Abstract
How do neurons encode and store information for long periods of time? Recurring patterns of activity have been reported in various cortical structures and were suggested to play a role in information processing and memory. To study the potential role of bursts of action potentials in memory mechanisms, we investigated patterns of spontaneous multi-single-unit activity in dissociated rat cortical cultures in vitro. Spontaneous spikes were recorded from networks of approximately 50 000 neurons and glia cultured on a grid of 60 extracellular substrate-embedded electrodes (multi-electrode arrays). These networks expressed spontaneous culture-wide bursting from approximately one week in vitro. During bursts, a large portion of the active electrodes showed elevated levels of firing. Spatiotemporal activity patterns within spontaneous bursts were clustered using a correlation-based clustering algorithm, and the occurrences of these burst clusters were tracked over several hours. This analysis revealed spatiotemporally diverse bursts occurring in well-defined patterns, which remained stable for several hours. Activity evoked by strong local tetanic stimulation resulted in significant changes in the occurrences of spontaneous bursts belonging to different clusters, indicating that the dynamical flow of information in the neuronal network had been altered. The diversity of spatiotemporal structure and long-term stability of spontaneous bursts together with their plastic nature strongly suggests that such network patterns could be used as codes for information transfer and the expression of memories stored in cortical networks.
Introduction
Cortical structures produce recurring spatiotemporal activity patterns, which could potentially be involved in cortical information processing and storage. Beggs and Plenz (2004) enumerated the requirements for recurring activity patterns to serve as substrates of memory. In order to represent information, the activity patterns should (1) exhibit long-term stability over hours, (2) possess millisecond temporal precision and (3) should occur in diverse varieties (Beggs and Plenz 2004). They demonstrated that these properties were satisfied by recurring patterns of local field potentials (neuronal avalanches) observed in cultured cortical slices. Induction of synaptic plasticity is widely believed to be necessary for information storage (Martin et al 2000). Hence, to ascertain the role of repeated activity patterns in memory mechanisms it is important to study their functional plasticity, in addition to the three properties mentioned above.
Several laboratories have demonstrated the presence of recurring spontaneous patterns of action potentials in various brain structures and recommended these patterns as mechanisms for information transmission and storage. In vivo recordings from the rat hippocampus and macaque cortex revealed neural ensemble activity patterns which were repeated in a temporally compressed fashion during slow wave sleep (Nadasdy 2000, Hoffman and McNaughton 2002), possibly to consolidate the information acquired during active behavioral episodes (Wilson and McNaughton 1994, Nadasdy et al 1999). Repeating precisely timed ‘motifs’ of Ca2+ signals and post-synaptic potentials have been reported in spontaneous activity in cortical networks both in vivo and in vitro (Cossart et al 2005, Ikegaya et al 2005), suggesting that these dynamic ensembles are substrates of information storage and flow in cortical networks. Stable spatiotemporal attractors in sequences of culture-wide bursts (‘superbursts’) have been found in dissociated cortical cultures, indicating that dissociated networks can exhibit precise spatiotemporal activity patterns previously thought to require a specific network structure (Wagenaar et al 2006c). Although these studies provide evidence for repeating patterns in the brain and suggest that they are expressions of memory, the effect of external stimulation on these patterns cannot be directly extrapolated from these results.
In order to investigate the plasticity of spontaneous activity in neuronal networks as a substrate for information storage, we studied the effects of electrical stimulation on spontaneous burst patterns exhibited by dissociated cortical networks. Spontaneously active networks in vitro provide a unique model to study and manipulate the network dynamics of intrinsic spontaneous activity, for long periods up to many months (Potter and DeMarse 2001), through electrical recording and stimulation, without the usual in vivo confounds of anesthesia and uncontrolled sensory input. Although spontaneous and evoked ensemble activity patterns are now being studied in awake animals in vivo, these studies are presently limited to a couple of electrodes (Jackson et al 2006) or local field potential recordings (Werk et al 2005). We propose that in order to better understand and harness the capabilities of the network, it is necessary to increase the bandwidth of possible inputs and outputs. Multi-electrode arrays (MEAs, figure 1(A)), allowing for simultaneous stimulation and recording of action potentials from thousands of neurons, present the ideal technology to increase input–output bandwidth. We found that tetanic stimulation changed the distribution of spontaneous burst patterns in dissociated cultures and induced the expression of some new patterns, while some other recurring patterns ceased to exist after the tetanus, indicating a change in the intrinsic tendencies of the network to express certain patterns.
Figure 1.
Multi-electrode recording of spontaneous activity in a dissociated cortical culture. (A) Typical three week old culture on a multi-electrode array (MEA). (B) Bursts were classified into two categories: small and big bursts depending on the threshold number of spikes within a burst. The figure shows a histogram of the number of spikes in each burst from three of the cultures used. The red line indicates the threshold for each culture. Note that the axis scales are different for each culture. (C) Spike raster plot and array-wide spike rate histograms of spontaneous activity for 5 min of spontaneous recording. The inset shows typical examples of bursts classified as ‘big’ and ‘small’ bursts. Recording was taken at 24 days in vitro.
Materials and methods
Cell culture. A suspension of neocortical cells was prepared by enzymatically and mechanically dissociating brains of day-18 rat embryos. This was plated on MEAs at high density to form a planar network 1–3 cells thick. Timed-pregnant Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats (Charles River) were euthanized with isoflurane according to NIH-approved protocols. Embryos were removed and euthanized by chilling and decapitation. The entire neocortex, excluding the hippocampus, was dissected in the Hanks Balanced Salt solution (HBSS, Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA) under sterile conditions. After enzymatic digestion in 2.5 U mL−1 Papain (Roche Scientific, Indianapolis, IN) in Segal's medium (Banker and Goslin 1998) for 20 min, cells were mechanically dissociated by 6–9 passes through a 1 mL pipette tip (Potter and DeMarse 2001), in the Neurobasal medium (Invitrogen) with B27 (Invitrogen), 0.5 mM glutamax (Invitrogen) and 10% horse serum (Hyclone, Logan, UT). Cells were passed through a 40 μm cell strainer (Falcon, Bedford, MA) to remove large debris and then were centrifuged at 150 × g onto 5% bovine serum albumin (BSA) in phosphate buffered saline (PBS). The pellet of cells was resuspended and 50 000 cells were plated in a 20 μL drop of Neurobasal on pre-coated MEAs, at a density of ∼3000 cells per mm2, which results in a thin (15–30 μm) but three-dimensional network 1–3 cell bodies thick, with neurites above, below and between somata (determined by multiphoton microscopy, data not shown). MEAs were pre-coated with polyethylene imine (PEI, Sigma, St Louis, MO) and laminin (Invitrogen) as previously described (Potter and DeMarse 2001). After 30 min of incubation, 1 mL of Neurobasal media was added to each culture dish. After 24 h, the Neurobasal medium was replaced by the feeding medium adapted from Jimbo et al (1998) (Dulbecco's modified Eagle's medium (DMEM, Irvine scientific, Santa Ana, CA), 10% horse serum (Hyclone), 0.5 mM glutamax (Invitrogen) and 1% sodium pyruvate (Sigma)). Cultures were maintained in an incubator at 35 °C, 65% RH, 5% CO2 and 9% O2. The culture medium was exchanged with a fresh feeding medium every seven days. Cultures were maintained in dishes sealed with a gas-permeable Teflon membrane (Potter and DeMarse 2001) to prevent infection and evaporation. The use of Teflon-sealed dishes allows for the maintenance of the incubator at 65% humidity, making it an electronics-friendly environment. All experiments were performed inside the incubator, ensuring long-term stability of our recordings. All recordings were done on two–four week old cultures.
