioning) to the same odor. The reported delay of 15 s corresponds to the time at which we observed olfactory interference, suggesting a relationship between olfactory interference and backward pairing induced inhibitory conditioning. Therefore, the animals from the previous experiment (Figure 3) were trained using a single forward pairing conditioning trial one hour after the olfactory interference protocol. The training consisted of presenting the odor used during olfactory interference (a new odor in the “nothing” group) followed by feeding with sucrose. A retrieval test was performed 3 hours after this training to evaluate the performance of the bees. In this retrieval test, we recorded the proportion of bees responding to the odor. This protocol is summarized in Figure 4A, and the results can be seen in Figure 4B.
In order to try to find a relationship between the performance of the bees during the retrieval test and their behavior during the backward pairing, we defined a behavioral factor corresponding to different types of response during backward pairing. Levels of this behavioral factor are presented in Figure 4A. We analyzed the performance during the retrieval test by using a forward stepwise logistic regression with three factors: odor (1-nonanol or octanal), foraging group (nectar or pollen), the behavioral factor and all of the possible interactions between factors. The stepwise procedure only kept one significant variable: the behavior factor (4 degrees of freedom χ2 = 24.832, p = 5.4 * 10−5). Figure 4B clearly illustrates that the performance of the group that retracts the proboscis early as well as the performance of the olfactory interference group are lower than in the group in which the proboscis was extended beyond 19 s (respectively 1 degree of freedom χ2 = 24.820, p = 6.3* 10−7 and χ2 = 6.148, p = 0.013). This indicates that olfactory interference disrupts the benefits of having a long lasting (>15 s) PER after sucrose feeding. Through interruption of proboscis extension, olfactory interference impaired subsequent learning, probably by forming an inhibitory association between the PER and the odor. This latter result is consistent with a previous report showing that following a 15 s backward pairing honey bees learn poorly relative to controls when given excitatory conditioning with the same CS [29]. In regard to our work, the major finding from this experiment is that olfactory interference is linked to backward inhibitory conditioning and is, as we will argue below, a kind of “inhibitory sensitization”.
However, this result does not specify whether olfactory interference generates a specific inhibition concerning only the odor that elicited it, or whether olfactory interference is unspecific (the bee learns to be unresponsive whatever the odor). To address this question, we used nectar foragers to perform a 15 s backward pairing with either 1-nonanol or octanal, as in our olfactory interference experiments above. This treatment was followed one hour later by two forward pairing trials, one of each with the olfactory interference odor and the other one with the other odor (trials were in random order and separated by 15 minutes). This conditioning was followed by a retrieval test for both odors 3 hours after. If olfactory interference inhibition is specific to the odor that was used during backward pairing, then the performance toward the novel odor should exceed that to the backward paired odor.
The results are presented Figure 5. We examined the treatment effect on three factors: the odor used during backward pairing (1-nonanol or octanal), the behavior of the bee during the backward pairing (retracted its proboscis before, during or after the odor presentation) and whether the odor had been presented during backward pairing or was the novel odor (this factor is a within individual measurement). Neither these three factors nor their interactions had a significant effect (generalized estimating equations, p>0.050 in all cases). This means that the bees did not discriminate between the odor that had been pre exposed and other odor. As a result, olfactory interference is not specific to the odor with which it was conducted. In summary, inhibitory conditioning is correlated to olfactory interference. However, this inhibition is not odor specific and rather affects the tendency of the bee to release a PER.
Cimetidine, histamine and sucrose sensitivity
In a first attempt to unravel the potential role of inhibitory receptors in olfactory interference, we assessed the effect of potential antagonists and agonists of histamine receptors on backward pairing (see materials and methods for details); however, we cannot exclude that they target other types of receptors, because their specific effects are unknown in bees. We first examined the effect of injection of drugs or saline into the deutocerebrum on sensitivity to sucrose stimulation. Injection into the deutocerebrum affects two distinct areas: the antennal lobe (the first relay of olfactory information in the insect brain [56], [57]) and the dorsal lobe (this structure receives gustatory and mechanosensory information from the antennae [58]–[60]). Histamine and other inhibitory neurotransmitters have been shown to affect inhibition in the antennal lobe during the processing of olfactory information in the honeybee [33], [37], [48], [49]. Furthermore, the antennal lobe also plays a role in olfactory plasticity [30], [40], [61]–[66]. We therefore hypothesized that the drugs we used could affect olfactory interference and/or olfactory pathways.
Sucrose sensitivity was assessed before and after injection into the deutocerebrum with either cimetidine (antagonist; 1 or 10 mM) or histamine (agonist; 1 or 10 mM); saline alone (0 mM) served as the control. A modulation index (MI, see materials and methods and [42]) was computed for each bee. The MI evaluates changes in sucrose sensitivity as a result of, in our case, pharmacological treatment. Neither 1 mM nor 10 mM of cimetidine (Figure 6A) or histamine (Figure 6B) had a significant effect on sensitivity when injected in the deutocerebrum (cimetidine, Kruskall-Wallis test: χ2 = 1.743, 2 degrees of freedom, p = 0.418; histamine, Kruskall-Wallis test: χ2 = 1.046, 2 degrees of freedom, p = 0.593). In all cases the MI was equal to or close to zero. Experiments conducted with histamine led to a wider range of MI values (compare the interquartile interval in Figure 6A and 6B), but this effect is controlled by having a saline group specific to each experiment. Since the same range is apparent in the control group, it is unlikely to be due to an effect of histamine injection. Instead, it was probably a result of seasonal- or weather-related variation in bee physiology [67]–[69]. Furthermore, after the injection PER release for the highest sucrose concentration (30%) was similar across the three groups in both experiments (data not shown; cimetidine, Fisher's exact test: p = 0.568; histamine: χ2 = 0.663, 2 degrees of freedom, p = 0.718). In conclusion, histamine and cimetidine did not affect sucrose sensitivity or proboscis extension.
