Abstract
Background
Alexithymia is a personality trait characterized by deficiency in understanding, processing, or describing emotions. Recent studies have revealed that alexithymia is associated with less activation of the anterior cingulate cortex, a brain region shown to play a role in cognitive and emotional processing. However, few studies have directly investigated the cognitive domain in relation to alexithymia to examine whether alexithymic trait is related to less efficient voluntary control.
Methodology/ Principal Findings
We examined the relationship between alexithymic trait and voluntary control in a group of healthy volunteers. We used the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) to measure alexithymic trait. Additionally, we examined state and trait voluntary control using the revised Attention Network Test (ANT-R) and the Adult Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ), respectively. Alexithymic trait was positively correlated with the overall reaction time of the ANT-R, and negatively correlated with the Effortful Control factor of the ATQ.
Conclusions/Significance
Our results suggest that alexithymic trait is associated with less efficient voluntary control.
Introduction
Alexithymia was first introduced in the field of psychosomatic medicine and has recently been referred to as a personality trait characterized by a deficiency in the cognitive processing of emotions, namely, difficulties in identifying and communicating emotions, and externally-oriented thinking [1]–[4]. These characteristics reflect a disruption in the conscious experience of emotions [2]–[4]. Voluntary control, as an important aspect of consciousness and the source of attention, is critical for regulating mental computations including emotional processes. [5], [6]. However, this supervisory system is possibly severely impaired in individuals with high alexithymia [1], [2], [7], [8]. In addition to difficulties in recognizing and expressing emotions, these individuals often manifest flattened emotions at default, yet accompanied by random and abrupt emotional outbursts which they cannot interpret; they also have overcontrol of their internal needs, an exaggerated defensive system, and dysregulated autonomic responses such as increased heart rate to emotion-evoking stimuli, although always report less emotional experiences [7], [9], [10]. All these manifestations indicate a disconnection between the physiological responses and the voluntary control of emotions in alexithymia.
The notion that alexithymic trait is associated with voluntary control is derived from the aforementioned findings from psychosomatic medicine, and has been supported by experimental psychology studies carried out in nonclinical samples as well as neuroimaging findings. Behavioral studies have reported that alexithymic individuals are impaired in the cognitive processing of emotions [11]–[13]. One study used both the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) and the Level of Emotional Awareness Scale (LEAS) to assess alexithymic trait in a community sample of 380 subjects [11], and the participants were asked to identify emotions in a Perception of Affect Task (PAT). People with higher alexithymia scores had a decreased ability of recognizing both verbal and nonverbal emotions. Another study found that healthy adults with higher TAS-20 scores showed a diminished priming effect from contextual information to emotional words [12]. In other words, the presentation of an emotional context facilitated the processing of a related emotional word in a lesser extent in people with higher alexithymia scores than those with lower alexithymia scores. These studies clearly demonstrated that alexithymic trait is associated with the ability of the cognitive processing of emotions. As is mentioned before, although the top-down control of physiological responses are disrupted, autonomic responses per se are not impaired in people with high alexithymia [10], [14]. Therefore, alexithymia is viewed as “blindfeel”, the emotional equivalent of blindsight [4]. According to this thesis, alexithymia is a deficit in reaching the conscious awareness and in maintaining the voluntary control of emotions, rather than a disruption in the sensory/perceptual aspect of emotions. Alexithymic individuals can be emotionally aroused just as much as non-alexithymic individuals; however, they would report they do not feel anything or do not know how they feel, and consequently can not regulate their emotional states.
Neuroimaging studies have further supported this view by revealing that alexithymic trait is associated with a common neural substrate subserving voluntary control. Voluntary control is known to be implemented by a brain network including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and other frontoparietal regions [5], [6]. The ACC subserves a wide range of high-level functions including executive control, error detection, reward, anticipation, and consciousness [5], [15], [16]. The ACC is consistently activated in situations where competing information needs to be processed [17]–[20]. When the ACC is lesioned, executive control is likely to be affected, although this is not always the case [21]–[23]. On the other hand, accumulating evidence also suggests that the ACC is associated with overall reaction time (RT) in cognitive tasks, which represents the general efficiency of voluntary control. fMRI and PET studies have reported that the amplitude of ACC activation changes as a function of RTs in various cognitive tasks [24], [25]. Decreased ACC volumes are associated with longer RTs in cognitive tasks where controlled processes are required [26]. Indeed patients with focal ACC lesion have slower response speed during cognitive tasks [22]. We expect that a normal individual with a less efficient ACC, in that case, would also have diminished executive control, or slower response speed in cognitively challenging tasks, or both.