Recording system. Electrical signals were recorded through a square array of 60 titanium nitride electrodes (Multichannel systems, Reutlingen, Germany). Fifty nine of the electrodes were 30 μm in diameter and 200 μm apart. The signals from these were referenced to those of a larger ground electrode. In all analyses, the ground electrode's signal was included, appearing as a channel with no spikes. After 1200× amplification, signals were sampled at 25 kHz using Multichannel Systems data acquisition card (MCCard). Data acquisition, spike detection, artifact suppression (Wagenaar and Potter 2002) and visualization were controlled using our open-source MeaBench software (Potter et al 2006) which allows for detecting spikes as early as 2 ms after stimulation (Wagenaar and Potter 2002). Spikes were detected online by thresholding at 5× RMS noise.
Stimulation system. Stimulus pulses were delivered using our custom-built 60-channel stimulator (Wagenaar and Potter 2004). Biphasic voltage-controlled rectangular pulses (400 μs per phase, positive phase first, 600–800 mV) were used, since these were found to be the most effective at eliciting neural responses (Wagenaar et al 2004).
Experiment protocols
Before the start of each experiment, each of the 59 recordable electrodes was stimulated ten times with a 600 mV, 400 μs, biphasic stimulation pulse at 1 Hz. The electrodes that showed the highest network firing rate in the first 200 ms after stimulation were chosen as candidate electrodes. Two out of these candidate electrodes were chosen arbitrarily for delivering electrical stimulation for the duration of one experiment.
Activity-dependent changes in stimulus-evoked responses as a result of rapid-fire focal stimulation (called tetanus) have been successfully used to demonstrate plastic changes both in in vitro (Bliss and Lomo 1973, Berry et al 1989, Kirkwood and Bear 1994, Castro-Alamancos et al 1995, Bi and Poo 1999) and in vivo (Mulder et al 1997, Werk and Chapman 2003). We used tetanic stimulation to induce functional changes in spontaneously bursting dissociated cultures. Spontaneous activity was recorded for 3 h (period Pre). Tetanization consisted of a train of electrical pulses (as described above) at 20 Hz, applied simultaneously on two electrodes for 15 min. Another recording of spontaneous activity for 3 h followed this treatment (period Post). The tetanization was unusually long to increase the likelihood of inducing plasticity. This experiment was repeated five times on four cultures. One culture was tested twice with different tetanus electrode pairs. The interval between these two experiments was 4 h.
Analysis
Burst detection. Network-wide spontaneous bursts were identified by a burst detector algorithm described elsewhere (Wagenaar et al 2005). Briefly, any 100 ms window with more than five spikes on one electrode was considered to be a part of a burst. A burst consisted of multiple single electrode bursts overlapping in time. The onset of a burst was defined as the first time before the peak of the burst when the array-wide spike rate during a burst was 20% of the peak firing rate during the burst (figure 2(A)). The offset of a burst was defined as the last time after the peak of the burst when the array-wide firing rate was 20% of the peak firing rate during the burst (figure 2(A)).
Classification of bursts depending on the burst size. Bursts with distinct sizes were generally observed. In order to cluster the spatiotemporal patterns of bursts without the bias of size, we classified bursts into two groups, ‘small’ and ‘big’, based on the total number of spikes within each network-wide burst. For each experiment, a histogram of the total number of spikes in every burst in the experiment was generated (figure 1(B)). Two distinct peaks were observed in the histograms in all experiments. The threshold for classifying bursts was determined as the minimum point between the two peaks (figure 1(B)). Bursts having a number of spikes below the threshold were considered ‘small’ and bursts having a number of spikes above the threshold were considered ‘big’.
-
Calculation of the burst activity matrix (BAM). For each burst, a firing rate time histogram was generated for each recording electrode by counting the number of spikes within a 100 ms sliding time bin, moving from the burst onset to the burst offset, with a time step of 10 ms. Histograms of all bursts for a given experiment were made to be of the same length (that of the longest burst) by padding the shorter histograms with zeros at the ends. The burst activity matrix (BAM) for each burst was then constructed by appending (in order of their hardware numbers) the histograms from the 60 recording electrodes, together to form a 1 by 60 N matrix of action potential histograms, where N is the number of time bins in the longest burst (figure 2(A)). The BAM was calculated for big bursts and small bursts separately, and was the same length for all big bursts in a given experiment or all small bursts.
Before settling on 10 ms as the time step to slide the spike histogram window, we tried others. The BAM was calculated with different temporal resolutions by using time steps of 10 ms, 5 ms and 1 ms and clustered using methods described below. No significant difference was found in the number of clusters determined using different temporal resolutions. In order to obtain finer resolution during periods of high firing rates, we also tried variable step sizes that were inversely proportional to the instantaneous firing rates. Again, no significant difference was observed in the number of clusters determined. Therefore, a resolution of 10 ms was selected since this would reduce the dimensions of the BAM and thus lessen the computational time.
Clustering of BAMs. The method used for clustering the BAMs was a correlation-based technique with a global contrast function, from Beggs and Plenz (2004), who used the algorithm to cluster local field potentials (LFPs) recorded from cortical slices. It derives from other similarity-based neural data clustering techniques (Fellous et al 2004). We applied the same clustering algorithm to investigate the presence of the spatiotemporal structure within big bursts and small bursts separately (see supplementary material available at stacks.iop.org/PhysBio/4/181, figure S1). Bursts with similar BAMs were grouped together, and the optimal number of groups was determined as the peak of a similarity contrast function (see supplementary material).
Occurrence and integrated occurrence. The word ‘occurrence’ is used here to represent when bursts with different spatiotemporal patterns, determined by the clustering results, occur (figure 2(B)). An integrated occurrence vector (figure 2(B)) was created by counting the total number of occurrences of each cluster in a 15 min time window with a time step of 30 s. One time step of this vector represents the distribution of burst types during a 15 min period. Changes in the network's tendencies to express certain spatiotemporal activity patterns can be quantified by changes in integrated occurrence over time.
Statistics on integrated occurrence. The integrated occurrences were calculated for both the Pre (before the tetanus) and the Post (after the tetanus) periods for all experiments. The Pre period was split into two periods of equal duration Pre1 and Pre2, separated by a sham interval equal to the duration of the tetanus (15 min). The Post period was split into two periods of equal duration Post1 and Post2, also separated by a 15 min sham period. The centroids of integrated occurrences in periods Pre1, Pre2 and Post1 were calculated (figure 2(C)). The mean Euclidean distance of integrated occurrences in Pre2 to the centroid of integrated occurrences in Pre1 (C, for change) was compared to the mean Euclidean distance of integrated occurrences in Pre1 to their own centroid (D, for drift). The ratio of change to drift, C/D, was used to quantify the change from Pre1 to Pre2 before the tetanus (no change if this ratio is ∼1) (Chao et al 2007). Similarly, C/D between Pre2 and Post1 was used to quantify the change across the tetanus, and C/D between Post1 and Post2 was used to quantify the stability of tetanus-induced changes. The significance of the tetanus-induced change in integrated occurrences was quantified by testing whether C/D across the tetanus (Pre2–Post1) was significantly greater than C/D before the tetanus (Pre1–Pre2), by using Wilcoxon's rank sum test. C/D after the tetanus (Post1–Post2) was also compared to C/D across the tetanus (Pre2–Post1) to evaluate the stability of induced changes after the tetanus.
Shuffling methods. The BAMs of spontaneous bursts were shuffled in various ways to determine whether their structure was more significant than expected by chance. ‘Frame shuffling’ randomly rearranged the sequence of time bins within each BAM. This method changes the temporal order while preserving the spatial information. ‘Electrode shuffling’ randomly rearranged the electrode order within each BAM. This method changes the spatial order while preserving the temporal information. ‘Matched shuffling’ randomly rearranged the activity on each electrode within each time bin within each BAM. This method changes both the temporal and the spatial structures of the data. Unlike ‘frame shuffling’ and ‘electrode shuffling’, ‘matched shuffling’ does not change the firing rate on each electrode. ‘Spike swapping’ repeatedly exchanged the electrode numbers of a randomly selected pair of spikes. This method retains the same spike times and the same distribution of the total number of spikes at each electrode, but removes the biologically induced correlations between spike times (Rolston et al 2007). ‘Spike jittering’ perturbed each spike's time by a Gaussian random noise (with zero mean and 10 ms or 20 ms standard deviation). This method preserved the firing rates at each electrode, but slightly altered the time intervals between spikes. The average correlation values of every pair of BAM clusters were calculated for each shuffled dataset, and compared with results from the original non-shuffled dataset (Rolston et al 2007).
