Abstract
Increased computer use in clinical settings offers an opportunity to develop new neuropsychological tests that exploit the control computers have over stimulus dimensions and timing. However, before adopting new tools, empirical validation is necessary. In the current study, our aims were twofold: to describe a computerized adaptive procedure with broad potential for neuropsychological investigations, and to demonstrate its implementation in testing for visual hemispatial neglect. Visual search results from adaptive psychophysical procedures are reported from 12 healthy individuals and 23 individuals with unilateral brain injury. Healthy individuals reveal spatially symmetric performance on adaptive search measures. In patients, psychophysical outcomes (as well as those from standard paper-and-pencil search tasks) reveal visual hemispatial neglect. Consistent with previous empirical studies of hemispatial neglect, lateralized impairments in adaptive conjunction search are greater than in adaptive feature search tasks. Furthermore, those with right hemisphere damage show greater lateralized deficits in conjunction search than do those with left hemisphere damage. We argue that adaptive tests, which automatically adjust to each individual’s performance level, are efficient methods for both clinical evaluations and neuropsychological investigations, and have the potential to detect subtle deficits even in chronic stages, when flagrant clinical signs have frequently resolved.
MESH TERMS: Neuropsychology, Visual Search, Diagnosis, Attention, Vision Tests, Perceptual Disorders
INTRODUCTION
Reliable and valid diagnostic tests form the foundation for neuropsychological assessment across many domains of cognition. These paper-and-pencil tests are often standardized and are relatively easy to administer, requiring minimal equipment. For instance, line cancellation, line bisection and visual search have played a major role in measuring and studying visual hemispatial neglect (a neurological disorder in which patients fail to attend to or make explicit use of contralesional information; e.g., Albert, 1973; Behrmann et al., 2004; Buxbaum et al., 2004; Ferber & Karnath, 2001; Halligan & Marshall, 1991). Although traditional paper-and-pencil tests are useful in assessing clinical signs, computerized testing can offer better control of stimulus parameters. By exploiting dynamic control over stimulus presentation time, speed of stimulus motion or stimulus salience, some investigations have already shown that otherwise-obscured factors can contribute to neglect (e.g., Brooks et al., 2005; Deouell et al., 2005).
Nevertheless, almost all widely-used tests of visual hemispatial neglect (computerized or otherwise) use fixed stimulus parameters. For example, most experiments use only a single duration for stimulus presentation. If the chosen duration is too long, only patients with the most severe impairments may show a deficit, whereas those with subtle deficits will not1. Conversely, if the chosen stimulus duration is too short, all patients may perform poorly and there may be little differentiation between individuals’ performance (regardless of the severity of their impairments). Analogous assessment failures can occur when manipulating other fixed stimulus parameters (e.g., contrast, intensity, position).
In neuropsychological populations, a truly successful diagnostic tool should accommodate each individual’s level of impairment (as argued by Barrett et al., 2006 and others). Adaptive psychophysical (or, staircase) procedures permit this diagnostic flexibility. In adaptive procedures, a chosen stimulus parameter (e.g., stimulus duration) is manipulated until a desired level of performance is reached. The resultant value of the parameter at a particular level of performance (e.g., 500 ms duration required for 75% accuracy) can then be used as a quantitative index of impairment.
Here, we used such adaptive psychophysical procedures to assess visual hemispatial neglect in patients with unilateral brain injury. We employed feature and conjunction search tasks for two reasons. First, we wanted to compare adaptive search measures with outcomes from traditional search tasks, which are frequently included in neuropsychological assessments of neglect (e.g., the Behavioural Inattention Tests [BIT], Wilson et al., 1987; Standardized Comprehensive Assessment of Neglect [SCAN], McGlinchey-Berroth et al., 1996). Second, we wanted to determine whether the adaptive search measures reflect a dissociation previously found in fixed-parameter search studies. Specifically, patients with hemispatial neglect have shown greater difficulty finding conjunctions of features (such as a red square among red triangles and blue squares) than finding singleton features (such as a blue circle among red and yellow circles) when presented in their contralesional field (e.g., Eglin et al., 1989, 1991, 1994; Esterman et al., 2000; Hildebrandt et al., 2005; Riddoch, & Humphreys, 1987; Laeng et al., 2002; Pavlovskaya et al., 2002)2.
Furthermore, we focused our investigation on lateralized visual impairments in hemispatial neglect. We minimized response demands by requiring participants to respond verbally without time pressure. The adaptive procedure we used also incorporates a response bias correction (Kaernbach, 1990; see Methods)3. We thereby reduced the influence of concurrent motor deficits and response decision factors on visual search performance.
In the experiments that follow, we examined the ability of adaptive procedures to capture lateralized visual search impairments. In Experiment 1, we tested a group of healthy individuals to determine whether performance was symmetric across visual fields when using adaptive procedures in visual search. In Experiment 2, we tested a group of patients with unilateral brain injury on both standard paper-and-pencil search tasks, and adaptive feature and conjunction search tasks. We compared results from the standard and adaptive tasks, and examined whether adaptive outcomes replicate previous fixed-measure findings differentiating feature and conjunction search performance. In Experiment 3, we re-tested a subset of patients using better matched search displays than were used in Experiment 2, ruling out a potential stimulus confound. Together, the experiments reveal adaptive procedures to be a useful tool via which neuropsychological deficits may be characterized.
EXPERIMENT 1
Twelve healthy individuals performed three adaptive search tasks: two feature search tasks (Feature Search [FS] and Scattered Feature Search [SFS]) and a Conjunction Search (CS) task. No lateralized differences were expected in healthy individuals’ performance.