Effect of cimetidine on olfactory interference
Fifteen minutes before starting the olfactory interference experiment (conducted as above), cimetidine (either 1 or 10 mM) or saline control (0 mM) was injected into the deutocerebrum. The results are reported in Figure 7. Treatment with cimetidine showed a significant impairment of olfactory interference. There was no effect of cimetidine on performance before the odor was delivered (before the odor, 0–14 s, Wald test; 0 mM vs. 1 mM: χ2 = 0.143, p = 0.705; 0 mM vs. 10 mM: χ2 = 0.106, p = 0.744), but the 10 mM cimetidine group was significantly different from the saline group once the odor was delivered (Wald test; 0 mM vs. 10 mM during the odor, 15–19 s: χ2 = 4.169, p = 0.041). Bees in the 10 mM group failed to show the expected decrease in the rate of PER (i.e. there was no olfactory interference). The response of the 1 mM cimetidine group was intermediate between that of the 10 mM and saline groups, but it was not significantly different from the saline group (Wald test; 0 mM vs. 1 mM during the odor, 15–19 s: χ2 = 1.015, p = 0.314). After the odor, the behavior of the drug-treated groups were identical to that of the saline group (Wald test after the odor, 20–34 s; 0 mM vs. 1 mM: χ2 = 0.150, p = 0.699; 0 mM vs. 10 mM: χ2 = 0.373, p = 0.541).
We performed the following control experiments to rule out other interpretations of the pharmacological effects. The spatial specificity of the injection was confirmed by injecting cimetidine in the lobula of the optic lobe, instead of into the deutocerebrum. The lobula is not involved in chemosensory processing or olfactory learning and is a standard control for the spatial specificity of treatments in bee olfactory learning [62], [63], [70]. If the drug spreads from the injection site to other brain areas, then we could not localize the effect to the deuterocerebrum and injection into the lobula would have the same effect. However, injection into the lobula had no effect on olfactory interference (supplementary Figure S5). Similarly, we injected into the deutocerebrum either cimetidine (10 mM) or saline 15 minutes before presenting sucrose to the bees, but this time either an air puff or no stimulation at all was presented instead of the odor (Figure 8). In this situation, the respective cimetidine- and the saline-treated groups (comparison of both solid lines or both dashed lines in Figure 8) did not significantly differ at any time-period (Wald test, p>0.050), ruling out the possibility that cimetidine enhances expression of the PER. Interestingly, presenting an air puff to the saline-treated bees elicited a significant decrease of the PER, even though no odor was presented (comparison of blue solid and dashed lines in Figure 8). There was a significant difference between untreated bees and bees exposed to air puff at the 15–19 s time period (Wald test, χ2 = 4.620, p = 0.032), whereas these two groups did not differ at other time periods (0–15 s, χ2 = 0.070, p = 0.792; 20–34 s, χ2 = 0.119, p = 0.731). Hence, there could be a small “mechanosensory interference” effect as well, although it is not very consistent (Figure 1).
We conclude that cimetidine starts to impair olfactory interference at a dose between 1 mM and 10 mM (Figure 7). This effect cannot be explained by an effect of cimetidine on sucrose sensitivity (Figure 6A), nor on the course of the ongoing PER (Figure 8). Thus, cimetidine blocks odor perception and/or olfactory interference itself. Interestingly, pyrilamine had an effect on olfactory interference similar to that of cimetidine (see supplementary Figure S6).
Effect of histamine on olfactory interference
Histamine (1 or 10 mM) was injected to bees using saline (0 mM) as control, in a way identical to the experiment with cimetidine. Results are reported in Figure 9. There was no difference between the treated groups and the control group at any time, including during the odor presentation (Wald test: p>0.050 in all cases). This indicates that injection of exogenous histamine does not affect olfactory interference. Although cimetidine impaired olfactory interference, this does not necessary mean that histamine should enhance it. Indeed, it is possible that the histaminergic system in the deutocerebrum is already acting close to one end of its dynamic range. In this case, adding more histamine would not have an effect, whereas addition of the histamine receptor antagonist cimetidine would have measurable consequences. Alternatively, the effect of cimetidine might be unrelated to histamine receptors, especially because the effective dose (10 mM) is quite high.
Discussion
Olfactory interference
The main finding of our work is the observation of olfactory interference, previously unreported for honey bees. We have found that in olfactory interference, presentation of an odor stimulus 15 sec after sucrose feeding terminates an on-going PER (i.e. it desensitizes the bee). Moreover, presentation of odor earlier (7 sec) or later (30 sec) did not interfere with the ongoing PER. A similar phenomenon was previously reported by Dethier [54] in blow flies. When flies feed on a droplet of sucrose solution they respond appetitively for several seconds after feeding. Dethier reported that this sensitization could be ‘discharged’ by application of a water droplet to the mouthparts; however it is unclear whether this discharge effect resulted from a sensory or more central process. Olfactory interference in the honey bee is likely due to a central process, because it involves the interaction of taste, PER release and olfaction; it is unlikely that an odor stimulus would interfere with PER release or taste at a peripheral level.
