Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008 Aug 12;18(2):166–172. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.006

Fear, Faces, and the Human Amygdala

Ralph Adolphs 1
PMCID: PMC2580742  NIHMSID: NIHMS73540  PMID: 18655833

Summary

The amygdala’s historical role in processing stimuli related to threat and fear is being modified to suggest a role that is broader and more abstract. Amygdala lesions impair the ability to seek out and make use of the eye region of faces, resulting in impaired fear perception. Other studies in rats and humans revive earlier proposals that the amygdala is important not for fear perception as such, but for detecting saliency and biological relevance. Debates about some features of this processing now suggest that while the amygdala can process fearful facial expressions in the absence of conscious perception, and while there is some degree of pre-attentive processing, this depends on context and is not necessarily more rapid than cortical processing routes. A large current research effort extends the amygdala’s putative role to a number of psychiatric illnesses.

Introduction

A variety of brain structures implicated in social behavior, reward learning, and emotion have also been implicated in the recognition of emotions from stimuli, such as facial expressions. For instance, ventral regions of the prefrontal cortex are activated in neuroimaging studies of fear and anger; and emotion recognition can be impaired by lesions in this region. Sectors of right parieto-temporal cortex appear important for emotion recognition, especially fear recognition, as well. Some of the most compelling evidence comes from structures that participate in processing reward and punishment: the insula, the ventral striatum, and the amygdala. The insula appears disproportionately important for recognition of disgust (in facial expressions, related to distaste, and also related to moral disgust). Lesions of the ventral striatum can impair anger recognition [1]. And, perhaps most studied of all, lesions of the amygdala can impair recognition of fear from static [2] and dynamic [3] facial expressions. Similarly, amygdala activation can be disproportionate for facial expressions of fear or threat [4•] as well as correlate with correct identification of fear faces [5]. These findings are particularly intriguing given the amygdala’s well-known role in the acquisition and expression of Pavlovian fear conditioning [6], and may be related to its more recently demonstrated role in social transmission of fear information in rats [7•] and humans [8•]. Despite the large literature on this topic, there is considerable discrepancy: for instance, amygdala lesions can also impair recognition of emotions other than fear, and amygdala activation may not be at all specific to fear [9,10], nor to facial expressions of emotion [11]. Here I review the latest developments in the amygdala’s role in fear perception and debates regarding the automatic and non-conscious aspects of such processing, which rely on a number of different tasks (Table 1).

Table 1.

Fear-related processing and tasks to assess it.

Process Tasks
Fear expression Psychophysiology, startle, freezing, facial expression
Fear experience Inferred from fear expression; subjective report in humans
Innate fear Responses to smell of predators, biologically basic sensory stimuli
Learned fear Conditioning through experience, observation, or instruction
Fear detection Reaction time to fear stimuli, or accuracy in discriminating their spatial location of time of occurrence
Fear discrimination Accuracy in distinguishing fear from other contemporaneous stimuli
Fear classification Accuracy in distinguishing fear from a particular closed set of other stimuli
Fear recognition Multivariate judgments related to conceptual knowledge about fear
Rapid Neurological (EEG, MEG, electrophysiology) latency from stimulus onset Response (behavioral, neural, or psychophysiological) to stimuli of very brief duration
Automatic Pre-attentive responses to stimuli
Pop-out in visual search
Attentional effects by fear stimuli on performance related to other stimuli
(related in time or space)
Non-conscious Neurological, behavioral, or psychophysiological responses to stimuli with null-sensitivity in overt detection or discrimination

Is fear processing fast and automatic?

There is good evidence that at least some aspects of social perception can be quite rapid, based on faces shown for 100 ms or less [12]. Judgments of threat, but not other judgments, can even be made with faces that are presented as briefly as 39 ms (backward masked) [13•]. It is conceivable that a rapid initial sweep of stimulus processing detects basic features and gist, such as “threat”, and provides the basis for subsequent biasing of further information processing.

Other studies have argued that threat detection is not only rapid, but automatic, in the sense of being independent of attention. Data supporting this idea have come from visual search tasks, in which threat-related stimuli are reported to pop out [14], arguing that some affective processing precedes (and drives) attention to the stimuli, an idea about the primacy of affective processing that has a long history of debate in psychology. However, attempts to replicate these findings on visual search tasks involving threat-related faces have been inconsistent, and some experiments, perhaps especially with photographic-quality faces that the earlier studies did not use, argue against automatic processing of threat [15]. Fearful faces do appear to facilitate early visual processing in some way, however, since their locations enhance contrast sensitivity discrimination to subsequently shown gratings [16•]. A further complexity is that the extent to which threat processing is automatic depends on individual differences in anxiety [17]. This finding is in line with a rapidly expanding field investigating the role of individual differences in emotional face processing, both at the level of personality traits [18] as well as genetic polymorphisms [19,20]. Attention may come into play not as an essential ingredient for fear detection, but rather for modulating the degree of specificity in such detection: the amygdala responds most specifically to fear when subjects attend to the stimuli, but responds more broadly to threatening stimuli during unattended processing [21].