Methods
Participants
Twelve healthy individuals (7 women) gave informed consent before participation, in compliance with the University of California, Berkeley’s Office for the Protection of Human Subjects. Their mean age was 63 years (range=52-70). Two participants were ambidextrous, and all others were right-handed (Table 1).
Table 1.
Participant | Sex | Age | Handedness |
---|---|---|---|
OC1 | M | 52 | A |
OC2 | F | 55 | R |
OC3 | F | 56 | A |
OC4 | M | 58 | R |
OC5 | M | 64 | R |
OC6 | M | 66 | R |
OC7 | F | 66 | R |
OC8 | M | 68 | R |
OC9 | F | 68 | R |
OC10 | F | 68 | R |
OC11 | F | 69 | R |
OC12 | F | 70 | R |
Note. M=Male, F=Female, A=Ambidextrous, R=Right.
Apparatus
Participants viewed stimuli from 60-70 cm, with their line of sight perpendicular to the widescreen LCD panel of a laptop computer. Screen resolution was 1280 by 800 pixels and the refresh rate was 60 Hz. The adaptive procedures were controlled by Presentation software (http://www.neurobs.com/).
Stimuli
Feature Search
Displays contained eight “O”s (Arial font, size 40). Items were arranged every 45° along an invisible 5.72°-radius ring, centered about the central fixation crosshair (Figure 1). None of the items fell on the vertical or horizontal meridians of the display. Targets were blue (RGB=14, 67, 202) and distracters were red (RGB=255, 0, 0), presented on a gray (RGB=190, 190, 190) background. Targets appeared equiprobably at each location. Target-present trials contained one target, whereas target-absent trials contained none.
Scattered Feature Search
Displays contained a central fixation crosshair and 14 squares (spanning 1.64°/side). Red targets and blue distracters were presented on a gray background. Colors were as above. One item was always presented in each of 14 locations (positioned between 5.72° and 8.17° from center; Figure 2). Targets appeared equiprobably at all locations. Target-present trials contained one target, whereas target-absent trials contained none.
Conjunction Search
Displays contained a central fixation crosshair and 14 items (spanning 1.64°/side), arranged as in the SFS. In addition to red and blue squares, red triangles were presented (Figure 3). Targets (red squares) were a conjunction of one feature from each of the distracters (i.e., color: red and shape: square). Colors were as in the feature search displays. Targets appeared equiprobably at all locations: Seven displays contained a right target and seven displays contained a left target, each with different distracter arrangements. Fourteen different target-absent displays were presented.
Design
Side (left, right) was entered as the single factor in the within-subjects experimental design. For each side of the display, the dependent measure was the stimulus duration necessary for 75% adjusted accuracy rate (described in detail below).
Procedure
To orient participants to each adaptive search task, they were shown three sample displays (with and without targets). Participants were instructed to fixate the central crosshair at the start of each trial, and to indicate whether or not a target was present on each trial by verbally responding “yes” or “no.” The experimenter entered participants’ responses. Participants were encouraged to report what they saw as accurately as possible and were reminded that the speed of response was not important. If uncertain of their response, they were asked to guess. If participants failed to fixate initially, trials were marked and excluded from the staircase procedure (described below). FS, SFS and CS task order was counterbalanced across participants.
In adaptive procedures, a stimulus parameter is manipulated until a desired performance level is attained. This is accomplished by adjusting the chosen stimulus parameter according to participant responses. The type of response (hit, miss, false alarm or correct rejection) on one observation determines the direction of the adjustment (increment or decrement) for the next observation. Adjustment increments can be made progressively finer to more efficiently approach the desired performance level (Figure 4).
We adopted a yes-no adaptive procedure described by Kaernbach (1990). We manipulated stimulus duration to reach an adjusted accuracy rate of 75%. To accomplish this, the total adjustment to the stimulus duration was a function of the adjustment size (ΔT), response type and a set of ratios (determined based on the desired accuracy rate). Accordingly, stimulus duration was adjusted as follows: hits reduced it by 1*ΔT, misses increased it by 3*ΔT, false alarms increased it by 4*ΔT, and correct rejections resulted in no change. Over the course of this procedure, adjustment size (ΔT) decreased after every two staircase reversals (reversals occur when stimulus parameter adjustments change from increments to decrements, or vice versa; gray points in Figure 4). Appendix I lists the adjustments according to response type and reversals encountered. Initial stimulus durations were 800 ms for FS and SFS, and 2000 ms for CS.
The adjustment size calculations are formally defined as ΔT= [6 − ([r+1] − mod[(r+1),2]) /2] * 16.6, where ΔT=the (time) adjustment size in ms, r=the number of reversals and mod[(r+1),2] is the modulus of (r+1) by 2, i.e., the remainder after dividing (r+1) by 2. By design, initial adjustment size is made progressively finer: ΔT ranged from 83 ms (no reversals yet, early in the procedure) to ~16.6 ms (after eight reversals, late in the procedure). As described above, based on the participant’s response, the adjustment time was then multiplied by the corresponding ratio (hits *-1, misses *+3, false alarms *+4 and correct rejection *0).
Staircases terminated after ten reversals, and a threshold presentation time (TPT) was calculated by averaging the stimulus durations over the final eight reversal points. Two separate TPTs were estimated: one from the adaptive staircase for left target detection, one from the adaptive staircase for right target detection. These two estimations occurred simultaneously because all trial types (left target-present, left target-absent, right target-present, right target-absent) were randomly interleaved and equiprobable until one staircase terminated (Figures 1, 2, 3). Thereafter, displays from the completed staircase continued to be presented, including target-present and target-absent trials (each at a reduced probability of 0.1). These post-convergence data points were not included in the TPT calculation.