Olfactory interference and inhibitory conditioning
Backward presentation of US and CS (i.e. the reverse of forward pairing that produces excitatory conditioning) inhibits subsequent conditioning with this CS [71]–[73]. Previous work with honey bees reported that backward pairing with a specific delay of 15 s produces inhibitory conditioning [29]. Similar backward inhibitory conditioning has been reported for aversive learning in fruit fly [74] (see also [75] for a study of backward pairing of various odors in bees). We show that olfactory interference is related to backward pairing based inhibitory conditioning in a way that is analogous to the relationship between sensitization and excitatory conditioning [15], [16]. In other words, forward excitatory conditioning is initially related to sensitization, whereas backward inhibitory conditioning is initially related to olfactory interference. Curiously, the only paper on backward pairing of odor and sucrose in honey bees [29] did not report olfactory interference. Inhibitory conditioning induced by backward pairing [29] and olfactory interference both are maximal at 15 s after sucrose presentation. Furthermore, shortened PER durations induced by olfactory interference are correlated to a lower performance on subsequent excitatory conditioning. This means that olfactory interference and inhibitory conditioning probably share, at least in part, some underlying mechanisms. Therefore, it seems likely that the neural and biochemical basis for olfactory interference is engaged for inhibitory conditioning, just as the neural and biochemical machinery for sensitization is engaged to drive excitatory conditioning [15], [16].
Interestingly, pairing the same stimuli (in our case, sucrose and odor) can lead to both excitatory and inhibitory conditioning depending on to the way the pairing is made. The decisive parameter for switching between excitation and inhibition is the timing of the pairing of odor and sucrose: forward pairing will produce excitatory learning, whereas backward pairing will lead to inhibitory conditioning through olfactory interference. Further work is needed to completely explore this effect, for example testing the effect of repeated backward pairing (to see if olfactory interference is facilitated and if subsequent learning becomes even more inhibited) or using various time delays between backward pairing and forward pairing (thus testing the duration of the inhibitory trace formed during backward pairing).
The air stimulus (a mechanosensory stimulation) carrying the odor might also elicit interference with the ongoing PER (Figure 8), although our results established that odor was more salient in causing the proboscis retraction (Figure 1). This suggests that antennal tactile [34], [39], [76]–[79] and antennal mechanosensory [80] conditioning could also be used in backward pairing with sucrose. We therefore predict the occurrence of a tactile/mechanosensory interference, as well as inhibitory tactile/mechanosensory conditioning. Moreover, olfactory aversive conditioning can also be done in laboratory conditions [81], [82]. The aversive conditioning paradigm rests upon extension of the sting elicited by an electric shock; several pairings of an odor with an electric shock condition the bee so that subsequent presentations of the same odor elicit extension of the sting. Olfactory interference and backward inhibitory learning could also occur with this protocol.
Finally, the main interest of olfactory interference is that it links the odor and the PER (CS/response association). Following the work of Hammer [14], [40], [64], most studies of the neural basis of olfactory conditioning have focused on finding central sites of convergence between olfactory and sucrose stimuli (CS/US association, [13], [14], [16]). However, during Pavlovian conditioning the CS/US association is not the only one to occur and CS/response association can also be elicited [83]. As olfactory interference is based on an interaction between PER and olfactory perception, understanding the basis of olfactory interference may shed new light on the basis of olfactory conditioning by providing the possibility of studying the interaction between PER and odor.
Pharmacological and neuroanatomical aspects
Further investigations of olfactory interference may establish the biochemical basis for its time course. We have reported our first investigation of the neural network basis of olfactory interference by injecting cimetidine into specific brain regions. In fruit flies, cimetidine is an antagonist of histamine receptors [84]–[86]. Because of the similarity between fruit fly and honey bee histamine receptors [84], cimetidine might block the histamine receptors in bees as well. However, in the honey bee the targets for cimetidine and the histamine receptors have not been characterized, so it is too early to specifically conclude that histamine receptors are involved in olfactory interference or olfactory perception. A reasonable, somewhat more conservative, conclusion is that cimetidine-sensitive receptors are involved in olfactory processing that gives rise to olfactory interference [33], [37], [48], [49], [88]. The effects of other drugs were not significant.
We made injections into the deutocerebrum, which includes the antennal lobes. Antennal lobes are analogs to the mammalian olfactory bulb as they are the first-order synaptic process of olfactory information [56], [57]. However, the antennal lobe is close to the dorsal lobe (together they form the deutocerebrum). Because of this proximity, it is likely that injection made in the antennal lobe affected the dorsal lobe as well. This is relevant because the dorsal lobe receives gustatory information from the antennae [59], and thus could also be involved in olfactory interference. As a localization control we injected cimetidine into the lobula of the optic lobe. If the drug had spread broadly to many areas of the brain, then injection into the optic lobe should have produced the same effect as injection into the antennal deutocerebrum. Failure to find this effect substantiates our argument that olfactory interference involves an interaction between olfactory and taste modalities in the deuterocerebrum.
As the antennal lobe is also the first relay of olfactory information, one explanation for the effect of an injection of cimetidine into the deutocerebrum could be that this drug blocks or modifies the perception of odor. In particular, this explanation is consistent with results previously obtained in insects that indicated that when inhibition is altered by injection of histamine, bicuculine or picrotoxin in the antennal lobe, it results in greatly increased odor detection thresholds [49], [88]. As a result, two hypotheses can be proposed for the neural substrate of olfactory interference. In the first one, olfactory interference is driven by the output of the antennal lobe, which affects the motor integration centers that control PER. In this case, the output of the antennal lobe would be either permissive (during olfactory conditioning) or repressive of the PER (during olfactory interference) depending on when odor is detected in the process of extending the proboscis. Cimetidine would block antennal lobe output, preventing the odor from affecting PER (probably because the odor is simply not detected). Alternatively, in the second hypothesis, the antennal lobe would not have a direct role in olfactory interference and would simply relay olfactory information; cimetidine would affect or block this relay. In this case, another part of the brain would gate the PER release as a function of olfactory information (according to the timing, the presence of an odor would switch the system to PER repression in olfactory interference or to PER release after olfactory conditioning).