Interestingly, a deficiency in the ACC is indeed evident in alexithymic individuals [2]–[4]. In a positron emission tomography (PET) study by Lane and colleagues, a group of healthy adults performed an emotion-generating task in the scanner and completed the LEAS [27]. Covariate analysis revealed a significant cluster of activity in the dorsal ACC (Brodmann's area 24) that was positively correlated with LEAS scores. A more recent PET study replicated these results, and further demonstrated that the correlation between emotional awareness and dorsal ACC activity was specific to highly arousing pictures, and was stronger in women than men [28]. Other functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and PET studies reported that alexithymic individuals had decreased activation of the dorsal ACC in response to painful pictures [29] and emotional movie clips [30]. In the “blindfeel” theory, Lane proposed clearly that the ACC might be the core neuroanatomical structure involved in alexithymia [4]. Together with the evidence indicating the role of the ACC in voluntary control, it is likely that a person with more profound alexithymic trait would also possess poorer capacity of voluntary control, because of the common cause of a less efficient ACC function.
To date, few studies have directly tapped the relationship between alexithymic trait and voluntary control in general, especially in healthy adults, although a few domains of cognitive functions have been studied in association with alexithymia in patients. This line of research is important in that a significant correlation between measurement of voluntary control and that of alexithymia could verify that the two constructs are behaviorally relevant; and that a nonclinical sample would better elucidate the nature of alexithymia as a personality trait and/or an endophenotype and exclude the confounding factors introduced by other neurological and psychiatric conditions in patient studies. More specifically, it remains unclear (1) whether this impairment is specific to emotional processing, or is also related to a deficit in general cognitive processing, or a result of interaction of both cognitive control and emotional processing, and (2) which aspects of voluntary control might be related to alexithymic trait.
We used the TAS-20 to measure alexithymic trait in a group of healthy adult participants. Additionally, we measured participants' performance on the revised Attention Network Test (ANT-R) (state voluntary control), and individual differences in effortful control using the Adult Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ) (trait voluntary control). The ANT-R is a relatively challenging version that measures the general efficiency of voluntary control of attention, and the three networks subserving attention (alerting, orienting, and executive control). Correlation and regression analyses were performed on TAS-20 scores and participants' performance on ANT-R, and ATQ scores, to examine the association between alexithymic trait and voluntary control, and other aspects of temperament. We hypothesized that high alexithymia is correlated with (1) lower efficiency of state voluntary control indicated by slower response speed on the ANT-R task, and greater conflict effect; and (2) a deficiency in trait voluntary control, indexed by low scores on ATQ subscale Effortful Control.
Methods
Participants
Thirty young healthy adult volunteers without reporting any neurological or psychiatric disorders (15 females and 15 males; mean age, 25.4 years; range, 22–34 years) participated in this study. The consent procedure was approved by the institutional review board and written informed consent was obtained from each participant.
The 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20)
Alexithymic trait was measured by the TAS-20, which has been validated in both patient and nonclinical samples [31], [32]. The TAS-20 is a 20-item self-report questionnaire that was designed to measure the ability to regulate and communicate one's own emotions. The questionnaire is based on a 5-point scale from “1-strongly disagree” to “5-strongly agree”. A high score indicated high alexithymia – great difficulty in emotional awareness and regulation. The TAS-20 includes three factors: difficulty identifying feelings (DIF, e.g. “I am often confused about what emotion I am feeling”), difficulty describing feelings (DDF, e.g. “I am often confused about what emotion I am feeling”), and externally-oriented thinking (EOT, e.g. “I prefer to analyze problems rather than just describe them”). The factor structure of TAS-20 has been largely confirmed by confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) [31].
The revised Attention Network Test (ANT-R)
We used two measurements to assess state and trait voluntary control, respectively: the revised Attentional Network Test (ANT-R), and the Adult Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ). ANT-R and the original version of Attentional Network Test (ANT) were designed to assess three attentional networks that are critically involved in voluntary control: alerting, which refers to the ability of achieving and maintaining a vigilant state; orienting, for the selection of certain information out of numerous incoming stimuli; and executive control, a more complex system that monitors and resolves conflicts between competing processes[5], [6]. The ANT-R is designed to increase the task difficulty and to challenge one's ability to rapidly and accurately process information, thus the overall RT is an index of the efficiency of mental operation under complex and unpredictable situations.
The task is illustrated in Figure 1. Details of the task has been described elsewhere (Fan et al., under review). In brief, there are three cue conditions in each run: no-cue (baseline, 12 trials), double-cue (alerting, temporally informative, 12 trials), and spatial-cue (alerting and orienting, temporally and possibly spatially informative, 48 trials). RTs for the no- and double-cue conditions are used to assess the alerting benefit. To introduce the orienting component, a spatial cue and the subsequent stimulus are presented left or right of a fixation crosshair shown in the center of the screen. Participants need to shift their attention from the fixation point to the target stimulus to make the correct response. The validity of the spatial cue is manipulated in order to measure the disengagement and move operations (see [33], [34]. Specifically, 75% of the 48 spatial cues (36 trials) are valid and 25% (12 trials) are invalid. The probability of valid cue is the sum of the individual conditions of no-cue, double-cue, and invalid cue. Interpretation of these comparisons should be made with caution because of the frequency difference.