Search Scoring
Each adaptive search procedure output a left and a right TPT. To calculate lateralized FS scores (FSSs), SFS scores (SFSSs) and CS scores (CSSs), a difference score was obtained by subtracting left from right TPTs. A score of 0 revealed no lateralized difference. Positive scores resulted when right TPTs were longer and negative scores resulted when left TPTs were longer.
Results
Mean deviations from 0 are reported, as are the distributions of signed scores. Each task was evaluated against 0 with one-sample t-tests. We also tested for correlations between tasks.
A +3 ms FSS mean was obtained (SD=11, range= -18-22). Eight participants scored 0, three required longer right presentations and one required longer left presentations. The mean SFSS was -1 ms (SD=2, range= -8-0). Of eleven participants (one was excluded as an outlier: >3 SD from the mean), 10 scored 0 and one required longer left presentations. The mean CSS was -10 ms (SD=117, range= -180-192). Of eleven participants (one was excluded as an outlier), seven required longer left durations and four required longer right durations.
None of the mean scores were significantly different from 0 (all t-values≤1.02). None were significantly different from one another (all t-values≤1.13), nor were any correlated (all p-values>0.25, Pearson; Figure 5).
Discussion
As predicted, Experiment 1 demonstrated that performance was symmetric on all adaptive measures in healthy participants. Performance of healthy individuals provides a context in which patient performance (Experiments 2 and 3) can be evaluated.
EXPERIMENT 2
In Experiment 2, patients with unilateral brain injuries were tested on both traditional paper-and-pencil Standard Search (SS) tasks (Figure 6) and adaptive FS and CS tasks (Figures 1 and 3). We hypothesized that SS performance would positively correlate with adaptive CS performance, but not necessarily with adaptive FS performance. Furthermore, we expected patients to show contralesional search deficits, and that these would be greater on the CS task than on the FS task.
Methods
Participants
Twenty-three patients (9 women) provided informed consent before participating. Their ages spanned 37-88 years, (mean=66, SD=14). Two were left-handed and all others were right-handed (Table 2). Participants were recruited on the basis of a unilateral lesion (13 right-sided). Those with a history of seizure disorder, dementia or other neurological impairment were excluded from enrollment, as were those who had been substance abusers within the past three years, were legally blind or required an interpreter. Remaining candidate patients were assessed for visual field deficits through confrontation testing4, and those exhibiting homonymous hemianopsia or quadrantanopsia were not enrolled in this study.
Table 2.
Participant | Sex | Age | Handedness | Lesion Side | Experiments | Etiology | Lesion site |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PT1 | M | 84 | R | R | 2 (x2), 3 | Unknown | F; P; T; WM |
PT2 | M | 59 | R | R | 2 (x2), 3 | Hemorrhagic | BG; I; WM |
PT3 | M | 72 | R | L | 2 (x2) | Ischemic | F; P; T |
PT4 | F | 85 | R | L | 2 (x2) | Ischemic | Unknown |
PT5 | F | 61 | R | L | 2 (x2) | Ischemic | MCA territory; F; P |
PT6 | M | 45 | R | L | 2, 3 | Hemorrhagic | P |
PT7 | M | 53 | R | L | 2, 3 | Hemorrhagic | BG |
PT8 | M | 72 | R | R | 2, 3 | Unknown | F; P; WM |
PT9 | M | 77 | R | L | 2, 3 | Unknown | sc; WM |
PT10 | M | 80 | R | R | 2, 3 | Unknown | BG; WM |
PT11 | F | 71 | R | L | 2, 3 | Ischemic | MCA territory |
PT12 | F | 37 | R | L | 2, 3 | Ischemic | BG |
PT13 | M | 48 | R | L | 2, 3 | Ischemic | MCA territory; F; P; T |
PT14 | F | 49 | R | R | 2, 3 | Ischemic | MCA territory; BG; WM |
PT15 | F | 53 | R | R | 2, 3 | Hemorrhagic | BG |
PT16 | M | 75 | R | R | 2 | Unknown | MCA territory; F; P |
PT17 | F | 84 | R | R | 2 | Hemorrhagic | BG |
PT18 | F | 78 | R | R | 2 | Unknown | WM |
PT19 | M | 61 | L | R | 2 | Hemorrhagic | BG |
PT20 | M | 65 | R | R | 2 | Unknown | MCA territory |
PT21 | M | 73 | R | R | 2 | Unknown | T |
PT22 | F | 56 | L | R | 2 | Hemorrhagic | F; th; sc |
PT23 | M | 88 | R | L | 2 | Unknown | Unknown* |
Note. For each patient, age from their first testing session is listed in years. M=Male, F=Female, L=Left, R=Right, F=frontal, P=parietal, T=temporal, WM=white matter, BG=basal ganglia, I=insula, MCA=middle cerebral artery, sc=subcortical (unspecified), th=thalamus. Lesion information was collected from all available neurological reports, radiological reports and/or CT and MRI scans.
Despite a sudden onset of right-sided symptoms, PT23’s acute brain scan was negative (although it did reveal bilateral WM and BG damage); the results of Experiment 2 were qualitatively unchanged by including or excluding this patient’s data.
Standard Search Tasks
Stimuli
Black stimuli were presented on white letter-sized (8.5” × 11”) pages, with half of the items on each side of the page (details below), and one central “demonstration” item.
Line Cancellation
Twenty-nine lines were presented: 14 on each side of the page surrounding one central demonstration item (Figure 6A). Lines measured 35 mm with a 1-mm line-width, and were presented at four possible orientations (vertical, horizontal, or +/- 45°).