Quantifying tetanus-induced changes in the information content of cultured networks. The amount of new information created by the tetanus was defined as the number of new clusters that occurred in the Post period but never in the Pre period. Note that this measure does not quantify any information that might be represented in the detailed spatiotemporal structure carried within individual bursts, nor the specific timing of the occurrences of bursts. The amount of information eliminated by the tetanus was defined as the number of clusters that occurred in the Pre period but did not occur in the Post period. We define the changes in the information content of a network due to tetanic stimulation as the formation and deletion of the burst patterns, i.e. alterations in the network's repertoire of spontaneous activity patterns. The scale of the tetanus-induced burst pattern formation and/or deletion, ΔI, was defined as the percentage change of the number of newly formed and/or disappeared clusters after the tetanus. Only clusters that occurred at least five times in the 3 h Pre and Post periods were included in the analysis. We define CPre = {x: x ∈ cluster indices appearing in Pre, occurrences of x > 5} and CPost = {y: y ∈ cluster indices appearing in Post, occurrences of y > 5}; then
Figure 2.
Pictorial description of the analysis of spatiotemporal patterns within spontaneous bursts and statistics on the change of distribution of burst patterns across the tetanus. (A) Raster plot and corresponding histogram of spontaneous spiking activity on 60 electrodes. Small and big bursts were detected, the onset and offset times of bursting activity were calculated and the difference between the offset and onset times was determined as the length of the burst. The burst activity matrix (BAM) for each burst was generated by counting the number of spikes within the length of the burst on each of the 60 electrodes using a 100 ms moving time bin (time step = 10 ms). The right panel shows frames of a BAM for different time bins for 60 electrodes (arranged in the original coordinates of the multi-electrode array). The size of the circle represents the number of spikes on that particular electrode for a particular time bin with the colored circle representing the value at electrode k, and the firing rate time histogram for electrode k is shown below. The BAM for each burst was then constructed by appending the histograms from the 60 recording electrodes, together to form a 1 by 60 N matrix, where N is the number of time bins in the longest burst. (B) A two-dimensional cross-section of a BAM plotted in 60 by N dimensional space is shown. BAMs of all bursts were compared against each other and clustered using a paired clustering algorithm (dendrogram, see supplementary materials stacks.iop.org/PhysBio/4/181). Occurrence represents when a burst occurs within a specific BAM cluster (figures 3 and 4). Integrated occurrence was defined as the total number of occurrences of bursts in a 15 min time window moved by a time step of 30 s. Integrated occurrences were calculated across three periods of the same length equally spaced in time, Pre1, Pre2, Post1, and Post2. Periods Pre1 and Pre2 were before the tetanus stimulation and periods Post1 and Post2 were after the tetanus. (C) A two-dimensional cross-section of integrated occurrences of all BAM clusters (S by R dimensional matrix, S = number of BAM clusters, R = number of 15 min moving time windows) is shown. The color bar represents the time elapsed during the experiment and each point represents the integrated occurrence of the BAM clusters. The centroids of integrated occurrences in periods Pre1, Pre2 and Post1 were calculated. The change across periods was calculated as the Euclidean distance of each point in a period from the centroid of the previous period (d12, d23 and d34 in the figure) normalized by the Euclidean distance of each point in the previous period from its centroid (d11, d22 and d33 in the figure). This calculation yielded three quantities: C/D before the tetanus (Pre1–Pre2), C/D across the tetanus (Pre2–Post1) and C/D after the tetanus (Post1–Post2). The difference between two C/Ds from consecutive periods was tested (Wilcoxon's rank sum test) to evaluate changes in the burst pattern across periods.
where ∩ and ∪ represent the intersection and union of two sets, respectively, and | · | represents the number of elements in the set. Furthermore, the scale of the burst patterns that were retained after the tetanus, , which denotes the amount of information content preserved across the tetanus, is defined as the complement of ΔI:
Burst initiation site. Bursts originate from various locations in the network, termed as ‘burst initiation sites’, and then propagate to the entire network (Maeda et al 1998, Eytan and Marom 2006). In order to visualize the changes in network dynamics, in addition to the BAM, we used burst initiation properties as a measure to quantify the tetanus-induced change. We define the burst initiation site as the first electrode that spiked next after the burst onset (burst onset defined earlier, figure 2(A)). For each 15 min time window (time step = 30 s) during an experiment, the burst initiation probability (BIP) was calculated for every electrode. The BIP vector was computed as the number of times that bursts were initiated at each electrode, normalized by the total number of bursts in the time window. This results in a 1 by 60 BIP vector for every time window (summation of these 60 values equals 1). The same statistic (as used to evaluate the integrated occurrence of the BAM) was applied on BIP vectors to quantify the changes before and across the tetanus.
Results
General properties of spontaneous network-wide bursts in dissociated cultures
Dissociated cortical networks cultured on multi-electrode arrays (figure 1(A)) (Gross et al 1977, Pine 1980) express spontaneous network-wide bursting, persisting for the lifetime of the culture (Murphy et al 1992, Gross et al 1993, Canepari et al 1997, Voigt et al 1997, Gross and Kowalski 1999, Corner et al 2002, van Pelt et al 2005, Wagenaar et al 2006a). Spontaneous bursts occur in different sizes and patterns (Wagenaar et al 2006a) and can be classified in various ways. A spike raster plot for 5 min of spontaneous recording is shown in figure 1(C). Culture-wide bursts were detected as periods of an increased firing rate across most of the active electrodes (figure 1(C)). Since there was a clear bimodal distribution in the burst size for all tested cultures (figure 1(B)), bursts were classified into two categories, big and small bursts (figure 1(C)), with the threshold set at the minimum in the burst size histogram (figure 1(B)). A burst activity matrix (see Materials and Methods) was generated for each burst by counting the number of spikes on each electrode within the length of the burst using a 100 ms sliding time window (time step = 10 ms) (figure 2(A)).
Spontaneous bursts express different spatiotemporal structures
To explore the presence of temporal and spatial structures within spontaneous bursts, the correlation between BAMs (see Materials and Methods) of all bursts in tetanus experiments was determined (see Materials and Methods) and hierarchical clustering algorithms were used to cluster the BAMs of bursts having the least distance in correlation space into groups (see supplementary material). To visualize these clusters the correlation matrix was sorted (see figure S1, supplementary materials stacks.iop.org/PhysBio/4/181), resulting in regions of high correlation values along the diagonal showing spontaneous bursts that exhibit a high similarity in their spatiotemporal structure (figure 3(A)).
Figure 3.
Shuffled data do not show a significant structure in the BAM of spontaneous bursts. (A) Sorted correlation matrix of actual unshuffled data for a representative tetanus experiment. There were high correlations indicating a high similarity among the BAM of spontaneous bursts. (B) Average correlation values between the BAM clusters from N = 5 tetanus experiments after different shuffling methods: ‘electrode shuffle’, ‘temporal shuffle’, ‘matched shuffle’, ‘spike swapping’ and ‘spike jittering’ with 10 and 20 ms standard deviations. The average correlation of the original data showed significantly higher correlation values compared to shuffled data (the asterisk indicates p < 0.001, Wilcoxon's rank sum test for equal medians).