To introduce the conflict effect, the target (center arrow) is flanked on either side by two arrows of the same direction (congruent condition), or of the opposite direction (incongruent condition). To challenge the executive control function, double conflict that combines the flanker conflict effect [35] and the location conflict (Simon) effect [36] is introduced. There are 2 flanker congruency (congruent, incongruent) and 2 location congruency (congruent, incongruent) conditions.
A fixation cross is visible at the center of the screen throughout the duration of the task. In each trial, depending on the condition, either a transient cue (flashing of the box surrounding the stimulus row) is presented for 100 ms (the cued conditions) or the stimulus display remains unchanged (the no cue condition). After a variable duration (either 0, 400, or 800 ms, mean = 400 ms), the target and flankers are presented and remain visible for 500 ms. Cue-to-target interval is manipulated to challenge and measure the alerting and orienting speed. The duration between the offset of the target and the onset of the next trial is varied systematically, approximating an exponential distribution ranging with a mean trial duration of 5000 ms. The response collection window closes 1700 ms after the onset of the target and flankers as used in our original study [37]. The experiment consists of 4 runs, each with 72 test trials. The total duration for each run is 420 seconds. The total time required to complete this task is about 30 minutes.
The significant increase in attentional demands compared to our original design [37] is introduced by (1) manipulating the cue-to-target interval (0, 400, 800 ms), and using the brightening box for alerting as in a modified version of the ANT by Fernandez-Duque and Black [38]; (2) displaying the target on the left or right side of the fixation, manipulating cue validity to introduce the disengagement component, and extending the visual angle to create a larger size of the orienting effect; and (3) introducing the flanker by location dual conflict, and displaying the target only for 500 ms instead of 1700 ms.
The function of each of the three attentional networks is operationally defined as a comparison of the performance (RT and accuracy) between one condition and the appropriate reference condition, resulting in a score for each attentional network.
The phasic alerting (benefit) effect is defined as: Alerting = RT no cue−RT double cue, representing the benefit of the target response speed because of alerting.
-
Orienting operations can be separately measured as: Validity effect = Disengaging+(Moving+Engaging) = RT invalid cue−RT valid cue
Moving+Engaging = RT double cue−RT valid cue, for the benefit of target response under valid cue condition because of orienting and engaging in advance. Here, the Moving+Engaging is equivalent to the “orienting” effect we defined in our previous study [37].
Disengaging = RT invalid cue−RT double cue for the cost of disengaging from invalid cue.
In addition, Orienting time = RT valid cue, 0 ms cue-to-target interval−RT valid cue, 800 ms cue-to-target interval for benefit of the target response because of the advanced orienting.
- The conflict (cost) effect is defined as:
The inhibition of return (IOR) effect [33], [39] (if the difference is positive) or the cost of invalid cue under shorter (0 ms) compared to longer (400 ms) cue-target interval (if the difference is negative) is defined as: IOR = (RT invalid cue, 0 ms cue-to-target interval−RT valid cue, 0 ms cue-to-target interval)−(RT invalid cue, 400 ms cue-to-target interval−RT valid cue,400 ms cue-to-target interval).
The effects in accuracy follow the same formulas. Here, the interactions between attentional networks were not specific defined because we did not have related hypotheses to test in this study.
The task was compiled and run on a PC, with a 17 inch LCD monitor, using E-Prime™ software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA). Participants performed a brief practice task on a PC until they demonstrate at least 90% accuracy. Participants then performed the actual test.
The Adult Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ)
We administered ATQ to measure trait voluntary control, considering there is strong evidence suggesting that alexithymia is largely inherited [40]–[43], and that ATQ includes an Effortful Control subscale that measures the ability of voluntary control derived from biology.
The long version ATQ is a self-report questionnaire of 177 items and four factors. The factors and facets of ATQ are: 1) Effortful Control (EC): Inhibitory Control, Activational Control, and Attentional Control; 2) Extraversion (E): Sociability, High Pleasure, and Positive Affect; 3) Negative Affect (NA): Fear, Sadness, Discomfort, and Frustration; and 4) Orienting Sensitivity (OS): Internal Perceptual Sensitivity, External Perceptual Sensitivity, and Affective Perceptual Sensitivity. These factors have been shown to be closely associated with the Big Five personality factors [44]. Scores range from 1 (“extremely untrue of you ”) to 7 (“extremely true of you”). The ATQ is a psychometrically sound instrument to measure temperament based on Derryberry and Rothbart's temperament model [45]. The validity and reliability of ATQ have been previously confirmed by factorial analysis [44].
Statistical analysis
Means, standard deviations (SDs) were calculated for TAS-20, ANT-R, and ATQ. Skewness and kurtosis were also calculated to examine the distribution of these scores. Pearson correlation was used to examine the relations between TAS-20 and ANT-R, and between TAS-20 and ATQ. We further performed multiple regression analyses to examine the common and unique association among these variables. The first model included alerting, orienting, and flanker conflict scores as independent variables (IVs) and the overall score of TAS-20 as the dependent variable (DV). We then assessed a model where the four factors of ATQ were used as IVs and the overall score of TAS-20 as the DV. Finally we tested a model with the overall RT of ANT-R, the flanker conflict effect, and the EC factor of ATQ as IVs and TAS-20 overall score as the DV. The last model was used to examine the unique contribution of the general efficiency of state voluntary control (ANT-R overall RT) and trait voluntary control (EC).