A candidate for the location of the motor control of the PER is the protocerebral lobe. Indeed, previous results have suggested that this brain region could be a pre-motor area for the PER release [89]. More specifically, an inhibitory system would block the PER, and this inhibitory system would be activated by odor presentation during olfactory interference but deactivated after olfactory learning (from Figure 7 in [89]). To investigate these hypotheses further, calcium imaging [56] and electrophysiology [37] may be used in conjunction with pharmacology [49].
Materials and Methods
Animals
Honey bees workers (Apis mellifera) were standardized in regard to foraging task. Bees were collected in front of the hive entrance by capturing non-pollen foragers in glass vials as they returned to the colony. Pollen foragers can be identified by the pollen load carried on the hind legs, and for most experiments they were excluded because their sensitivity to sucrose differs from non-pollen foragers [55]. Furthermore, pollen foragers are less common, particularly at certain times of the year, whereas non-pollen foragers are readily available. However, in some experiments pollen foragers were used for comparison purposes (see results).
The bees were immobilized by leaving the vials on ice. Animals were then restrained in small metal tube with strips of tape on the abdomen and behind the head. Once they recovered from chilling, they were fed 15 µl of 1.17 M sucrose solution and kept overnight in a humidified box until they were used for experiments the next morning. When the animals were to be injected, the head was immobilized by a small drop of orthodontic sticky wax before feeding. In order to prepare them for injection (see below), a window was cut in the cuticle above the antennae and between the eyes immediately after the preparation of all the bees. The antennal lobes were then revealed by gently pushing aside the glands and the white tracheal sheath. The survival rate after the operation varied between 30% and 75%, according to the season.
Behavioral procedures: olfactory interference
All experiments started by presenting 0.6 µl of 1.17 M sucrose solution to both antennae and then to the proboscis, except for one experiment when the effect of presenting the sucrose to antennae only was investigated (supplementary Figure S3). The bees were allowed time to drink all of the solution (i.e. a few seconds), which ensured that all bees had consumed the same amount of sucrose. Under normal circumstances, the proboscis can remain extended and appetitive feeding movements may continue after the sucrose droplet disappears. We evaluated the effect of odor presentation on this ongoing response by presenting odor for 4 s beginning at three different points after feeding onset (defined at the moment when the tip of the proboscis touches the sucrose solution droplet). In different experiments, delays of 7 s, 15 s or 30 s were used. When bees were treated with a drug before the experiment (see below), the injection took place 15 minutes before the sucrose stimulation.
In some experiments, bees were also trained using olfactory conditioning. A conditioning trial consisted of presenting an odor for 4 s, and 3 s after the onset of the odor 0.6 µl of 1.17 M sucrose solution was presented to both antennae and then to the proboscis. The bees consumed the sucrose solution. Three hours after conditioning, bees were tested (retrieval test) by presenting the same odor without reinforcement and the response was recorded. If the bee did not release a PER to the odor during this retrieval test, the sucrose solution was presented again; if the bee failed to respond to the sucrose, then its performance was not taken into account.
The odor was delivered by an electronically controlled device that directed an air flow (ca 1450 ml/min) through a syringe containing a strip of filter paper bearing 3 µl of the pure odorant. The odor strips were freshly prepared for each experiment. The syringe remained 2 cm in front of the platform on which the bees were placed for the whole experiment. The platform was positioned in front of an exhaust hood to prevent odorant accumulation after delivery of the odor. Odors used were 1-nonanol or octanal (both from VWR).
Sucrose feeding led to a long-lasting PER that often continued even after the bee had finished drinking the sucrose solution. We measured the PER initial duration, defined as the duration of the PER after sucrose feeding onset. The performance of the bees was videotaped for 35 s after the feeding onset, and for each second we recorded whether the initial PER was still occurring (i.e. analyses were always done in discreet time). Bees would sometimes briefly retract their proboscis before resuming the PER; retractions of one second or less were not considered to interrupt the initial PER. In theory, bees that retract their proboscis could restart to release a PER when odor is presented if they are sensitized. However, we hardly ever observed that a bee that stopped releasing the initial PER for more than one second would resume it, whether or not an odor was presented. Thus, the total time spent displaying the PER is strongly correlated to this initial duration (see supplementary Figure S1 for comparison of the two values and a detailed discussion of this aspect).
Behavioral procedures: sucrose sensitivity
Sucrose sensitivity is an important parameter correlated with many honey bee behaviors [90] and the effects of drugs on sucrose sensitivity can be assessed using the protocol developed by Scheiner et al. [42]. An important first step before using drugs in olfactory interference was to check the potential of effect of these drugs on sucrose sensitivity. A series of six sucrose solutions of various concentrations was presented in random order (0.1%, 0.3%, 1%, 3%, 10% and 30%, with 10% = 100 g.l−1 or 29 mM; this corresponds to an increasing logarithmic series: −1, −0.5, 0, 0.5, 1, 1.5). For each one of these solutions, both antennae were touched by a 0.6 µl drop of sucrose solution and the occurrence of PER was recorded; animals were not fed with the sucrose to prevent satiation effects. There was a delay of 2 minutes between each concentration. To avoid sensitization or habituation of the animals to repeated sucrose presentation, a 0.6 µl drop of distilled water was presented to both antennae between each sucrose presentation (1 minute before the next sucrose presentation). Furthermore, an interval of 2 minutes between sucrose presentations does not promote habituation to sucrose [91].