The data were shuffled in various ways to verify that the structure in spontaneous bursts was not an artifact of the clustering algorithm (see Materials and Methods). On shuffling the data, both temporally and spatially (Beggs and Plenz 2004), the correlation values in the shuffled data were significantly lower than those in the original dataset (figure 3(B), p < 0.001). Thus, the lack of high correlation values in the shuffled data shows that the original data consisted of various spatiotemporal activity patterns that have a much more specific structure than would be expected by chance. In five experiments on four cultures, we identified an average of 60 ± 8 (mean ± SEM) clusters of small bursts and 158 ± 23 clusters of big bursts per culture. There were an average of 4128 ± 553 spontaneous bursts per experiment (five experiments on four cultures).
Spontaneous bursts as indicators of tetanus-induced network plasticity
Bursts at the single cell and population level have been suggested as a reliable neural code since they can facilitate neurotransmitter release at a synapse more reliably than a single spike could (Lisman 1997). Since we found a specific structure in spontaneous bursts (figure 3), we sought to use changes in spatiotemporal patterns of spontaneous culture-wide bursts as a measure for detecting stimulus-induced functional plasticity in the network.
Bursts in 3 h periods before and after the tetanus (see Materials and Methods: Experiment Protocols) were classified into big and small bursts depending on their size (figure 1(B)) and clustered into separate types using a correlation-based clustering algorithm. This analysis showed that spontaneous bursts occurred in clusters having different spatiotemporal patterns that remained stable during the 3 h period before the tetanus (figure 4). Stability of occurrence of BAM clusters for spontaneous bursts, in the absence of any stimulation, persisted for at least 9 h (the longest experiment, not shown). The structure within temporally spaced BAM clusters of spontaneous small and big bursts (arrowheads in figure 4) was conserved over time for all five experiments (one representative experiment shown in figure 5). Thus, precisely timed dynamic patterns within spontaneous bursts recurred with high fidelity and remained stable over hours. Wagenaar et al (2006b) documented the presence of a recurring spatiotemporal structure in burst sequences (‘superbursts’) in dissociated cortical cultures, which remained stable for hours. The present study was not restricted to ‘superbursts’. We found a highly conserved structure for all bursts occurring during the recording period. The stability of burst patterns represents the tendency of the culture to express certain types of bursts spontaneously, and we sought to determine whether a strong (tetanic) stimulus, representing repetitive salient sensory input, would change this tendency.
Figure 4.
Occurrence of spontaneous bursts was stable before the tetanus and changed across the tetanus. (A) Each stroke represents one occurrence of a BAM cluster of small spontaneous bursts. (B) Each stroke represents one occurrence of a BAM cluster of big spontaneous bursts. This is a representative example from one experiment. The gray bar indicates the period of tetanization. The arrowheads indicate the timing of the different BAM clusters shown in figure 5. (C) In five experiments, the average change in integrated occurrences across the tetanus (C/D between Pre2 and Pos t1) was significantly greater than the average drift before the tetanus (C/D between Pre1 and Pre2) (p < 0.01). C/D = 1 indicates that there was no change in the occurrence between periods. C/D before the tetanus (Pre1–Pre2) was close to 1, indicating that there was very little drift in the Pre period before the tetanus and the integrated occurrence of the BAM of spontaneous bursts was stable for this period. **indicates p < 0.01 and * indicates p < 0.05 (Wilcoxon's rank sum test for equal medians).
Figure 5.
Examples of clusters for the BAM of spontaneous bursts for the experiment of figures 4(A) and (B). The sampled times are marked by arrows in figure 4. A given cluster shows similarity in the spatiotemporal structure over hours, but there was an easily visible difference between the patterns of any two clusters. (A) Frames from two representative BAM clusters for small bursts are shown, in the original coordinates of the multi-electrode array, at 20 ms intervals. (B) Frames from two representative BAM clusters for big bursts are shown, in the original coordinates of the multi-electrode array. Frames are shown at 100 ms intervals since the big bursts were much longer than the small bursts shown in (A). The sizes of the circles represent the number of spikes in that particular electrode for a particular time bin.
Any change in the distribution of burst clusters at different intervals was quantified by the change in the integrated occurrence of BAM clusters (figure 2(C)). The stability of integrated occurrence of BAMs of spontaneous bursts was determined on two 9 h long spontaneous recordings without any electrical stimulation. The average change in the integrated occurrence of BAMs across sequential 3 h periods was not significant (p = 0.17, Wilcoxon's rank sum test).
To quantify whether the change in the distribution of burst patterns across the tetanus was more than their drift in the periods of no stimulation, the change in the integrated occurrence of BAM clusters across the tetanus was compared to their change across a ‘sham’ period of equal duration to the tetanus (see Materials and Methods, Statistics on Occurrence). The burst patterns changed significantly across the tetanus but not within the spontaneous period before it (figure 4(C)). The average change across the ‘sham’ period (C/D between Pre1 and Pre2 periods) for five tetanus experiments was not significantly greater than 1 (p = 0.063). C/D = 1 indicates that the distribution of burst clusters did not change across the periods (see Materials and Methods). In addition, the average change in the distribution of burst patterns across the tetanus (C/D between Pre2 and Post1 periods) for five experiments was significantly greater than C/D before the tetanus (p < 0.01, Wilcoxon's rank sum test) (figure 4(C)). This indicates that the tetanus induced significant redistribution in the patterns of bursts expressed by the culture. Following the tetanus, though the cluster distributions varied significantly in a 30 min period immediately after the tetanus, the change across the periods Post1 and Post2 was lower than the change across the tetanus (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon's rank sum test). This is consistent with the findings in a simulated network of 1000 integrate-and-fire neurons, where strong tetanic stimulation resulted in the network settling down into a new state after a short transition period, during which the network drifted to its new state (Chao et al 2005).
Tetanus-induced changes in the information content of cultured networks
As shown above, spontaneous burst patterns possessed high temporal fidelity and a specific spatiotemporal structure, which could be modified by tetanic stimulation (figures 4 and 5). Thus, they satisfy many of the requirements for a memory substrate (Martin et al 2000, Martin and Morris 2002, Beggs and Plenz 2004) and can be viewed as containing information. Although not done here, we previously used specific burst patterns to determine the behaviors of simulated animals and robots (DeMarse et al 2001, Bakkum et al 2004), imbuing them with specific meanings. To quantify the changes in the information content of the network as indicated by the changes in the occurrences of bursts after the tetanus, we measured the information modified or preserved across the tetanus (see Materials and Methods). This simple quantification allows us to estimate the extent of plastic changes caused by the external stimulus. The tetanus caused a change in network dynamics resulting in the creation or loss of an average of 70.91 ± 15.74% BAM clusters of big bursts (mean ± SEM) and 55.33 ± 8.64% BAM clusters of small bursts for N = 5 tetanus experiments. We take this as a measure of the amount of information in burst tendencies that was modified by the tetanus. Thus, the tetanus induced the creation of new patterns while some other existing patterns were annihilated (figure 4). In the absence of stimulation, an average of 2.9% BAM clusters of big bursts were modified across pre-tetanus periods Pre1 and Pre2 and 4.5% BAM clusters of big bursts were modified across the periods Post1 and Post2 after the tetanus, indicating that the burst patterns were robust in the absence of external perturbations.
We also quantified the amount of information retained across the tetanus as the number of BAM clusters that occurred in the Post period after the tetanus given that they had occurred in the Pre period before the tetanus. An average of 29.1 ± 15.74% BAM clusters of big bursts and 44.67 ± 8.63% BAM clusters of small bursts were retained across the tetanus in five tetanus experiments. This quantity represents the fraction of clusters that were preserved across the tetanus, but does not indicate whether the temporal distribution (occurrence) of the ‘preserved clusters’ was also retained across the tetanus. In order to quantify the stability of the ‘preserved clusters’ across the tetanus, the integrated occurrences of these clusters were compared for the Pre and Post periods. There was no significant change in the integrated occurrence of the ‘preserved’ clusters across the tetanus (p = 0.55, Wilcoxon's rank sum test).