Results
Alexithymic trait: Statistics and correlations of TAS-20
The mean scores, SDs, distribution, and correlations of TAS-20 and its three factors are listed in Table 1. The overall mean score was 42.6 (SD = 9.08, range 26–62), which is very close to the published norm [46]. This also indicates that on average our subject sample was not high alexithymia (cutoff point = 61), although one subject scored 62 and can be considered as high alexithymia [46]. The skewness and kurtosis of TAS-20 scores indicated that the distributions of TAS-20 scores were normal. We then performed a Shapiro-Wilk test [47] on these scores and the p values were all well above .05, indicating that the alexithymia scores in our subject sample were not deviated from normal distribution. The distribution curve of TAS-20 overall scores is shown in Figure 2. These results indicate that as a personality dimension, alexithymia is a continuous variable with normal distribution in healthy adults, which is consistent with previous findings [46], [48], [49].
Table 1. Means, SDs, distribution, and correlations of TAS-20 scores.
DIF | DDF | EOT | Overall | |
Descriptives | ||||
Mean | 13.7 | 11.7 | 17.2 | 42.6 |
SD | 3.74 | 3.81 | 4.00 | 9.08 |
Skewness | .35 | .28 | .39 | −.10 |
Kurtosis | −.45 | −.48 | −.71 | −.16 |
Correlations | ||||
DDF | .55** | |||
EOT | .32 | .41* | ||
Overall | .78** | .83** | .75** |
Note: * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). DIF: difficulty identifying feelings; DDF: difficulty describing feelings; EOT: externally – oriented thinking.
Table 1 also lists the correlations of TAS-20 and its subscales. Difficulty in Identifying Feelings (DIF) was significantly correlated with Difficulty in Describing Feelings (DDF) (r = .55, p<.01). DDF was also correlated with Externally-Oriented Thinking (EOT) (r = .41, p<.05). No significant correlation was found between DIF and EOT.
Correlations between alexithymic trait and performance on ANT-R
The operationally defined effects of ANT-R are shown in Table 2. Error trials, which included incorrect and missing responses, were excluded from the calculation of mean RTs. We checked the distributions of the mean RTs to ensure the validity of correlation analysis. All the RTs were normally distributed in this sample. For the attentional networks, the alerting effect was 29±24 (mean±SD) ms. The validity effect was 95±32 ms. Breaking down the orienting effect, the moving+engaging effect was 41±21 ms, and the disengaging effect was 54±24 ms. The cost of invalid cue under 0 ms cue-target interval was −60±39 ms and the orienting time was 57±31 ms. The cost of invalid cue under 0 ms cue-target interval here is the cost under short cue-target interval and the “orienting time” is an index of the orienting cost in time. The flanker conflict effect was 137±43 ms and the location conflict effect was −11±27 ms. The negative value of the location conflict effect indicates that the RT was shorter under the location incongruent condition, indicating an opposite direction of the location conflict effect. The global mean RT of ANT-R, representing the overall efficiency of voluntary control, was 605±59 ms.
Table 2. Means and SDs of ANT-R, and their correlations with TAS-20.
ANT-R | TAS-20 | |||||
ANT-R | Mean | SD | DIF | DDF | EOT | Overall |
Alerting | 29 | 24 | −.17 | .25 | −0.15 | −.03 |
Validity | 95 | 32 | −.14 | .20 | −.28 | −.10 |
Moving+Engaging | 41 | 21 | −.21 | −.01 | −.29 | −.20 |
Disengaging | 54 | 24 | .00 | .28 | −.13 | .06 |
IOR | −60 | 39 | −.04 | −.29 | −.02 | −.15 |
Orienting | 57 | 31 | −.16 | .27 | −.20 | −.04 |
Flanker conflict | 137 | 43 | .17 | .11 | −.05 | .10 |
Location conflict | −10 | 27 | −.10 | −.03 | −.04 | −.07 |
Overall RT | 605 | 59 | .43* | .29 | .17 | .37* |
Note: * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). DIF: difficulty identifying feelings; DDF: difficulty describing feelings; EOT: externally-oriented thinking; IOR: inhibition of return.
The correlations between TAS-20 and ANT-R are also shown in Table 2. The overall score of TAS-20 was positively correlated with the mean RT of ANT-R (r = .37, p<.05, see Figure 3A), indicating that subjects who were more alexithymic responded more slowly on the ANT-R task. In addition, TAS-20 subscale DIF was correlated with overall RT of ANT-R (r = .43, p<.05). Surprisingly, no significant correlation was found between TAS-20 (overall and subscales) and any of the individual effects of ANT-R, including the conflict effect.