Letter Search
Seventy-three letters were presented: 36 on each side of the page, including eight target letters (“A”s), and one central demonstration “A” (Figure 6B). Each letter measured 9 × 6.5 mm.
Symbol Search
Seventy-three symbols (Figure 6C) were presented: 36 on each side of the page, including eight target symbols ( s), and one central demonstration target symbol ( ). Each symbol measured 8-15 mm2 (targets spanned 10 mm2).
Procedure
Participants completed the SS tasks in a fixed order: line cancellation, letter search, and symbol search (Figure 6; adapted from McGlinchey-Berroth et al., 1996), as is customary when administering assessment batteries (e.g., SCAN, BIT). Test materials were presented 60-70 cm in front of participants and were centered with their body midline. The experimenter marked the central demonstration item and then asked participants to find and similarly mark all like items. Specifically, for the line cancellation task, participants were asked to mark each line with a pen stroke, and to circle all target letters (“A”s) and target symbols ( s) in the letter and symbol search tasks, respectively. Patients were to inform the experimenter when they were done (i.e., they were not under time pressure).
Adaptive Search Tasks
In Experiment 2, patients participated in two adaptive search tasks: FS and CS (described in Experiment 1). Because a version had been previously used with neglect patients (Brooks et al., 2005), we used the Feature Search (FS) task in the current experiment. Task order was partially counter-balanced. All TPTs were calculated as in Experiment 1. In the CS task, one participant viewed stimuli presented on a black background and, due to experimenter error, four CS sessions began with start times other than 2000 ms (600, 800, 1200 and 1500 ms). In two of the reported CS sessions, the procedure was terminated after 8 and 9 reversals (instead of the usual 10 reversals) due to technical difficulty. In those sessions, as with other TPT calculations, the final eight reversals were averaged to obtain the TPT.
Search Scoring
For each task, a single score was calculated to reflect the lateralized deficit. As before, performance was quantified as a difference score between left and right TPTs. Depending on each patient’s lesion side, left and right TPTs were classified as either contralesional or ipsilesional. Ipsilesional TPTs were subtracted from contralesional TPTs. Each patient’s ipsilesional performance thereby served as the control for contralesional performance. Therefore, greater contralesional than ipsilesional deficits resulted in positive scores (i.e., hemispatial neglect)5 and zero scores indicated no lateralized impairment6.
Standard Search Score (SSS)
For each SS task (line cancellation, letter and symbol search), the total number of items missed on the left and right sides were classified as contralesional or ipsilesional7. For instance, in a patient with left hemisphere damage (LHD), right-sided omissions were considered contralesional misses, and left-sided omissions were considered ipsilesional misses (see Figure 6B). Ipsilesional misses were then subtracted from contralesional ones. The difference scores from all three SS tasks were summed for a total Standard Search Score (SSS), ranging from -30 to +30.
Adaptive Feature Search Score (FSS) and Conjunction Search Score (CSS)
Adaptive search procedures output left and right TPTs. Again, TPTs were classified as either contralesional or ipsilesional, and a difference score was calculated. Figure 4 shows the staircase progression for a patient with right hemisphere damage (RHD). For this patient, the left TPT (834 ms) is contralesional and the right TPT (390 ms) is ipsilesional, resulting in a CSS of 444 ms. There was no a priori maximum score range in the adaptive tasks.
Results
Twenty-eight sessions were included in the analysis: five participants contributed two sessions, and 18 contributed one session8. Each session met the criterion that the Standard Search tasks and at least one of the adaptive search tasks were performed within the same 30-day period (mean delay between tests=3 days, range 0-26, mode=0). Sessions were run at various delays post-stroke (Table 3), ranging from 17-1250 days (mean delay=507 days, SD=411).
Table 3.
Experiment 2
|
Experiment 3
|
|||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Session 1
|
Session 2
|
Session 3
|
||||||||||||
Patient | Age | SSS | FSS | CSS | SSS delay until adaptive | Age | SSS | FSS | CSS | SSS delay until adaptive | Age | SFS | CSS | delay between CSS sessions |
PT1 | 84 | 104 | 111 | 111 | 7 | 84 | 363 | 378 | 378 | 15 | 85 | 658 | 658 | 547 |
PT2 | 59 | 104 | 111 | 111 | 7 | 59 | 359 | 367 | 367 | 8 | 60 | 702 | 702 | 591 |
PT3 | 72 | 119 | 119 | 119 | 0 | 73 | 512 | 512 | 512 | 0 | ||||
PT4 | 85 | 228 | 228 | 228 | 0 | 85 | 423 | 423 | 423 | 0 | ||||
PT5 | 61 | 260 | 265 | 5 | 62 | 400 | 426 | 426 | 26 | |||||
PT6 | 45 | 38 | 38 | 0 | 46 | 373 | 373 | 335 | ||||||
PT7 | 53 | 360 | 360 | 360 | 0 | 53 | 501 | |||||||
PT8 | 72 | 289 | 289 | 289 | 0 | 73 | 584 | 584 | 295 | |||||
PT9 | 77 | 388 | 391 | 391 | 3 | 78 | 643 | 643 | 252 | |||||
PT10 | 80 | 665 | 665 | 665 | 0 | 81 | 994 | 994 | 329 | |||||
PT11 | 71 | 947 | 947 | 947 | 0 | 72 | 1241 | 1241 | 294 | |||||
PT12 | 37 | 949 | 949 | 949 | 0 | 38 | 1277 | 1277 | 328 | |||||
PT13 | 48 | 1247 | 1247 | 1247 | 0 | 49 | 1495 | 1495 | 248 | |||||
PT14 | 49 | 1250 | *1250 | 1250 | 0 | 50 | *1509 | 1509 | 259 | |||||
PT15 | 53 | 648 | 648 | 0 | 56 | 1648 | 1648 | |||||||
PT16 | 75 | *1198 | 1202 | 1202 | 4 | |||||||||
PT17 | 84 | 17 | 24 | 7 | ||||||||||
PT18 | 78 | 18 | 18 | 0 | ||||||||||
PT19 | 61 | 83 | 83 | 83 | 0 | |||||||||
PT20 | 65 | 132 | 132 | 132 | 0 | |||||||||
PT21 | 73 | 890 | 890 | 890 | 0 | |||||||||
PT22 | 56 | 975 | 975 | 975 | 0 | |||||||||
PT23 | 88 | 1161 | 1161 | 0 |
Note. Blank cells denote that the test was not performed for that patient in that session. Performance that was excluded as an outlier (> 3 SD from the group mean) is indicated with an asterisk (*).