Tetanic stimulation caused changes in the locus of burst initiation
In addition to the BAM, we quantified functional changes in the network by tracking tetanus-induced changes in the burst initiation site (see Materials and Methods). Typically, spontaneous bursts originate from various locations in the networks, termed burst initiation sites, and spread to the entire network. Eytan and Marom (2006) identified early-to-fire neurons during the start of the burst, and these initiation areas were found to be characterized by high neuronal density and recurrent excitatory and inhibitory connections (Feinerman et al 2007). Hence, changes in the burst initiation site could imply a change in the network topology.
We quantified the probability that neurons around any particular electrode tend to fire at burst onsets, and found that this BIP (see Materials and Methods) was stable if no external stimulation was delivered, but was changed significantly by tetanic stimulation (figure 6(B), p < 0.05, Wilcoxon's rank sum test). Hence, the tetanus changed not only the spatiotemporal dynamics of burst patterns (figure 4(C)), but also the locus of the neurons involved in triggering bursts, suggesting that a long, repetitive artificial sensory input substantially altered the functional connectivity in cultured cortical networks.
Figure 6.
Burst initiation sites were significantly changed by a strong tetanus. (A) The probability (burst initiation probability, BIP) of a recorded neuron having initiated a burst is shown by the different sizes of the circles for a representative experiment. Although this probability did not change much in the spontaneous recording periods before the tetanus (Pre1 and Pre2), it changed significantly across the tetanus. (B) The average change in the probability of burst initiation for all experiments changed significantly after (p < 0.05) but not before the tetanus (N = 5 experiments). The asterisk indicates p < 0.05, Wilcoxon's rank sum test. The error bars represent SEM.
Discussion
Synchronized or correlated activity within neuronal networks has been proposed as a likely mechanism for encoding information (for a review, see Rieke (1997)). Building on this idea, we demonstrated that spontaneous bursts in vitro occurred in diverse but recurring patterns that were stable for hours in the absence of electrical stimulation, and could be altered significantly by a strong tetanus applied simultaneously on two electrodes (figure 4, p < 0.01). Furthermore, the loci of burst initiation were changed by the tetanus (figure 6), suggesting that tetanic stimulation caused alterations in the network connectivity. Such changes could entail morphological changes, which are necessary for long-term plastic changes, like long-term potentiation (Matsuzaki 2007). Simultaneous cellular level imaging with extracellular recording might provide further insight into the anatomical changes associated with functional plasticity of spontaneous burst patterns (Potter et al 2006). In vitro systems are especially useful for such detailed studies since they provide enhanced accessibility to network properties, across time scales of milliseconds to months, compared to animal models. Other multi-electrode array studies on tetanus-induced plasticity in dissociated cultures used responses to single electrode stimulation to quantify functional changes (Jimbo et al 1998, Tateno and Jimbo 1999), but we found that these responses vary with time even in the absence of tetanic stimulation (see figure S2, supplementary materials stacks.iop.org/PhysBio/4/181). In contrast, spontaneous bursts occurred in spatially diverse patterns that were comparatively stable until perturbed by strong external stimulation, like a tetanus.
Bursts as information carriers in cortical networks
Single neuron bursts have been proposed to be reliable coding elements (Lisman 1997, Izhikevich et al 2003). Recordings from area MT of macaque monkey have shown that the burst rate was on average a more sensitive measure of the visual stimulus direction than the total number of spikes (Bair et al 1994). Models of bursting in pyramidal neurons have shown that the spike count of a burst encodes the rate of increase of inputs (Kepecs et al 2002). These studies propose that bursts are a special neural code and carry information about input characteristics (Kepecs and Lisman 2004).
Spontaneous bursts observed in cultured dissociated networks could be expressions of stored information; we have shown them to be stable over hours (figure 4), diverse enough to encode different memories (figure 5) and susceptible to stimulus-induced functional plasticity (figure 4). These properties exhibited by spatiotemporal patterns of spontaneous bursting satisfy the requirements of a memory substrate (Beggs and Plenz 2004). We analyzed the occurrences of spontaneous bursts in various clusters across the tetanus and showed that while some clusters were preserved across the tetanus others were modified by the tetanus. If spontaneous bursts with different spatiotemporal structures represent different memories, this study suggests that electrical stimulation can drive the expression of new patterns, while retaining some of the old patterns. Since spatiotemporally diverse bursts carry information about the network state, we suggest that a burst with a specific spatiotemporal structure could be used by the brain as an initiator of a specific movement if connected to an efferent system. By artificially re-embodying cultured networks in robotic or simulated animals, we can attach ensemble activity to real-world events (DeMarse et al 2001, Potter et al 2006) and quantify the information-carrying capacity of network activity patterns.
For example, we can classify an incoming burst as a new cluster or as one of the existing clusters by the similarity measure, and use integrated occurrence to generate a specific motor output in a hybrot (hybrid neural-robotic system) in real time. The sensory inputs from the hybrot's artificial body can be encoded as different stimuli fed back to the network to change the spatiotemporal patterns of bursts (Potter et al 2006). The plasticity-inducing artificial sensory input to our cultures in the present experiments was very simple, i.e. repetitive tetani delivered to two electrodes. More complex stimuli could potentially induce specific, predictable changes in burst tendencies. We expect this to be a fruitful approach for studying memory encoding mechanisms at the network level.
Bursts as dynamic attractors in cortical networks
Spontaneous bursts with different but stable BAM patterns could represent different intrinsic spatiotemporal tendencies of the network. Our modeling study of network activity in a network of 1000 integrate-and-fire neurons revealed attractors in the network intrinsic synaptic state, represented by network synaptic weights, and a corresponding set of stable spontaneous bursts (Chao et al 2005). If the simulated network was driven from one attractor to another (by a tetanus), the spontaneous burst pattern was also changed. In the present experiments using living networks, the change of occurrence of spontaneous bursts with different BAMs after tetanus suggests that the network was driven from one attractor in the synaptic weight space to a different one by the tetanus. Precisely timed sequences of a single-unit activity were reactivated with high temporal precision in cortical networks both in vivo and in vitro, and these repeated sequences have been suggested to represent attractors in the cortex (Ikegaya et al 2004, Rolston et al 2007).
Here, BAMs of spontaneous bursts consisting of specific sequences of network activity patterns were found to recur over hours. The robust spatiotemporal activity patterns within bursts could represent attractors in dissociated cortical cultures. The steady occurrence of a set of attractors over hours could correspond to a stable underlying intrinsic state of the network. This intrinsic state of the network could be modified by tetanic stimulation (or sensory input in vivo) resulting in a new set of attractors, which represents new information in the network.
Comparison with previous work
Spontaneous burst patterns were stable for hours in the absence of external stimulation (maximum recording period was 9 h). This finding is contrary to Sasaki et al (2007) who demonstrated that calcium flux patterns in cultured hippocampal slices were ‘metastable’ over a maximum recording period of 30 min; a pattern was rarely repeated. In addition, weak electrical stimulation did not perturb the network state more than changes that occurred spontaneously (Sasaki et al 2007). Our results provide evidence for the opposite, perhaps due to differences in our preparations or the stimuli delivered. We have the capability of non-invasively recording single action potentials for much longer (up to months, Potter and DeMarse 2001), allowing us to observe the long-term stability of the activity patterns. Our stronger (longer duration) stimulation caused drastic state changes in the network dynamics (figure 4) showing that network attractor-like states, represented by spontaneously repeating patterns, are robust and might need a sufficiently strong stimulation to push the network to a different state (attractor).