We also examined the overall response accuracy and the accuracy of each effect. There was no significant correlation between the global mean accuracy of ANT-R and TAS-20 overall score ( r = −.26, p = .16), or between the accuracy of any effect of ANT-R and TAS-20 scores, indicating there was no speed-accuracy tradeoff.
Correlations between alexithymic trait and ATQ
Table 3 shows means and SDs of ATQ, and the correlations between TAS-20 and ATQ scores. We examined the distributions of the ATQ scores to ensure the validity of correlation analysis, and all the scores are normally distributed. As expected, participants who were more alexithymic scored lower on Effortful Control (r = −.50, p<.01, see Figure 3B), suggesting alexithymia is reliably correlated with trait voluntary control. Moreover, alexithymia was inversely correlated with Extraversion (r = −.41, p<.05), and positively with Negative Affect (r = .51, p<.01). No significant correlation was found between the overall score of TAS-20 and Orienting Sensitivity.
Table 3. Means and SDs of ATQ, and correlations between TAS-20 and ATQ scores.
TAS-20 | ||||||
ATQ | Mean | SD | DIF | DDF | EOT | Overall |
Effortful Control | ||||||
Activation Control | 4.6 | .99 | −.34 | −.09 | −.26 | −.30 |
Attentional Control | 4.1 | .99 | −.52** | −.37* | −.38* | −.53** |
Inhibitory Control | 4.6 | .67 | −.46** | −.14 | −.22 | −.35 |
Overall | 4.4 | .71 | −.55** | −.26 | −.37* | −.50** |
Extraversion | ||||||
High Pleasure | 4.5 | .83 | −.25 | −.19 | −.35 | −.34 |
Positive Affect | 5.0 | .84 | −.26 | −.42* | −.45* | −.48** |
Sociability | 5.0 | .94 | −.09 | −.40* | −.26 | −.32 |
Overall | 4.8 | .82 | −.21 | −.37* | −.38* | −.41* |
Negative Affect | ||||||
Discomfort | 4.1 | .86 | .31 | .21 | .28 | .34 |
Fear | 4.2 | .51 | .49** | .37* | .32 | .50** |
Frustration | 3.5 | .59 | .32 | .19 | .35 | .37* |
Sadness | 4.0 | .96 | .43* | .25 | −.03 | .27 |
Overall | 4.0 | .54 | .52** | .34 | .34 | .51** |
Orienting Sensitivity | ||||||
Affective Perceptual Sensitivity | 4.7 | .67 | −.19 | −.16 | −.18 | −.22 |
Associative Sensitivity | 4.5 | .68 | .15 | .20 | .19 | .06 |
Neutral Perceptual Sensitivity | 4.6 | .79 | .00 | .03 | .12 | .07 |
Overall | 4.6 | .59 | −.01 | .03 | −.09 | −.03 |
Note: *Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). DIF: difficulty identifying feelings; DDF: difficulty describing feelings; EOT: externally – oriented thinking.
The correlations between the three factors of TAS-20 and the factors and facets of ATQ are also listed in Table 3. DIF was negatively correlated with both Attentional Control and Inhibitory Control, indicating greater difficulty in identifying feelings is associated with reduced attentional and inhibitory control. DDF and EOT were only negatively correlated to Attentional Control. DDF was also negatively correlated to Positive Affect and Sociability, indicating increased difficulty in describing feelings was associated with decreased positive affect and sociability. EOT was also negatively correlated to Positive Affect. In addition, DIF was positively correlated to Fear and Sadnness, suggesting more difficulty in identifying feelings was related to more fear and sadness. DDF was also positively correlated to Fear. No significant correlation was found between the TAS-20 subscales and Orienting Sensitivity.
Multiple regression models
We first examined a multiple regression model with alerting, orienting, and flanker conflict scores as IVs and TAS-20 overall score as the DV, and the model was not significant (F(3,26) = .10, p>.05).
A second multiple regression model with the four factors of ATQ (EC, E, NA, and OS) as IVs and the overall TAS-20 score as the DV was significant (F(4,25) = 4.14, p = .01), and explained 39.8% of the variance in TAS-20. However, only the contribution of Extraversion reached a marginal significance (E: β = −.37, p = .088). None of the other IVs made a significant contribution to TAS-20 in this model (EC: β = −.27, p>.05; NA: β = .23, p>.05; OS: β = .14, p>.05).
Lastly the overall RT of ANT-R, EC, and flanker conflict was entered together as IVs. The model was significant (F(3,26) = 4.41, p = .01), and explained 33.7% of the total variance in TAS-20. In this model, EC had a significant contribution to TAS-20 (β = −.45, p = .01); ANT-R mean RT reached a marginal significance (β = .30, p = .099); and the contribution of flanker conflict was still not significant (β = .003, p>.05). This model confirmed our findings from the correlation analyses that the overall efficiency of ANT-R and Effortful Control contributed significantly to alexithymic trait, while conflict processing did not yield similar effect.