Standard Search Scores (SSSs) were available from 27 sessions (one was excluded as an outlier: >3 SDs from the mean). SSSs averaged to 1.11-item misses (SD=1.34, range= -1.0-4.0), with a mode of 0 (no lateralized deficits in 10 sessions), and deviated from 0 [t(26)=4.31, p<0.001], indicating that contralesional deficits exceeded ipsilesional ones (Figure 7A). An independent-samples t-test failed to reveal any differences between those with LHD or RHD [1.00 vs. 1.21, respectively; t-values<1.0].
Adaptive Feature Search Scores (FSSs) were available from 23 sessions. In four sessions, the test was not conducted and one session was excluded as an outlier (>3 SDs from the mean). FSSs averaged 24 ms longer for contralesional than ipsilesional targets (SD=60, range= -100-158, mode=0; Figure 7B) and showed a trend to differ from 0 [t(22)=1.93, p=0.07]. An independent samples t-test was run comparing those with LHD and RHD, and revealed no difference in FS performance [32 ms vs. 17 ms, respectively; t-value<1.0].
Adaptive Conjunction Search Scores (CSSs) were available from 26 sessions (in two sessions, the test was not conducted). CSSs averaged 366 ms longer for contralesional than ipsilesional targets (SD=630, range= -1026-2088; Figure 7C), and reliably differed from 0 [t(25)=2.96, p<0.01]. An independent samples t-test showed that RHD resulted in greater contralesional CS deficits than LHD [589 ms vs. 106 ms, respectively: t(24)=2.07, p<0.05]. Exploring further, the CSSs for those with LHD was not different from 0 [t-value<1.0], whereas the CSSs for those with RHD was different from 0 [t(13)=3.47, p<0.005].
The 329-ms difference between FSSs and CSSs reached significance [t(20)=2.47, p<0.05]. Those with LHD performed equivalently in the two search tasks [t-value<1.0, N=10], whereas those with RHD performed worse on the CS than FS task [t(10)=2.83, p<0.05].
We conducted bivariate Pearson correlations between Standard, Feature and Conjunction SSs (Figure 8). Consistent with our initial hypothesis, the SSSs correlated with CSSs [r=0.447, p<0.05, N=25], but not with FSSs [r=0.255, p>0.25, N=22]9. Moreover, there was no correlation between Feature and Conjunction SSs [r=0.052, p>0.80, N=21; not shown].
To determine the influence of time since stroke on search performance, we conducted a median split on the basis of delay post-stroke (i.e., the 28 sessions were divided into early and late test groups: conducted within or after 384.5 days post-stroke, respectively). When this factor was entered into independent-samples t-tests, SSSs did not reliably change with delay post-stroke [1.43 early vs. 0.77 late; t(25)=1.29, p>0.20], nor did CSSs [339 ms early vs. 393 ms late; t-value<1.0]. However, FSSs improved with delay post-stroke [49 ms early vs. 1 ms late; t(21)=2.05, p=0.053].
Two additional points bear mention. First, when patient scores are evaluated on the basis of whether they exceed healthy mean performance (by 1, 2 or 3 SDs), the adaptive measures clearly differentiate the two groups (Table 4). Second, using eight reversal points in the TPT calculation may have included “pre-asymptote” staircase points. However, when TPTs were recalculated on the basis of a mean of four reversals or a median of eight reversals, the results from the adaptive tasks were consistent regardless of the measure adopted (Table 5).
Table 4.
Number of patient scores outside healthy means
| |||
---|---|---|---|
Task | +/- 1 SD | +/- 2 SDs | +/- 3 SDs |
FSS | 16 | 11 | 9 |
CSS | 23 | 16 | 14 |
Table 5.
Task | Mean of 8 Reversals | Mean of 4 Reversals | Median of 8 Reversals | Paired-Comparison | Mean of 8 Reversals | Mean of 4 Reversals | Median of 8 Reversals |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
FSS | +24 ms ‡ | +26 ms ‡ | +30 ms ‡ | FSS vs. CSS | * | * | * |
CSS | +366 ms * | +370 ms * | +392 ms * | FSS LHD vs. RHD | ¬ | ¬ | ¬ |
CSS LHD vs. RHD | * | * | * |
Note. Each task is tested in one-sample t-tests vs. 0, and against one another in paired- comparisons.
p<0.05;
<0.10;
t- value<1.0.
Discussion
Experiment 2 illustrates that both standard and adaptive procedures capture lateralized visual search deficits in the patient population tested (albeit only a trend in FS). Furthermore, comparing performance from Experiments 1 and 2 reveals an obvious differentiation between symmetric healthy performance and lateralized patient performance in the same adaptive tasks.