The finding that cortical networks express spatiotemporally diverse stable patterns was not unexpected. Other studies on spontaneous activity in cortical slices without tetanic stimulation have shown that spatiotemporal patterns of activity, termed neuronal avalanches, were stable for over 10 h (Beggs and Plenz 2004). These stable recurring patterns in local field potentials (LFPs) lasted tens of milliseconds. The spatiotemporal patterns of bursts in dissociated cortical cultures described here typically last much longer (100 ms–1 s) than avalanches in slices do. Unlike neuronal avalanches, the size distributions of spontaneous bursts (defined here as the number of spikes in a specified time bin) were bimodal and did not follow a power law (supplementary materials, figure S3 stacks.iop.org/PhysBio/4/181). Similar bimodal distribution for burst size (number of electrodes) and burst duration in dissociated cortical cultures has been reported elsewhere (Eytan and Marom 2006). In dissociated cortical cultures, spatiotemporally diverse burst activity motifs have been thought to result from varied connectivity architecture within the network (Volman et al 2005). These highly repeatable ensemble patterns could arise from strongly connected neuronal subgroups with different group properties (Izhikevich et al 2004). Segev et al (2004) have described correlated spontaneous burst patterns in dissociated cortical networks (Segev et al 2004), but the study did not attempt to investigate the effects of external electrical stimulation on these patterns. Local chemical stimulation with picrotoxin (GABA antagonist) resulted in the formation of new patterns which persisted for days (Baruchi and Ben-Jacob 2007). We extend the previous findings by demonstrating that the distribution of spontaneous burst clusters changed significantly after electrical stimulation, resulting in the creation of new patterns and elimination of some existing ones.
Responses to single electrode (probe) stimulation pulses have been used to demonstrate functional plasticity in dissociated cultures (Jimbo et al 1998, 1999, Tateno and Jimbo 1999, Shahaf and Marom 2001, Ruaro et al 2005). However, we found significant variability in the responses to single electrode stimulation, which made it difficult to distinguish between tetanus-induced changes and intrinsic drift in the stimulus-evoked responses (figure S2). Other experiments on dissociated cultures, using several different protocols to induce network-level functional plasticity, failed to demonstrate significant changes unless spontaneous bursting was suppressed by elevated levels of extracellular Mg2+ (Wagenaar et al 2006b). Considering that cortical networks seem resistant to change, we used an unusually long tetanic train in the present study. The plating densities in our cultures were an order of magnitude higher than those in these studies, i.e. closer to in vivo densities if extended in the thickness dimension (Beaulieu 1993). Cultures with higher plating densities show faster development of network activity and more synchronized bursting than cultures with lower plating densities (Wagenaar et al 2006a). Possibly due to higher levels of ongoing activity in our densely plated cultures, we observed that stimulus-evoked responses continuously fluctuate with time making it more difficult to detect functional plasticity in evoked responses than in spontaneous bursts (Madhavan et al 2006). In simulated networks with high levels of ongoing activity, tetanus-induced changes did not last and the network returned to its pre-tetanus state (Chao et al 2005).
Comparison with in vivo studies
Spontaneous and evoked ensemble activity patterns are now being studied in vivo (Bragin et al 2000, Nicolelis 2003). Although acute extracellular recordings are possible in vivo, realizing chronic systems that can record single and multi-unit activity over periods of months to years has been limited by reactive tissue encapsulation of the implant (Turner et al 1999, Szarowski et al 2003) and difficulties in determining the precise location for placement of the electrodes (Brecht et al 2004). They have also been limited by not being able to stimulate with complex patterns on many of an array's electrodes, including recording electrodes. We made circuitry to surmount this problem in vitro (Wagenaar and Potter 2004), and are now making smaller versions for use in vivo. Densely plated cultures on MEAs allow for non-invasive recording and stimulation of neuronal networks with each electrode recording and influencing the activity of multiple neurons in the electrode's vicinity. Recently, some studies have demonstrated functional plasticity in vivo using electrical stimulation (Werk et al 2005, Jackson et al 2006), but these studies involved only a couple of electrodes or local field potential recordings. In contrast, the present study of the spatiotemporally diverse ensemble activity, using recordings of many-single-unit activity coupled with simultaneous stimulation on multiple electrodes over extended periods, can help understand the network-level rules of information storage in cortical networks, without the complexities of recording in vivo.
Conclusions and outlook
By following the spontaneous activity patterns in dissociated cortical cultures on MEAs, we found that network-level spatiotemporally diverse patterns of bursting activity are robust in the absence of external perturbations (electrical stimulation), suggesting that such stable patterns could be readouts of memories stored in cortical networks. Application of high frequency tetanic stimulation resulted in the manipulation of the spatiotemporal structure of recurring burst patterns; while some existing patterns were sustained, other new patterns emerged because of the stimulus. We propose that these diverse, recurring patterns of activity involving large ensembles of neurons could potentially carry information in neural circuits, by controlling behaviors.
Brains are essentially information-processing devices. Since electrical stimulation can alter enduring patterns in neuronal networks, it would be interesting to program the neuronal network to express certain patterns based on the structure of the input stimulation. While such studies are still in their infancy, they could potentially reveal crucial parameters for use in the design of closed-loop embodied systems such as hybrots and neural prostheses that include both sensory and motor components. Easy manipulation and enhanced accessibility of dissociated cortical networks make MEA cultures the ideal platform to explore the effects of varied input patterns on the structure of output activity patterns. That said, the analyses used here are general measures of spatiotemporal activity patterns, not restricted to in vitro studies and could be applied to the analysis of multi-unit activity patterns in vivo obtained optically or electrically.
Supplementary Material
S This article has associated online supplementary data files
Figure S1: A. An example of a dendrogram showing different levels of the grouping process. All the individual BAMs are shown at the bottom in sorted order (BAMs that similar to each other were arranged together). At each level, the two most similar BAMs were grouped together, connected by a line. B. The corresponding sorted correlation matrix of BAMs. “Blocks” with high correlation values along the diagonal represent groups of bursts with similar BAMs. Data presented are from one representative Tetanus experiment. The colorbar indicates the correlation values.
Figure S2: Occurrence of cluster of BAM for evoked bursts changed before and across the tetanus. A. Each stroke represents one Occurrence of a BAM cluster of small evoked bursts. B. Each stroke represents one Occurrence of a BAM cluster of big evoked bursts. This is a representative example of a Probe experiment. The grey bar indicates the period of tetanization. The arrowheads indicate the timing of the each individual BAM cluster shown in C and D. C. Clusters of BAMs of small evoked bursts in the original coordinates of the multi-electrode array. The BAMs of each small evoked burst clusters were different from each other. Size of the circles represents the number of spikes in that particular electrode for a particular time bin. BAMs were collected every 20ms. D. Clusters of BAMs of big evoked bursts in the original coordinates of the multi-electrode array. The BAM of each of the big evoked burst cluster were different from each other. Size of the circles represents the number of spikes on that particular electrode for a particular time bin. BAMs were collected every 100ms since the big bursts were much longer the small bursts shown in part C.
Figure S3: Size distributions of spontaneous bursts were bimodal and did not follow the power law. Size distribution of spontaneous bursts for N=4 Tetanus experiments are plotted on log-log coordinates. Beggs and Plenz (2003, 2004) investigated recurring spontaneous patterns in local field potentials (LFPs) in acute slices of rat cortex (neuronal avalanches) and showed that the size distributions of avalanches obey the power law. The number of spikes per burst represents size of bursts here, which is similar to avalanche amplitude. For each experiment, a histogram of the number of spikes in every burst (time bin=100ms) in the experiment was plotted on a log-log scale.
Acknowledgments
This work was partially supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH)–National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke grant NS38628, by NIH–NIBIB grant EB00786, and by the Whitaker Foundation, the Georgia Research Alliance and the National Science Foundation Center for Behavioral Neuroscience and Faculty of the Future grant from the Schlumberger Foundation. The authors acknowledge Dr Daniel Wagenaar for designing the data-acquisition software, stimulator and burst-detection algorithms used in this study, John Rolston for help with the shuffling analysis, Dr Kacy Cullen for dissecting and providing tissue used in this study and Douglas Swehla for dissection and other technical assistance in cell culture.