Discussion
The main finding of the current study is that alexithymic trait is closely related to the general efficiency of state voluntary control on a cognitive task, and trait voluntary control measured by the ATQ. Subjects with higher TAS-20 scores responded more slowly on the ANT-R, a task that requires rapid information processing, and scored lower on Effortful Control factor of the ATQ. In addition, alexithymic trait is associated with two other factors of ATQ, Extraversion and Negative Affect, indicating that alexithymic trait is a stable and inheritable.
An important finding from the ANT-R task is that alexithymic trait is closely related to the general efficiency of state voluntary control, rather than the individual attentional networks subserving voluntary control. This is in accordance with previous findings on the association between response efficiency and alexithymia characteristics in the context of emotional processing. For example, subjects with high TAS-20 scores displayed impaired task performance on a signal-detection paradigm only under a time constraint condition, but not under a temporally luxurious condition [50]. This suggests that alexithymia is a deficit in the fast processing of emotional information. Alexithymic individuals also had longer response latency in naming emotional words compared with controls [51]. Compared with previous studies, the ANT-R used in the current study does not involve any emotional valence, thus is an objective and direct measurement of the general efficiency of fast information processing and voluntary control. Our finding of a significant relationship between the overall score of TAS-20 and global mean RT of ANT-R suggests that the alexithymic characteristics are associated with less efficient voluntary control in general, instead of voluntary control of emotions per se.
Surprisingly, we did not find a significant relationship between the alexithymic trait and conflict processing. Conflict processing is an important component of voluntary control. It involves complex mental operations and is mostly used in conflict detection and conflict resolution. One would expect that conflict processing should be disrupted, if the overall efficiency of voluntary control is impaired. However, our data suggest that although the overall response speed is indeed slower in people with higher alexithymia scores, conflict processing is not necessarily disrupted in these individuals. This coincides with the finding that when the general slowness was taken into account, the significant greater conflict effect observed in patients with Alzheimer's disease disappeared [38]. Lesion studies have showed that patients with focal ACC lesion have normal conflict processing while performing Stroop or go-nogo tasks [22], [52], [53], but with a slower overall response speed. These studies may suggest that the general response speed and conflict processing are driven by a common factor. In the context of this study of healthy adults, less extreme TAS scores may not be sufficient to produce an impairment in conflict processing. Considering the limited sample size used in the current study, further investigation is needed to validate the association between alexithymia and executive control by preselecting and comparing groups with high and low alexithymia to increase the effect size.
The significant correlation between TAS-20 and the EC factor of the ATQ further demonstrated that alexithymic trait is also tightly related to trait voluntary control. Temperament is believed to be the part of personality that is biologically rooted [5], [44], [54]. Accordingly, temperamental effortful control refers to dispositional cognitive capacities that allow us to initiate or inhibit a particular response. These cognitive control capacities start to develop at early stages of life and are relatively stable across the lifespan [44]. If the general ability to initiate a response is impaired, it is reasonable to argue that the ability to produce a response to emotional stimuli will also be disrupted.
The association between alexithymic trait and voluntary control coincides with the role of the ACC in linking cognitive and emotional processes [15], [55]–[57]. Recent brain imaging studies have repeatedly reported ACC activation in emotional processes, especially in high-level voluntary processing of emotions rather than low-level responses to emotional stimuli [18], [27], [58]–[61]. More importantly, neuroimaging studies have showed abnormal activation of the ACC in individuals with high alexithymia [29], [30], [62]–[66]. A lesion study reported that a patient with a right anterior cingulate infarct presented with an alexithymia-like disorder [67]. Thus it is likely that alexithymic individuals have deficits in the ACC, supported by converging evidence from functional and structural neuroimaging studies and lesion studies.
A secondary aim of the current study was to examine the relationship between alexithymia and temperament. We found that alexithymia is associated with two temperament dimensions other than Effortful Control – Extraversion and Negative Affect. Previous studies have used other personality questionnaires including the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI)[68], [69], and the Temperament and Character Inventory[70], [71]. These studies have consistently reported that high alexithymia is correlated with low extraversion/ openness and high neuroticism/negative emotions, which is consistent with our findings. The association between alexithymia and temperament indicates that alexithymia is a stable personal trait [70], [72], [73] that is largely derived from our temperament and biological endowments. This view can be further supported by behavioral genetics studies suggesting that alexithymia is largely influenced by genetic factors [40], [41], [43].