After the first presentation of the whole series, animals received an injection of saline (vehicle of the drugs, see below) or drug (histamine or cimetidine, 1 or 10 mM). All sucrose solutions were then presented as previously 15 minutes after the injection. For each bee, we recorded the number of PERs during the presentation of the sucrose solution series before (PRE) and after (POST) the injection; as there were six sucrose solutions presented, PRE and POST scores ranged from 0 to 6. We computed for each bee a modulation index (MI) according to Scheiner et al. [42]: MI = (POST−PRE)/(POST+PRE). MI varies from −1 (for a bee responding to sucrose before injection and stopping doing so after the injection) to 1 (for a bee not responding to any sucrose concentration before the injection and starting doing so after the injection). A value of 0 denotes the same performance after and before the injection. When both PRE and POST were equal to 0, MI was defined as 0 (no change).
Drugs and injection
Drugs were injected into both antennal lobes. However, it is likely that an injection in the antennal lobe affects the whole deutocerebrum, i.e. the antennal lobe plus the dorsal lobe. Therefore, to be conservative we used the term “injection into the deutocerebrum” throughout the text. The properties of histamine receptors in the deutocerebrum of adult honey bees are not known. Therefore, we chose drugs according to their effects on fruit fly histamine receptors [84]–[86], as they are similar in molecular structure to receptors in the honey bee and conserved across different insects [87], [92]. The drugs used (all from Tocris) were histamine (agonist of histamine receptors), cimetidine, pyrilamine ( = mepyramine) and dimaprit (in fruit fly, the last three are antagonists of histamine receptors; the results for pyrilamine and dimaprit are used in supplementary Figure S6). All the drugs were dissolved in a bee phosphate buffered saline solution which was also used for control injection (pH 7.8, 609 mosmol.l−1: 2.7 mM KCl, 154.0 mM NaCl, 1.8 mM CaCl2, 11.7 mM sucrose, 80.5 mM Na2HPO4, 18.5 mM NaH2PO4).
The solutions to be injected were loaded into a glass microelectrode connected to a microinjector (picospritzer II, Parker Hannifin Instrumentation). These microelectrodes were made by pulling quartz glass capillaries (outer diameter 1 mm, inner diameter 0.5 mm) with a microelectrode puller (P2000, Sutter Instrument Company). Then the pulled, unopened tip of each capillary was gently broken. The injection volume was calibrated by measuring with a graduated ocular the size of drops injected in mineral oil. Each deutocerebrum received a volume of 4.2 nl. Immediately after the injections, the injected volume was checked again, and if variation had occurred the bee was not used.
Data treatment
Unless otherwise mentioned, statistical analyses were performed with SPSS 15.0 (SPSS Science). All the tests were two-tailed. Statistics were evaluated using an α risk of 0.050. For sucrose sensitivity experiments, we compared the MI across the groups using the Kruskall-Wallis test, as this index is not distributed normally. We also compared the proportion of honey bees releasing a PER following the injection for the 30% sucrose solution, using χ2 tests, or Fisher's exact test (computed with R 2.4, http://www.r-project.org/ [93]) if the χ2 test assumptions were not met. In the conditioning experiment, we used the generalized linear model (forward stepwise logistic regression or generalized estimating equation) to assess the role on the performance of different factors.
For olfactory interference experiments, we used time-dependent Cox regression (also known as proportional hazard survival analysis, procedure COXREG in SPSS; see [94]) to compare the initial duration of the PER between different groups (that is, the “survival” of the PER across the time). This technique is especially designed to treat this type of data, and it allows us to take into account the few bees that were still releasing a PER at the end of the behavioral monitoring. We used the time-dependent version of Cox regression in order to consider the different time-periods: before odor onset, during and one second after the 4 s odor presentation and after the odor presentation. As there were usually few bees still extending their proboscis at the end of the recording period, it sometimes happened that the Cox regression could not yield p-values for these periods; this meant that there was not enough animals still displaying a PER to observe a relevant effect.
It is important to understand that the Cox regression compares across treatment groups the probability (‘risk’ in regard to the terminology of Cox regression) of terminating PER during a given time period; or equivalently, the “survival rate” of the PER in the considered time-period. This is better and more convenient than other types of statistical analyses (e.g. χ2) that would have repeatedly compared the proportion of animals exhibiting PER at each second and is more specific than comparing the PER duration with an ANOVA (which ignores the time-period).
We defined contrast tests for each of the time-periods (before, during and after the odor) to compare the probability of PER termination between treatment groups (in other words, the “survival” of the PER). The statistical significance of these contrasts was evaluated with Wald test (distributed like a 1 degree of freedom χ2). As contrasts are required to be orthogonal from each other, only a limited number of tests could be done. Thus, we could only compare each drug concentration (or each treatment) to the corresponding control group as supplementary contrasts would not have been independent. For all experiments, the performance of the bees was similar for both odors (1-nonanol vs. octanal within each group, before, during and after odor presentation: Wald test, p≥0.050 in all cases), so we pooled the results of the two odors (octane and 1-nonanol) in all figures and analysis (except in Figure 1 where results for the two odors are shown separately as an example).