Abbreviation list
- BAM
Burst activity matrix
- BIP
Burst initiation probability
- MEA
Multi-electrode array
- Pre
Periods before the tetanus
- Post
Periods post-tetanus
- C/D
Ratio of network functional change to drift
Glossary
- Tetanus
high frequency (e.g., 20 Hz) electrical stimulation used to induce functional plasticity
- Spike
a recording of a neuron's action potential
- Bursts
short periods (tens to hundreds of milliseconds) of intense spiking occurring simultaneously on a large fraction of active electrodes
- Hybrot
a hybrid of living neurons and robotic components
References
- Bair W, Koch C, Newsome W, Britten K. Power spectrum analysis of bursting cells in area MT in the behaving monkey. J Neurosci. 1994;14:2870–92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02870.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakkum DJ, Shkolnik AC, Ben-Ary G, Gamblen P, DeMarse TB, Potter SM. Removing some ‘A’ from AI: embodied cultured networks. In: Iida F, Pfeifer R, Steels L, Kuniyoshi Y, editors. Embodied Artificial Intelligence. New York: Springer; 2004. pp. 130–45. [Google Scholar]
- Banker G, Goslin K. Culturing Nerve Cells. 2nd. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Baruchi I, Ben-Jacob E. Towards neuro-memory-chip: imprinting multiple memories in cultured neural networks. Phys Rev. 2007;E 75:050901. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.75.050901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beaulieu C. Numerical data on neocortical neurons in adult rat, with special reference to the GABA population. Brain Res. 1993;609:284–92. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90884-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beggs JM, Plenz D. Neuronal avalanches are diverse and precise activity patterns that are stable for many hours in cortical slice cultures. J Neurosci. 2004;24:5216–29. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0540-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berry RL, Teyler TJ, Han TZ. Induction of LTP in rat primary visual cortex: tetanus parameters. Brain Res. 1989;481:221–7. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)90797-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bi G, Poo M. Distributed synaptic modification in neural networks induced by patterned stimulation. Nature. 1999;401:792–6. doi: 10.1038/44573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bliss TV, Lomo T. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232:331–56. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bragin A, Hetke J, Wilson CL, Anderson DJ, Engel J, Jr, Buzsaki G. Multiple site silicon-based probes for chronic recordings in freely moving rats: implantation, recording and histological verification. J Neurosci Methods. 2000;98:77–82. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(00)00193-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brecht M, Fee MS, Garaschuk O, Helmchen F, Margrie TW, Svoboda K, Osten P. Novel approaches to monitor and manipulate single neurons in vivo. J Neurosci. 2004;24:9223–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3344-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canepari M, Bove M, Maeda E, Cappello M, Kawana A. Experimental analysis of neuronal dynamics in cultured cortical networks and transitions between different patterns of activity. Biol Cybern. 1997;77:153–62. doi: 10.1007/s004220050376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castro-Alamancos MA, Donoghue JP, Connors BW. Different forms of synaptic plasticity in somatosensory and motor areas of the neocortex. J Neurosci. 1995;15:5324–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-07-05324.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chao ZC, Bakkum DJ, Potter SM. Region-specific network plasticity in simulated and living cortical networks: comparison of the center of activity trajectory (CAT) with other statistics. J Neural Eng. 2007;4:1–15. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/4/3/015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chao ZC, Bakkum DJ, Wagenaar DA, Potter SM. Effects of random external background stimulation on network synaptic stability after tetanization: a modeling study. Neuroinformatics. 2005;3:263–80. doi: 10.1385/NI:3:3:263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corner MA, van Pelt J, Wolters PS, Baker RE, Nuytinck RH. Physiological effects of sustained blockade of excitatory synaptic transmission on spontaneously active developing neuronal networks—an inquiry into the reciprocal linkage between intrinsic biorhythms and neuroplasticity in early ontogeny. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26:127–85. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00062-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cossart R, Ikegaya Y, Yuste R. Calcium imaging of cortical networks dynamics. Cell Calcium. 2005;37:451–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ceca.2005.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeMarse TB, Wagenaar DA, Blau AW, Potter SM. The neurally controlled animat: biological brains acting with simulated bodies. Auton Robots. 2001;11:305–10. doi: 10.1023/a:1012407611130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eytan D, Marom S. Dynamics and effective topology underlying synchronization in networks of cortical neurons. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8465–76. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1627-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinerman O, Segal M, Moses E. Identification and dynamics of spontaneous burst initiation zones in unidimensional neuronal cultures. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:2937–48. doi: 10.1152/jn.00958.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fellous JM, Tiesinga PH, Thomas PJ, Sejnowski TJ. Discovering spike patterns in neuronal responses. J Neurosci. 2004;24:2989–3001. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4649-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross GW, Kowalski JM. Origins of activity patterns in self-organizing neuronal networks in vitro. J Intell Mater Syst Struct. 1999;10:558–64. [Google Scholar]
- Gross GW, Rhoades BK, Kowalski JK. Dynamics of burst patterns generated by monolayer networks in culture Neurobionics: An Interdisciplinary Approach to Substitute. In: Bothe HW, Samii M, Eckmiller R, editors. Impaired Functions of the Human Nervous System. Amsterdam: North-Holland; 1993. pp. 89–121. [Google Scholar]
- Gross GW, Rieske E, Kreutzberg GW, Meyer A. A new fixed-array multi-microelectrode system designed for long-term monitoring of extracellular single unit neuronal activity in vitro. Neurosci Lett. 1977;6:101–5. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(77)90003-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman KL, McNaughton BL. Coordinated reactivation of distributed memory traces in primate neocortex. Science. 2002;297:2070–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1073538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikegaya Y, Aaron G, Cossart R, Aronov D, Lampl I, Ferster D, Yuste R. Synfire chains and cortical songs: temporal modules of cortical activity. Science. 2004;304:559–64. doi: 10.1126/science.1093173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikegaya Y, Le Bon-Jego M, Yuste R. Large-scale imaging of cortical network activity with calcium indicators. Neurosci Res. 2005;52:132–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2005.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izhikevich EM, Desai NS, Walcott EC, Hoppensteadt FC. Bursts as a unit of neural information: selective communication via resonance. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:161–7. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00034-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izhikevich EM, Gally JA, Edelman GM. Spike-timing dynamics of neuronal groups. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:933–44. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson A, Mavoori J, Fetz EE. Long-term motor cortex plasticity induced by an electronic neural implant. Nature. 2006;444:56–60. doi: 10.1038/nature05226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jimbo Y, Robinson HP, Kawana A. Strengthening of synchronized activity by tetanic stimulation in cortical cultures: application of planar electrode arrays. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1998;45:1297–304. doi: 10.1109/10.725326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jimbo Y, Tateno T, Robinson HP. Simultaneous induction of pathway-specific potentiation and depression in networks of cortical neurons. Biophys J. 1999;76:670–8. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(99)77234-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kepecs A, Lisman J. How to read a burst duration code. Neurocomputing. 2004:58–60. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