In summary, the current study suggests that more profound alexithymic trait is closely related to less efficient voluntary control. Alexithymic trait and voluntary control are behaviorally relevant and also possibly share the ACC as a common neural substrate. Alexithymic trait is also associated with less extraversion and more negative affect in our nonclinical sample. These findings may facilitate our understanding of the alexithymic trait construct and may have theoretical implications for future research on its neural basis
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This research was supported by NIH Grant Number R21MH083164 to J.F.. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Taylor GJ. Alexithymia: concept, measurement, and implications for treatment. Am J Psychiatry. 1984;141:725–732. doi: 10.1176/ajp.141.6.725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bermond B, Vorst HC, Moormann PP. Cognitive neuropsychology of alexithymia: implications for personality typology. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. 2006;11:332–360. doi: 10.1080/13546800500368607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Larsen JK, Brand N, Bermond B, Hijman R. Cognitive and emotional characteristics of alexithymia: a review of neurobiological studies. J Psychosom Res. 2003;54:533–541. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00466-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lane RD, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Kaszniak AW. Is alexithymia the emotional equivalent of blindsight? Biol Psychiatry. 1997;42:834–844. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00050-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Attention, self-regulation and consciousness. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1998;353:1915–1927. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sifneos PE. The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1973;22:255–262. doi: 10.1159/000286529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nemiah JC. Alexithymia: present, past–and future? Psychosom Med. 1996;58:217–218. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199605000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Nemiah JC, Sifneos PE. Psychosomatic illness: a problem in communication. Psychother Psychosom. 1970;18:154–160. doi: 10.1159/000286074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Asendorpf JB. The discrepant repressor: differentiation between low anxiety, high anxiety, and repression of anxiety by autonomic-facial-verbal patterns of behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;45:1334. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.45.6.1334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lane RD, Sechrest L, Reidel R, Weldon V, Kaszniak A, et al. Impaired verbal and nonverbal emotion recognition in alexithymia. Psychosom Med. 1996;58:203–210. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199605000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Suslow T. Impairments of emotion situation priming in alexithymia. Personality and individual differences. 2002;32:541. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Suslow T, Kersting A, Arolt V. Alexithymia and incidental learning of emotional words. Psychol Rep. 2003;93:1003–1012. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2003.93.3f.1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Weinberger DA, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ. Low-anxious, high-anxious, and repressive coping styles: psychometric patterns and behavioral and physiological responses to stress. J Abnorm Psychol. 1979;88:369–380. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.88.4.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:215–222. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Paus T. Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:417–424. doi: 10.1038/35077500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bush G, Whalen PJ, Rosen BR, Jenike MA, McInerney SC, et al. The counting Stroop: an interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging–validation study with functional MRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 1998;6:270–282. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:4<270::AID-HBM6>3.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Whalen PJ, Bush G, McNally RJ, Wilhelm S, McInerney SC, et al. The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:1219–1228. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00251-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fan J, Hof PR, Guise KG, Fossella JA, Posner MI. The functional integration of the anterior cingulate cortex during conflict processing. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:796–805. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Botvinick M, Nystrom LE, Fissell K, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature. 1999;402:179–181. doi: 10.1038/46035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cohen RA, Kaplan RF, Moser DJ, Jenkins MA, Wilkinson H. Impairments of attention after cingulotomy. Neurology. 1999;53:819–824. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.4.819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? Brain. 2005;128:788–796. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Swick D. Anterior cingulate cortex and the Stroop task: neuropsychological evidence for topographic specificity. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:1240. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00226-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Naito E, Kinomura S, Geyer S, Kawashima R, Roland PE, et al. Fast reaction to different sensory modalities activates common fields in the motor areas, but the anterior cingulate cortex is involved in the speed of reaction. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:1701–1709. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.3.1701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hahn B, Ross TJ, Stein EA. Cingulate activation increases dynamically with response speed under stimulus unpredictability. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1664–1671. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Casey BJ, Trainor R, Giedd J, Vauss Y, Vaituzis CK, et al. The role of the anterior cingulate in automatic and controlled processes: a developmental neuroanatomical study. Dev Psychobiol. 1997;30:61–69. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun LS, Holmes A, et al. Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness. Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10:525–535. doi: 10.1162/089892998562924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.McRae K, Reiman EM, Fort CL, Chen K, Lane RD. Association between trait emotional awareness and dorsal anterior cingulate activity during emotion is arousal-dependent. Neuroimage. 2008;41:648–655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Moriguchi Y, Decety J, Ohnishi T, Maeda M, Mori T, et al. Empathy and Judging Other's Pain: An fMRI Study of Alexithymia. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2223–2234. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Karlsson H, Naatanen P, Stenman H. Cortical activation in alexithymia as a response to emotional stimuli. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192:32–38. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.034728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bagby RM, Parker JD, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. New trends in alexithymia research. Psychother Psychosom. 2004;73:68–77. doi: 10.1159/000075537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Posner MI. Components of visual orienting of attention. The Journal of neuroscience. 1984;4:1863. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, Rafal RD. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci. 1984;4:1863–1874. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01863.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Eriksen BA. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys. 1974;16:143. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Simon JR. Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the Stroop effect vs. the Simon effect. Acta Psychol. 1990;73:159. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90077-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Fernandez-Duque D, Black SE. Attentional networks in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:133–143. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Posner MMI. Inhibition of return: Neural basis and function. Cognitive neuropsychology. 1985;2:211–228. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Heiberg A, Heiberg A. Alexithymia – an inherited trait? Psychother Psychosom. 1977;28:221–225. doi: 10.1159/000287066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Valera EM, Berenbaum H. A twin study of alexithymia. Psychother Psychosom. 2001;70:239–246. doi: 10.1159/000056261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Grabe HJ, Ruhrmann S, Ettelt S, Muller A, Buhtz F, et al. Alexithymia in obsessive-compulsive disorder - results from a family study. Psychother Psychosom. 2006;75:312–318. doi: 10.1159/000093954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jorgensen MM, Zachariae R, Skytthe A, Kyvik K. Genetic and environmental factors in alexithymia: a population-based study of 8,785 Danish twin pairs. Psychother Psychosom. 2007;76:369–375. doi: 10.1159/000107565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE. Temperament and personality: origins and outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78:122–135. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.1.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Derryberry D, Rothbart MK. Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;55:958–966. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.55.6.958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Parker JD, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. The 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. III. Reliability and factorial validity in a community population. J Psychosom Res. 2003;55:269–275. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00578-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Shapiro SS. An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika. 1965;52:591. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Loas G, Corcos M, Stephan P, Pellet J, Bizouard P, et al. Factorial structure of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: confirmatory factorial analyses in nonclinical and clinical samples. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2001;50:255–261. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(01)00197-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gignac GE, Palmer BR, Stough C. A confirmatory factor analytic investigation of the TAS-20: corroboration of a five-factor model and suggestions for improvement. J Pers Assess. 2007;89:247–257. doi: 10.1080/00223890701629730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Parker PD, Prkachin KM, Prkachin GC. Processing of facial expressions of negative emotion in alexithymia: the influence of temporal constraint. J Pers. 2005;73:1087–1107. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00339.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Pandey R, Mandal MK. Processing of facial expressions of emotion and alexithymia. Br J Clin Psychol. 1997;36 (Pt 4):631–633. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01269.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Vendrell P. The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:341. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00116-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Stuss DT. Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:771. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00013-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Evans DE, Rothbart MK. Developing a model for adult temperament. Journal of Research in Personality. 2007;41:868–888. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Vogt BA, Finch DM, Olson CR. Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cereb Cortex. 1992;2:435–443. doi: 10.1093/cercor/2.6.435-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain. 1995;118 (Pt 1):279–306. doi: 10.1093/brain/118.1.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Josephs O, O'Doherty J, Zanini S, et al. Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain. 2003;126:2139–2152. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Singer T, Seymour B, O'Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, et al. Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science. 2004;303:1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Jackson PL, Meltzoff AN, Decety J. How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. Neuroimage. 2005;24:771–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gu X, Han S. Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of empathy for pain. Neuroimage. 2007;36:256–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Gu X, Han S. Neural substrates underlying evaluation of pain in actions depicted in words. Behav Brain Res. 2007;181:218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Berthoz S, Artiges E, Van De Moortele PF, Poline JB, Rouquette S, et al. Effect of impaired recognition and expression of emotions on frontocingulate cortices: an fMRI study of men with alexithymia. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:961–967. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.6.961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kano M, Fukudo S, Gyoba J, Kamachi M, Tagawa M, et al. Specific brain processing of facial expressions in people with alexithymia: an H2 15O-PET study. Brain. 2003;126:1474–1484. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Gundel H, Lopez-Sala A, Ceballos-Baumann AO, Deus J, Cardoner N, et al. Alexithymia correlates with the size of the right anterior cingulate. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:132–140. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000097348.45087.96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Meriau K, Wartenburger I, Kazzer P, Prehn K, Lammers CH, et al. A neural network reflecting individual differences in cognitive processing of emotions during perceptual decision making. Neuroimage. 2006;33:1016–1027. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Frewen PA, Pain C, Dozois DJ, Lanius RA. Alexithymia in PTSD: psychometric and FMRI studies. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1071:397–400. doi: 10.1196/annals.1364.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Schafer R, Popp K, Jorgens S, Lindenberg R, Franz M, et al. Alexithymia-like disorder in right anterior cingulate infarction. Neurocase. 2007;13:201–208. doi: 10.1080/13554790701494964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Wise TN, Mann LS, Shay L. Alexithymia and the five-factor model of personality. Compr Psychiatry. 1992;33:147–151. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(92)90023-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Luminet O, Bagby RM, Wagner H, Taylor GJ, Parker JD. Relation between alexithymia and the five-factor model of personality: a facet-level analysis. J Pers Assess. 1999;73:345–358. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA7303_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Picardi A, Toni A, Caroppo E. Stability of alexithymia and its relationships with the ‘big five’ factors, temperament, character, and attachment style. Psychother Psychosom. 2005;74:371–378. doi: 10.1159/000087785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Grabe HJ, Spitzer C, Freyberger HJ. Alexithymia and personality in relation to dimensions of psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1299–1301. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Rufer M, Ziegler A, Alsleben H, Fricke S, Ortmann J, et al. A prospective long-term follow-up study of alexithymia in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2006;47:394–398. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2005.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Martinez-Sanchez F, Ato-Garcia M, Ortiz-Soria B. Alexithymia–state or trait? Span J Psychol. 2003;6:51–59. doi: 10.1017/s1138741600005205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]