Adaptive procedures also revealed lateralized deficits not detected by SS tasks. Some patients who scored 0 on the SS tasks showed contralesional deficits in the adaptive search tasks (five in FS and seven in CS, of 10 patients scoring 0 on SS). Additionally, the results from the adaptive CS task corroborated an often-reported finding that RHD produces greater spatial attention deficits than LHD (e.g., Behrmann et al., 2004; Stone et al., 1993). Although suggestive, we cannot rule out the possibility that other group differences (e.g., anatomical differences in left vs. right lesion volume, extent or location) might underlie the differences found in CS performance.
Two findings lend further credibility to the use of adaptive measures in neuropsychological investigations. First, even with patient participants, in whom performance can be noisy, the adaptive measures were impressively stable. Whether using the means of four or eight, or medians of eight staircase reversal points to calculate TPTs, statistical analyses were equivalent (Table 5). Second, performance in the adaptive visual search tasks was consistent with well-established fixed-measure findings in the neglect literature: CS tasks were more sensitive to lateralized deficits than were FS tasks (e.g., Behrmann et al., 2004; Eglin et al., 1991; Esterman et al., 2000). Nevertheless, to increase confidence in this conclusion, we ran another experiment matching the stimulus configuration of the feature and conjunction displays. Thus, in Experiment 3, FS was replaced with Scattered Feature Search (SFS).
EXPERIMENT 3
Twelve patients returned for an adaptive testing session in which they performed the SFS task and a repeat CS task. In the SFS task, the stimuli were spatially distributed in the same manner as in the CS task (as described in Experiment 1). The SFS and CS tasks were also matched in target-to-distracter ratio, and target shape, color and size (Figures 2 and 3). Experiment 3 also presented the opportunity to compare patients’ CSSs across two sessions (the original session reported in Experiment 2 and this repeat session collected as part of Experiment 3).
Methods
Participants
Twelve right-handed patients (4 women) participated. Their mean age at the time of testing was 63 years (range=38-85). The mean delay between testing sessions was 385 days (SD=230, range 141-1000; Table 3), during which time none had experienced intervening neurological events. Six had unilateral LHD and six had unilateral RHD (Table 2).
Stimuli and Procedure
Eleven patients were tested on both the SFS and CS tasks, and testing order was counter-balanced. One patient only contributed data on the SFS task. The tasks were administered and scored as in the previous Experiments.
Results
The mean SFSS was +9 ms (SD=42, range= -24-128) and was not reliably different from 0 [t-value<1.00]. One session was excluded as an outlier (>3 SDs from the mean). Six patients scored 0, three had ipsilesional scores and two had contralesional scores.
The mean CSSs deviated contralesionally by 430 ms (SD=551, range= -62-1862), with 9 of 11 patients showing a contralesional deficit. The CSS was reliably different from 0 [t(10)=2.59, p<0.05].
In the 10 patients with both SFS and CS scores, the CSSs differed from the SFSSs by 276 ms [t(9)=2.85, p<0.02], indicating a greater contralesional impairment in CS than in SFS, when stimuli were closely matched across search tasks. As before, this performance dissociation was supported by the lack of a correlation [r= -0.217, p>0.50].
Lastly, we compared CSSs across sessions in the 10 patients with two CSSs. The CSSs from session 1 (mean=583 days post-stroke, range=38-1250) were compared to CSSs from session 2 (mean=969 days post-stroke, range =373-1648). Session 1’s CSS mean was 536 ms (SD=794, range= -200-2088), tending to differ from 0 [t(9)=2.14, p=0.06] and session 2’s CSS mean was 476 ms (SD=558, range= -62-1862), which reliably differed from 0 [t(9)=2.70, p<0.05]. Although it appears that CS performance improved, this was not statistically borne out [t-value<1.00], nor were the scores correlated [one-way intra-class correlation: single measures=0.488; average measures=0.656] (Figure 9). Participants showed different patterns across testing sessions: Half improved and half worsened.
Discussion
As in Experiment 2, contralesional targets defined by conjunctions of two features were reliably more difficult to detect than contralesional targets defined by only one feature. Because display configuration was matched across search tasks in Experiment 3, we can more confidently claim that the present adaptive results converge with previous fixed-measure studies dissociating feature from conjunction search performance in patients with unilateral brain injury.
Although statistically ambiguous, the test-retest sessions revealed comparable performance: The CSSs were contralesionally deviated in both sessions (with some reduction in variability at second testing). There is insufficient data to examine all variables that may have influenced test-retest outcomes (e.g., practice, aging). However, the potential benefits of practice or spontaneous recovery were insufficient to reliably ameliorate the patients’ performance, at least as a group. Adaptive measures may yet prove useful in characterizing recovery of function, when initial testing sessions are carried out during more acute stages (the majority of the patients reported in Experiment 3 were in chronic stages for both testing sessions) or when interventions or treatments are applied between testing sessions.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
In an effort to provide a focused contribution to the growing use of computerized testing in the classification and quantification of common neuropsychological syndromes and their component deficits, the current investigation evaluated adaptive visual search tasks as a tool for detecting lateralized visual deficits in patients with unilateral brain injury. We used feature and conjunction search tasks in our adaptive procedures because they have been well-studied, in both healthy and neuropsychological populations (for reviews, see Robertson, 2004; Robertson & Schendel, 2000). By minimizing response demands, we aimed to isolate visual deficits. We compared the adaptive computerized procedure outcomes with fixed-measure outcomes from a standard hemispatial neglect search test.