Each contrast (e.g. control vs. treated) corresponds to the fitted ratio of survival rates in two groups, e.g. (fitted survival rate in treated)/(fitted survival rate in control). These values are plotted with their 95% confidence interval. When this confidence interval does not include 1, then the corresponding Wald test is significant, meaning that the survival rates are not the same in the considered period. For instance, if the ratio (fitted survival rate in treated)/(fitted survival rate in control) is lower than one during a given time period, this means that the bees retract their proboscis more often in the treated group than in the control group. These confidence intervals are reported in graphs along with the proportion of bees still displaying the initial PER as a function of time.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Christophe Pouzat for statistical advice, and Monique Gauthier and Julie A. Mustard for commenting on an earlier version of this article.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This research was funded by the NIH grants (NCRR grant RR014166 and NIDCD grant DC007997) to BHS. The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study, in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data, and in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Menzel R, Giurfa M. Dimensions of cognition in an insect, the honeybee. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2006;5:24–40. doi: 10.1177/1534582306289522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Menzel R, Leboulle G, Eisenhardt D. Small Brains, Bright Minds. Cell. 2006;124:237–239. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Menzel R, Giurfa M. Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:62–71. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01601-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Milner B, Squire LR, Kandel ER. Cognitive neuroscience and the study of memory. Neuron. 1998;20:445–468. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80987-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Krasne FB, Glanzman DL. What We can Learn from Invertebrate Learning. Annual Review of Psychology. 1995;46:585–624. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Shafir S, Waite T, Smith B. Context dependent violations of rational choice in honeybees (Apis mellifera) and gray jays (Perisoreus canadensis). Behav ecol sociobiol. 2002;51:180–187. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Van Swinderen B, Greenspan RJ. Salience modulates 20–30 Hz brain activity in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:579–586. doi: 10.1038/nn1054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Van Swinderen B. The remote roots of consciousness in fruit-fly selective attention? Bioessays. 2005;27:321–330. doi: 10.1002/bies.20195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Giurfa M, Eichmann B, Menzel R. Symmetry perception in an insect. Nature. 1996;382:458–461. doi: 10.1038/382458a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Giurfa M, Zhang S, Jenett A, Menzel R, Srinivasan MV. The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect. Nature. 2001;410:930–933. doi: 10.1038/35073582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Stach S, Benard J, Giurfa M. Local-feature assembling in visual pattern recognition and generalization in honeybees. Nature. 2004;429:758–761. doi: 10.1038/nature02594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Giurfa M. Cognitive neuroethology: dissecting non-elemental learning in a honeybee brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:726–735. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2003.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Giurfa M. Behavioral and neural analysis of associative learning in the honeybee: a taste from the magic well. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 2007;193:801–824. doi: 10.1007/s00359-007-0235-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hammer M, Menzel R. Learning and memory in the honeybee. J Neurosci. 1995;15:1617–1630. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-03-01617.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Menzel R. Learning, memory and “cognition” in honeybees. In: Kesner R, Olton D, editors. Neurobiology of comparative cognition. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum associates, Publishers; 1990. pp. 237–292. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Menzel R. Memory dynamics in the honeybee. J Comp Physiol A. 1999;185:323–340. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Page RE, Jr, Erber J. Levels of behavioral organization and the evolution of division of labor. Naturwissenschaften. 2002;89:91–106. doi: 10.1007/s00114-002-0299-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Shafir S, Wiegmann DD, Smith BH, Real LA. Risk-sensitive foraging: choice behaviour of honeybees in response to variability in volume of reward. Animal Behaviour. 1999;57:1055–1061. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Shafir S, Menda G, Smith BH. Caste-specific differences in risk sensitivity in honeybees, Apis mellifera. Animal Behaviour. 2005;69:859–868. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Deisig N, Lachnit H, Giurfa M. The effect of similarity between elemental stimuli and compounds in olfactory patterning discriminations. Learn Mem. 2002;9:112–121. doi: 10.1101/lm.41002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Deisig N, Lachnit H, Giurfa M, Hellstern F. Configural olfactory learning in honeybees: negative and positive patterning discrimination. Learn Mem. 2001;8:70–78. doi: 10.1101/lm.8.2.70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Menzel R, Greggers U, Smith A, Berger S, Brandt R, et al. Honey bees navigate according to a map-like spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:3040–3045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408550102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Menzel R, De Marco RJ, Greggers U. Spatial memory, navigation and dance behaviour in Apis mellifera. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2006;192:889–903. doi: 10.1007/s00359-006-0136-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Benard J, Stach S, Giurfa M. Categorization of visual stimuli in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Animal Cognition. 2006;9:257–270. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0032-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Couvillon PA, Ferreira TP, Bitterman ME. Delayed alternation in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Psychol. 2003;117:31–35. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. From Antenna to Antenna: Lateral Shift of Olfactory Memory Recall by Honeybees. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e2340. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Von Frisch K. The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Chadwick L, translator. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press; 1967. p. 592. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bitterman ME, Menzel R, Fietz A, Schafer S. Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Psychol. 1983;97:107–119. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hellstern F, Malaka R, Hammer M. Backward inhibitory learning in honeybees: a behavioral analysis of reinforcement processing. Learn Mem. 1998;4:429–444. doi: 10.1101/lm.4.5.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Faber T, Joerges J, Menzel R. Associative learning modifies neural representations of odors in the insect brain. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:74–78. doi: 10.1038/4576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sandoz JC, Galizia CG, Menzel R. Side-specific olfactory conditioning leads to more specific odor representation between sides but not within sides in the honeybee antennal lobes. Neuroscience. 2003;120:1137–1148. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00384-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Guerrieri F, Schubert M, Sandoz J-C, Giurfa M. Perceptual and Neural Olfactory Similarity in Honeybees. PLoS Biology. 2005;3:e60. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Sachse S, Galizia CG. Role of inhibition for temporal and spatial odor representation in olfactory output neurons: a calcium imaging study. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1106–1117. doi: 10.1152/jn.00325.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Dacher M, Gauthier M. Involvement of NO-synthase and nicotinic receptors in learning in the honey bee. Physiol Behav in press. 2008 doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mercer A, Menzel R. The effects of biogenic amines on conditioned and unconditioned responses to olfactory stimuli in the honeybee Apis mellifera. J Comp Physiol A. 1982;145:363–368. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Farooqui T, Robinson K, Vaessin H, Smith BH. Modulation of early olfactory processing by an octopaminergic reinforcement pathway in the honeybee. J Neurosci. 2003;23:5370–5380. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05370.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stopfer M, Bhagavan S, Smith BH, Laurent G. Impaired odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assemblies. Nature. 1997;390:70–74. doi: 10.1038/36335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cano Lozano V, Armengaud C, Gauthier M. Memory impairment induced by cholinergic antagonists injected into the mushroom bodies of the honeybee. J Comp Physiol A. 2001;187:249–254. doi: 10.1007/s003590100196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dacher M, Lagarrigue A, Gauthier M. Antennal tactile learning in the honeybee: Effect of nicotinic antagonists on memory dynamics. Neuroscience. 2005;130:37–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hammer M, Menzel R. Multiple sites of associative odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections of octopamine in honeybees. Learn Mem. 1998;5:146–156. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Menzel R, Heyne A, Kinzel C, Gerber B, Fiala A. Pharmacological dissociation between the reinforcing, sensitizing, and response-releasing functions of reward in honeybee classical conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 1999;113:744–754. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Scheiner R, Pluckhahn S, Oney B, Blenau W, Erber J. Behavioural pharmacology of octopamine, tyramine and dopamine in honey bees. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:545–553. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00205-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Maleszka R, Helliwell P, Kucharski R. Pharmacological interference with glutamate re-uptake impairs long-term memory in the honeybee, apis mellifera. Behav Brain Res. 2000;115:49–53. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00235-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gauthier M, Dacher M, Thany SH, Niggebrugge C, Deglise P, et al. Involvement of alpha-bungarotoxin-sensitive nicotinic receptors in long-term memory formation in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2006;86:164–174. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.El Hassani AK, Dacher M, Gauthier M, Armengaud C. Effects of sublethal doses of fipronil on the behavior of the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;82:30–39. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.El Hassani A, Dacher M, Gary V, Lambin M, Gauthier M, et al. Effects of Sublethal Doses of Acetamiprid and Thiamethoxam on the Behavior of the Honeybee (Apis mellifera). Archives of Environmental Contamination and Toxicology. 2008;54:653–661. doi: 10.1007/s00244-007-9071-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Müller U. Inhibition of nitric oxide synthase impairs a distinct form of long-term memory in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Neuron. 1996;16:541–549. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80073-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hosler JS, Buxton KL, Smith BH. Impairment of olfactory discrimination by blockade of GABA and nitric oxide activity in the honey bee antennal lobes. Behav Neurosci. 2000;114:514–525. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Sachse S, Peele P, Silbering A, Guhmann M, Galizia CG. Role of histamine as a putative inhibitory transmitter in the honeybee antennal lobe. Frontiers in Zoology. 2006;3:22. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-3-22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pelz C, Gerber B, Menzel R. Odorant intensity as a determinant for olfactory conditioning in honeybees: roles in discrimination, overshadowing and memory consolidation. J Exp Biol. 1997;200(Pt 4):837–847. doi: 10.1242/jeb.200.4.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hammer M, Braun G, Mauelshagen J. Food-induced arousal and nonassociative learning in honeybees: Dependence of sensitization on the application site and duration of food stimulation. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1994;62:210–223. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(05)80019-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kralj J, Brockmann A, Fuchs S, Tautz J. The parasitic mite Varroa destructor affects non-associative learning in honey bee foragers, Apis mellifera L. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 2007;193:363–370. doi: 10.1007/s00359-006-0192-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Sandoz JC, Hammer M, Menzel R. Side-specificity of olfactory learning in the honeybee: US input side. Learn Mem. 2002;9:337–348. doi: 10.1101/lm.50502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Dethier VG. To know a fly. San Francisco: Holden Day; 1963. p. 119. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Pankiw T, Page RE., Jr The effect of genotype, age, sex, and caste on response thresholds to sucrose and foraging behavior of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). J Comp Physiol A. 1999;185:207–213. doi: 10.1007/s003590050379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Galizia CG, Menzel R. The role of glomeruli in the neural representation of odours: results from optical recording studies. J Insect Physiol. 2001;47:115–130. doi: 10.1016/s0022-1910(00)00106-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Vosshall LB, Stocker RF. Molecular Architecture of Smell and Taste in Drosophila. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2007;30:505–533. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Haupt SS. Antennal sucrose perception in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.): behaviour and electrophysiology. J Comp Physiol A. 2004;190:735–745. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0532-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Haupt S. Central gustatory projections and side-specificity of operant antennal muscle conditioning in the honeybee. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 2007;193:523–535. doi: 10.1007/s00359-007-0208-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kloppenburg P. Anatomy of the antennal motoneurons in the brain of the honeybee (Apis mellifera). J Comp Neurol. 1995;363:333–343. doi: 10.1002/cne.903630213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Thany SH, Gauthier M. Nicotine injected into the antennal lobes induces a rapid modulation of sucrose threshold and improves short-term memory in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Brain Res. 2005;1039:216–219. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.01.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Déglise P, Dacher M, Dion E, Gauthier M, Armengaud C. Regional brain variations of cytochrome oxidase staining during olfactory learning in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:540–547. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Erber J, Masuhr T, Menzel R. Localization of short term memory in the brain of the bee, Apis mellifera. Physiol Entomol. 