- Kepecs A, Wang XJ, Lisman J. Bursting neurons signal input slope. J Neurosci. 2002;22:9053–62. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-20-09053.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkwood A, Bear MF. Hebbian synapses in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1994;14:1634–45. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-03-01634.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman JE. Bursts as a unit of neural information: making unreliable synapses reliable. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:38–43. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10070-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madhavan R, Chao C, Wagenaar DA, Bakkum DJ, Potter SM. Multi-site stimulation quiets network-wide spontaneous bursts and enhances functional plasticity in cultured cortical networks. Proc. of the 28th Int. Conf. of the EMBS 2006; New York, NY. 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maeda E, Kuroda Y, Robinson HPC, Kawana A. Modification of parallel activity elicited by propagating bursts in developing networks of rat cortical neurones. Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10:488–96. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00062.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin SJ, Grimwood PD, Morris RGM. Synaptic plasticity and memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:649–711. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin SJ, Morris RGM. New life in an old idea: the synaptic plasticity and memory hypothesis revisited. Hippocampus. 2002;12:609–36. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuzaki M. Factors critical for the plasticity of dendritic spines and memory storage. Neurosci Res. 2007;57:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2006.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulder AB, Arts MP, Lopes da Silva FH. Short- and long-term plasticity of the hippocampus to nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex pathways in the rat, in vivo. Eur J Neurosci. 1997;9:1603–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy TH, Blatter LA, Wier WG, Baraban JM. Spontaneous synchronous synaptic calcium transients in cultured cortical neurons. J Neurosci. 1992;12:4834–45. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-12-04834.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadasdy Z. Spike sequences and their consequences. J Physiol Paris. 2000;94:505–24. doi: 10.1016/s0928-4257(00)01103-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadasdy Z, Hirase H, Czurko A, Csicsvari J, Buzsaki G. Replay and time compression of recurring spike sequences in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1999;19:9497–507. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09497.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolelis MA. Brain-machine interfaces to restore motor function and probe neural circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:417–22. doi: 10.1038/nrn1105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pine J. Recording action potentials from cultured neurons with extracellular microcircuit electrodes. J Neurosci Methods. 1980;2:19–31. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(80)90042-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potter SM, DeMarse TB. A new approach to neural cell culture for long-term studies. J Neurosci Methods. 2001;110:17–24. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(01)00412-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potter SM, Wagenaar DA, DeMarse TB. Closing the loop: stimulation feedback systems for embodied MEA cultures. In: Taketani M, Baudry M, editors. Advances in Network Electrophysiology Using Multi-Electrode Arrays. New York: Springer; 2006. pp. 215–42. [Google Scholar]
- Rieke F. Spikes: Exploring the Neural Code. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Rolston JD, Wagenaar DA, Potter SM. Precisely-timed spatiotemporal patterns of neural activity in dissociated cortical cultures. Neuroscience. 2007;148:294–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruaro ME, Bonifazi P, Torre V. Toward the neurocomputer: image processing and pattern recognition with neuronal cultures. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2005;52:371–83. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2004.842975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasaki T, Matsuki N, Ikegaya Y. Metastability of active CA3 networks. J Neurosci. 2007;27:517–28. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4514-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segev R, Baruchi I, Hulata E, Ben-Jacob E. Hidden neuronal correlations in cultured networks. Phys Rev Lett. 2004;92:118102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.92.118102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shahaf G, Marom S. Learning in networks of cortical neurons. J Neurosci. 2001;21:8782–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-22-08782.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szarowski DH, Andersen MD, Retterer S, Spence AJ, Isaacson M, Craighead HG, Turner JN, Shain W. Brain responses to micro-machined silicon devices. Brain Res. 2003;983:23–35. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(03)03023-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tateno T, Jimbo Y. Activity-dependent enhancement in the reliability of correlated spike timings in cultured cortical neurons. Biol Cybern. 1999;80:45–55. doi: 10.1007/s004220050503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner JN, Shain W, Szarowski DH, Andersen M, Martins S, Isaacson M, Craighead H. Cerebral astrocyte response to micromachined silicon implants. Exp Neurol. 1999;156:33–49. doi: 10.1006/exnr.1998.6983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Pelt J, Vajda I, Wolters PS, Corner MA, Ramakers GJ. Dynamics and plasticity in developing neuronal networks in vitro. Prog Brain Res. 2005;147:173–88. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(04)47013-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voigt T, Baier H, Delima AD. Synchronization of neuronal-activity promotes survival of individual rat neocortical neurons in early development. Eur J Neurosci. 1997;9:990–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01449.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volman V, Baruchi I, Ben-Jacob E. Manifestation of function-follow-form in cultured neuronal networks. Phys Biol. 2005;2:98–110. doi: 10.1088/1478-3975/2/2/003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar DA, DeMarse TB, Potter SM. MEABench: a toolset for multi-electrode data acquisition and on-line analysis. 2nd Int. IEEE EMBS Conf. Neural Engineering; Arlington, VA. 2005. pp. 518–21. [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar DA, Pine J, Potter SM. Effective parameters for stimulation of dissociated cultures using multi-electrode arrays. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;138:27–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2004.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar DA, Potter SM. Real-time multi-channel stimulus artifact suppression by local curve fitting. J Neurosci Methods. 2002;120:113–20. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(02)00149-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar DA, Potter SM. A versatile all-channel stimulator for electrode arrays, with real-time control. J Neural Eng. 2004;1:39–45. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/1/1/006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar DA, Pine J, Potter SM. An extremely rich repertoire of bursting patterns during the development of cortical cultures. BMC Neurosci. 2006a;7:11. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-7-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar DA, Pine J, Potter SM. Searching for plasticity in dissociated cortical cultures on multi-electrode arrays. J Negat Results Biomed. 2006b;5:16. doi: 10.1186/1477-5751-5-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar DA, Nadasdy Z, Potter SM. Persistent dynamic attractors in activity patterns of cultured neuronal networks. Phys Rev. 2006c;E 73:519071–8. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.73.051907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werk CM, Chapman CA. Long-term potentiation of polysynaptic responses in layer V of the sensorimotor cortex induced by theta-patterned tetanization in the awake rat. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:500–7. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.5.500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werk CM, Harbour VL, Chapman CA. Induction of long-term potentiation leads to increased reliability of evoked neocortical spindles in vivo. Neuroscience. 2005;131:793–800. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. 1994;265:676–9. doi: 10.1126/science.8036517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
S This article has associated online supplementary data files
Figure S1: A. An example of a dendrogram showing different levels of the grouping process. All the individual BAMs are shown at the bottom in sorted order (BAMs that similar to each other were arranged together). At each level, the two most similar BAMs were grouped together, connected by a line. B. The corresponding sorted correlation matrix of BAMs. “Blocks” with high correlation values along the diagonal represent groups of bursts with similar BAMs. Data presented are from one representative Tetanus experiment. The colorbar indicates the correlation values.
Figure S2: Occurrence of cluster of BAM for evoked bursts changed before and across the tetanus. A. Each stroke represents one Occurrence of a BAM cluster of small evoked bursts. B. Each stroke represents one Occurrence of a BAM cluster of big evoked bursts. This is a representative example of a Probe experiment. The grey bar indicates the period of tetanization. The arrowheads indicate the timing of the each individual BAM cluster shown in C and D. C. Clusters of BAMs of small evoked bursts in the original coordinates of the multi-electrode array. The BAMs of each small evoked burst clusters were different from each other. Size of the circles represents the number of spikes in that particular electrode for a particular time bin. BAMs were collected every 20ms. D. Clusters of BAMs of big evoked bursts in the original coordinates of the multi-electrode array. The BAM of each of the big evoked burst cluster were different from each other. Size of the circles represents the number of spikes on that particular electrode for a particular time bin. BAMs were collected every 100ms since the big bursts were much longer the small bursts shown in part C.
Figure S3: Size distributions of spontaneous bursts were bimodal and did not follow the power law. Size distribution of spontaneous bursts for N=4 Tetanus experiments are plotted on log-log coordinates. Beggs and Plenz (2003, 2004) investigated recurring spontaneous patterns in local field potentials (LFPs) in acute slices of rat cortex (neuronal avalanches) and showed that the size distributions of avalanches obey the power law. The number of spikes per burst represents size of bursts here, which is similar to avalanche amplitude. For each experiment, a histogram of the number of spikes in every burst (time bin=100ms) in the experiment was plotted on a log-log scale.