The adaptive procedures were successful in detecting lateralized deficits in patient performance. Patient performance also varied by task, as we had predicted: Greater contralesional deficits were present in conjunction search (CS) performance than in either adaptive feature search task (FS or SFS). Adaptive CS performance also distinguished between those with LHD and RHD, corroborating research reporting greater spatial attention deficits after RHD than after LHD. Interestingly, this differentiation in patient performance was found in the adaptive CS task, but not in the Standard Search (SS) task, establishing the sensitivity of this adaptive technique. Notably, lateralized biases were not present in healthy individuals on any measure (Experiment 1).
One point of curiosity was the presence of ipsilesional scores (on standard and adaptive tasks).Various possible explanations for these exist. Ipsilesional scores may reflect noise in the measure and/or in the participants’ performance (e.g., arousal changes, environmental distractions). Or, perhaps a true ipsilesional deficit emerged. Patients may develop compensatory strategies encouraging contralesional hyper-attention (e.g., “look left”), which could induce impaired ipsilesional performance. Alternatively, Butter and colleagues (1988) described a neglect patient, who in the chronic stage demonstrated a strong contralesional visual grasp reflex10: With increased delay post-stroke, the patient showed an increasing inability to inhibit overt contralesional orienting. This visual grasp reflex has been corroborated in a group of chronic patients with lesions to frontal, but not parietal, oculomotor cortex (Machado and Rafal, 2004). Humphreys and colleagues (2002) have discussed another possibly-related effect (anti-extinction), in which a lateralized visual deficit reversed hemifields depending on stimulus duration (over or under 300 ms) in a patient with bilateral lesions. It is uncertain which of these factors, if any, contributed to the ipsilesional scores in our studies.
In summary, we implemented adaptive procedures to measure lateralized visual search deficits present in hemispatial neglect. Here, adaptive outcomes were successful in detecting lateralized visual deficits, and corroborated previous fixed-measure findings showing that contralesional CS was more impaired than FS in patients with unilateral brain injury. Furthermore, RHD was shown to be more detrimental than LHD for conjunction search, but this dissociation was only revealed by the adaptive measure, and not by the standard search measures employed. We therefore propose that adopting adaptive tools to test for component deficits in other neuropsychological syndromes may be more broadly beneficial.
Practically speaking, adaptive tests were quick to administer (5-20 minutes each). Contrary to fixed-measure studies, an exceptionally important asset of the adaptive procedure is its fast and effortless flexibility to adjust to individual patients’ level of performance (between individuals or at varying stages of recovery). Because the procedure is adaptive and scales the chosen experimental parameter (here, stimulus duration) according to the participant’s responses, patients with arousal difficulties, as well as those seemingly unaffected by their stroke, can be tested at their individual level of performance. Given the wide range of individual differences in deficits among patients with visual hemispatial neglect, and in stroke patients more generally, an adaptive procedure is an elegant way to minimize testing duration while obtaining meaningful data.
Acknowledgments
The research in this report was supported by NIH grant MH62331 and a VA Merit Grant Award to LCR. AL received funding from F31 NS047836, AVF and JLB received funding from T32 MH62997, and TMV was funded by a VA Merit Grant Award (98-11-00065).
We would like to acknowledge the generosity of the patients who volunteered their time participating in this research project. We also thank Lauren Friedman, Alice Albrecht, Mitchell Liverant, David Zlotnick, Linh Dang and Joseph Degutis for the time they dedicated to this project. We are also thankful for referrals and recruitment help from Dr. Jack Fahy and Dr. James Muir.
Appendix I
stimulus duration adjustments (ms): - indicates a decrement and + indicates an increment
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Reversals Encountered | Adjustment size or ΔT (ms) | Hit (*-1) | Miss (*+3) | FA (*+4) | CR (*0) |
0 | 83 | -83 | +249 | +332 | 0 |
1 | 83 | -83 | +249 | +332 | 0 |
2 | 66.4 | -66.4 | +199.2 | +265.6 | 0 |
3 | 66.4 | -66.4 | +199.2 | +265.6 | 0 |
4 | 49.8 | -49.8 | +149.4 | +199.2 | 0 |
5 | 49.8 | -49.8 | +149.4 | +199.2 | 0 |
6 | 33.2 | -33.2 | +99.6 | +132.8 | 0 |
7 | 33.2 | -33.2 | +99.6 | +132.8 | 0 |
8 | 16.6 | -16.6 | +49.8 | +66.4 | 0 |
9 | 16.6 | -16.6 | +49.8 | +66.4 | 0 |
Footnotes
Such omissions may be of particular concern when measuring perceptual and attentional processes operating on the order of milliseconds.
A broad literature suggests that feature and conjunction search tasks probe different forms of visual processing: Conjunction search requires the serial deployment of spatial attention, whereas feature search does not. In neurologically-healthy populations, feature search is often characterized as a parallel process (supported by data showing no increase in RT with increasing distracter number). Conjunction search is instead characterized as a serial process (supported by data showing a systematic increase in RT with increasing distracter number, and a ~2:1 target-absent to target-present RT ratio). See Treisman and Gelade (1980) for early experiments and Wolfe (2003) for a more recent review of the relevant literature.
For an alternative approach to analyzing fixed-measure data, while separately estimating perceptual and response biases in hemispatial neglect, see Toraldo et al. (2004).
Visual field confrontation testing required patients to fixate the experimenter’s nose from approximately arm’s length. Patients closed or covered one eye at a time (or, if necessary, a patch was applied over one eye). The patient was then asked to indicate when they saw a finger enter their visual field. The experimenter probed each quadrant of the visual field separately, mid-distance between themselves and the patient by advancing their finger from the outside edge of the field toward the midline on an oblique trajectory. Experimenters used their own nasal and temporal fields to determine visual loss. Patients with hemianopsia or quadrantanopsia fail to see the finger until it nears midline.
Conversely, greater ipsilesional than contralesional deficits resulted in negative scores (see the General Discussion for more on this issue).