1980;5:343–358. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hammer M. An identified neuron mediates the unconditioned stimulus in associative olfactory learning in honeybee. Nature. 1993;366:59–63. doi: 10.1038/366059a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Grünbaum L, Müller U. Induction of a specific olfactory memory leads to a long-lasting activation of protein kinase C in the antennal lobe of the honeybee. J Neurosci. 1998;18:4384–4392. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-11-04384.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Müller U. Prolonged activation of cAMP-dependent protein kinase during conditioning induces long-term memory in honeybees. Neuron. 2000;27:159–168. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00017-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Scheiner R, Barnert M, Erber J. Variation in water and sucrose responsiveness during the foraging season affects proboscis extension learning in honeybees. Apidologie. 2003;34:67–72. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Ray S, Ferneyhough B. Seasonal variation of proboscis extension reflex conditioning in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). J apicult res. 1997;36:108–110. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Riessberger U, Crailsheim K. Short term effect of different weather conditions upon the behaviour of forager and nurse honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica Pollman). Apidologie. 1997;28:411–426. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Oleskevich S, Clements JD, Srinivasan MV. Long-term synaptic plasticity in the honeybee. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:528–532. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.1.528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Denniston JC, Miller RR. Timing of omitted events: an analysis of temporal control of inhibitory behavior. Behav Processes. 2007;74:274–285. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Vogel EH, Castro ME, Saavedra MA. Quantitative models of Pavlovian conditioning. Brain Res Bull. 2004;63:173–202. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bouton ME, Moody EW. Memory processes in classical conditioning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28:663–674. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Tanimoto H, Heisenberg M, Gerber B. Event timing turns punishment to reward. Nature. 2004;430:983. doi: 10.1038/430983a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Hussaini SA, Komischke B, Menzel R, Lachnit H. Forward and backward second-order Pavlovian conditioning in honeybees. Learn Mem. 2007;14:678–683. doi: 10.1101/lm.471307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Erber J, Kierzek S, Sander E, Grandy K. Tactile learning in the honeybee. J Comp Physiol A. 1998;183:737–744. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Erber J, Pribbenow B, Grandy K, Kierzek S. Tactile motor learning in the antennal system of the honeybee (Apis mellifera L.). J Comp Physiol A. 1997;181:355–365. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Erber J, Pribbenow B, Kisch J, Faensen D. Operant conditioning of antennal muscle activity in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). J Comp Physiol A. 2000;186:557–565. doi: 10.1007/s003590000111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Kisch J, Erber J. Operant conditioning of antennal movements in the honey bee. Behav Brain Res. 1999;99:93–102. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00076-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Giurfa M, Malun D. Associative mechanosensory conditioning of the proboscis extension reflex in honeybees. Learn Mem. 2004;11:294–302. doi: 10.1101/lm.63604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Vergoz V, Roussel E, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Aversive learning in honeybees revealed by the olfactory conditioning of the sting extension reflex. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e288. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Vergoz V, Schreurs HA, Mercer AR. Queen Pheromone Blocks Aversive Learning in Young Worker Bees. Science. 2007;317:384–386. doi: 10.1126/science.1142448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Pavlov IP. Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. London: Oxford University Press; 1927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Jones AK, Sattelle DB. The cys-loop ligand-gated ion channel superfamily of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Invert Neurosci. 2006;6:123–132. doi: 10.1007/s10158-006-0026-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Gisselmann G, Pusch H, Hovemann BT, Hatt H. Two cDNAs coding for histamine-gated ion channels in D. melanogaster. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:11–12. doi: 10.1038/nn787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Gisselmann G, Plonka J, Pusch H, Hatt H. Unusual functional properties of homo- and heteromultimeric histamine-gated chloride channels of Drosophila melanogaster: spontaneous currents and dual gating by GABA and histamine. Neurosci Lett. 2004;372:151–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Zheng Y, Hirschberg B, Yuan J, Wang AP, Hunt DC, et al. Identification of two novel Drosophila melanogaster histamine-gated chloride channel subunits expressed in the eye. J Biol Chem. 2002;277:2000–2005. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M107635200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Mwilaria EK, Ghatak C, Daly KC. Disruption of GABAA in the Insect Antennal Lobe Generally Increases Odor Detection and Discrimination Thresholds. Chem Senses. 2008;33:267–281. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjm085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Okada R, Rybak J, Manz G, Menzel R. Learning-Related Plasticity in PE1 and Other Mushroom Body-Extrinsic Neurons in the Honeybee Brain. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11736–11747. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2216-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Scheiner R, Page RE, Erber J. Sucrose responsiveness and behavioral plasticity in honey bees (Apis mellifera). Apidologie. 2004;35:133–142. [Google Scholar]
- 91.Haupt SS, Klemt W. Habituation and dishabituation of exploratory and appetitive responses in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). Behav Brain Res. 2005;165:12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Jones AK, Sattelle DB. The cys-loop ligand-gated ion channel gene superfamily of the red flour beetle, Tribolium castaneum. BMC Genomics. 2007;8:327. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-8-327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Ihaka R, Gentleman R. R: A Language for Data Analysis and Graphics. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics. 1996;5:299–314. [Google Scholar]
- 94.Lee ET, Wang JW. Statistical Methods for Survival Data Analysis. New York: Wiley-Interscience; 2003. p. 534. [Google Scholar]
- 95.Scheiner R, Page RE, Jr, Erber J. The effects of genotype, foraging role, and sucrose responsiveness on the tactile learning performance of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2001;76:138–150. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2000.3996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Scheiner R, Page RE, Jr, Erber J. Responsiveness to sucrose affects tactile and olfactory learning in preforaging honey bees of two genetic strains. Behav Brain Res. 2001;120:67–73. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00359-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Scheiner R, Erber J, Page RE., Jr Tactile learning and the individual evaluation of the reward in honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). J Comp Physiol A. 1999;185:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s003590050360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.