Note that those who have a deficit, but omit equal numbers of items bilaterally, are given a score of 0. This has the added benefit of eliminating some general confounds (e.g., arousal), and focusing the measure on lateralized impairments.
All scores were blindly checked by a second experimenter. If a scoring discrepancy was found, a third experimenter determined the final score.
Five participants were tested at two different times post-stroke and for these participants both sessions were entered into the analysis. The first adaptive session was conducted within 3-9 months post-stroke [mean = 167 days, range 111-265 days] and the second adaptive session was conducted at least one year post-stroke [mean = 421 days, range 367-512 days]). When the same analyses were run with a maximum of one session/participant, no differences appeared (in direction of means, differences from 0 or correlations). The effect of lesion side on CSSs and the difference between FSSs and CSSs were less robust statistically, but were present numerically.
The results were comparable (numerically and statistically) whether or not line cancellation performance was included in the SSS.
We thank Stephen Nadeau for drawing our attention to this literature.
References
- Albert ML. A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology. 1973;23:658–64. doi: 10.1212/wnl.23.6.658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett AN, Buxbaum LJ, Costlett HB, Edwards E, Heilman KM, Hillis AE, Milberg WP, Robertson IH. Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for neglect and related disorders: Moving from bench to bedside in stroke patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18:1223–1236. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrmann M, Ebert P, Black SE. Hemispatial neglect and visual search: a large scale analysis. Cortex. 2004;40:247–63. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70120-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks JL, Wong YY, Robertson LC. Crossing the midline: Reducing attentional deficits via interhemispheric interactions. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:572–582. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butter CM, Rapcsak S, Watson RT, Heilman KM. Changes in sensory inattention, directional motor neglect and “release” of the fixation reflex following a unilateral frontal lesion: a case report. Neuropsychologia. 1988;26:533–545. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(88)90110-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro MK, Veramonti T, Farne A, Whyte J, Ladavas E, Frassinetti F, Costlett HB. Hemispatial neglect: Subtypes, neuroanatomy, and disability. Neurology. 2004;62:749–756. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000113730.73031.f4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deouell LY, Sacher Y, Soroker N. Assessment of spatial attention after brain damage with a dynamic reaction time test. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society. 2005;11:697–707. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eglin M, Robertson LC, Knight RT. Visual search performance in the neglect syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1989;1:372–385. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.4.372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eglin M, Robertson LC, Knight RT. Cortical substrates supporting visual search in humans. Cerebral Cortex. 1991;1:262–72. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.3.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eglin M, Robertson LC, Knight RT, Brugger P. Search deficits in neglect patients are dependent on size of the visual scene. Neuropsychology. 1994;4:451–463. [Google Scholar]
- Esterman M, McGlinchey-Berroth R, Milberg WP. Preattentive and attentive visual search in individuals with hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:599–611. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.4.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferber S, Karnath H-O. How to assess spatial neglect--line bisection or cancellation tasks? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2001;23:599–607. doi: 10.1076/jcen.23.5.599.1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halligan PW, Marshall JC. Left neglect for near but not far space in man. Nature. 1991;350:498–500. doi: 10.1038/350498a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hildebrandt H, Schutze C, Ebke M, Brunner-Beeg F, Eling P. Visual search for item- and array-centered locations in patients with left middle cerebral artery stroke. Neurocase. 2005;11:416–426. doi: 10.1080/13554790500263511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humphreys GK, Riddoch MJ, Nys G, Heinke D. Transient binding by time: Neuropsychological evidence from anti-extinction. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2002;19:361–380. doi: 10.1080/02643290143000222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaernbach C. A single-interval adjustment-matrix (SIAM) procedure for unbiased adaptive testing. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1990;88:2645–2655. doi: 10.1121/1.399985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laeng B, Brennen T, Espeseth T. Fast responses to neglected targets in visual search reflect pre-attentive processes: An exploration of response times in visual neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:1622–1636. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00230-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machado L, Rafal RD. Control of fixation and saccades during an anti-saccade task: an investigation in humans with chronic lesions of oculomotor cortex. Experimental Brain Research. 2004;156:55–63. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1765-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlinchey-Berroth R, Bullis DP, Milberg WP, Verfaellie M, Alexander M, D’Esposito M. Assessment of neglect reveals dissociable behavioral but not neuroanatomical subtypes. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society. 2000;2:441–51. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700001521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlovskaya M, Ring H, Groswasser Z, Hochstein S. Searching with unilateral neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:74–756. doi: 10.1162/08989290260138645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riddoch MJ, Humphreys GW. Perception and action systems in unilateral visual neglect. In: Jeannerod M, editor. Neuropsychological and Neurophysiological Aspects of Spatial Neglect. Amsterdam; New Holland: 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Robertson LC. Space, Objects, Minds & Brains. UK: Psychology Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Robertson LC, Schendel KL. Methods and converging evidence in neuropsychology. In: Grafman J, Boller F, editors. Handbook of Neuropsychology. Elsevier; New York: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Stone SP, Halligan PW, Greenwood RJ. The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke. Age and Ageing. 1993;22:46–52. doi: 10.1093/ageing/22.1.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toraldo A, McIntosh RD, Dijkerman HC, Milner AD. A revised method for analysing neglect using the landmark task. Cortex. 2004;40:415–431. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70136-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treisman AM, Gelade G. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology. 1980;12:97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson B, Cockburn J, Halligan P. Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect. Archives of Physical and Medical Rehabilitation. 1987;68:98–102. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe JM. Moving towards solutions to some enduring controversies in visual search. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2003;7:70–76. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00024-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]