Studies of neurological patients have provided further ammunition for the idea of threat-related processing in the absence of attention. Patients with spatial hemineglect following unilateral right parietotemporal lesions normally fail to detect stimuli presented contralateral to the side of the lesion (i.e., left visual field) when shown stimuli in both fields. This attentional failure under bilateral stimulus presentation, however, is considerably reduced if the to-be-detected stimulus is a face, and further reduced if the face shows emotional expressions including fear [22]. A possibly related finding is that fearful facial expressions are most efficacious in breaking interocular suppression-- they render the face visible under conditions of continuous flash suppression, when it is typically invisible [23•]. Yet differential responses in the amygdala to threat or fear-related stimuli also appears to be subject to attentional capacity limitations: no such differences are seen when attention is entirely occupied with a difficult distractor task [24,25].

Do threat-related faces “pop out” in search tasks because they signal threat, or because of particular aspects of contrast or other low-level visual feature properties? The only way to definitively resolve this issue is to formally decouple sensory from affective properties experimentally. In one study, visual stimuli (complex but intrinsically neutral visual images) were fear conditioned and it was found that the fear-conditioned stimuli influenced the attentional blink phenomenon [26]. In another study, it was found that strongly affective lexical stimuli (words like “death”, whose association between low-level sensory and affective properties is truly idiosyncratic) also influenced the attentional blink, and moreover that this influence depended on the integrity of the amygdala [27]. These findings argue that fear perception, and the amygdala-dependent processing that underlies it, does incorporate rapid processing that is pre-attentive to some degree, and that it depends both on the acquired emotional meaning of stimuli as well as innate components. This interpretation is also consistent with an fMRI study that found amygdala activation both to emotional facial expressions as such, and an additional activation component due to emotional learning associated with those facial expressions [28].

Conscious and non-conscious processing

The above findings that threat detection can be rapid did not yet address whether it could also be non-conscious. Indeed, one study of rapid threat judgments emphasized their conscious nature, since briefer presentations (going from 39 to 26 ms) led to noticeably less reliable results [13•]. However, there are a number of studies arguing to the contrary: that fear and/or threat processing occurs in the absence of conscious perception. These studies have focused on the amygdala in particular, and build on a large related background literature which argues that the amygdala participates in auditory fear conditioning in part through subcortical inputs, that it modulates responses to emotional stimuli in visual cortices [29], and that it is important for fear recognition [2]. The strong hypothesis is that certain aspects of visual information (contrast, visual motion and low spatial frequency) are conveyed also through a subcortical route comprising a special subset of retinal ganglion cells that project to the superior colliculus, which projects to the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus, which projects to the amygdala; and that this route can provide the amygdala with rapid information related to threat in the absence of any cortical processing and in the absence of any conscious awareness of the stimulus.

Several different findings provide circumstantial support for the hypothesis [30]. Unilateral lesions to the pulvinar thalamus (and subjacent white matter) can abolish the normal interference of threat-related stimuli on goal-directed task performance and can impair facial fear recognition if encompassing the medial pulvinar [31]. The amygdala is activated differentially by fearful faces in patients with blindsight, who lack conscious visual perception of the stimuli due to damage in early visual cortices [32]. Young infants show behavioral preferences for faces over other visual stimuli, and possess a very immaturely developed visual cortex but relatively more developed subcortical pathway [33]. And fearful faces rendered non-conscious through interocular suppression (as verified by psychophysical assessment) resulted in a BOLD-response in the amygdala essentially indistinguishable in magnitude from that seen with consciously perceived fearful stimuli-- an effect that was not observed in the fusiform gyrus, nor with neutral facial expressions [34••].

Some difficulties with the idea that fear processing in the amygdala can be truly non-conscious comes from contrary findings arguing that amygdala responses to fearful faces require conscious perception [35••], raising important issues about how to establish with certainty the absence of conscious perception. Serious difficulties about the idea of a direct and rapid subcortical route of processing to the amygdala stem from the lack of anatomical and electrophysiological evidence. While the basolateral amygdala does receive direct input from the medial pulvinar [36], the inputs from the superior colliculus terminate in the lateral pulvinar [37]. This makes it necessary to interpose either intrinsic connections within the pulvinar or a collicular-pulvinar-extrastriate cortical route to the amygdala, both of which would be expected to be slower. Finally, there are inconsistencies in the latencies of responses to visual stimuli observed in the amygdala. Some electrophysiological studies in monkeys [38] have reported latencies around 60 ms, and MEG studies in humans reported latencies as short as 30 ms [39] (although these cannot be assigned with certainty to the amygdala given the spatial resolution of the technique). fMRI studies have also argued for a shorter latency of the BOLD response in the amygdala to fearful than to neutral faces [40]. By contrast, other studies in monkeys [41,42] as well as in humans [4345] have found responses well in excess of 100ms, and as slow or slower than those observed in temporal visual cortex. Additionally, it appears that visual response latencies in the pulvinar are similar to those in visual cortex [46].

The cortical temporal dynamics of emotional face classification were investigated in detail in a recent study that examined ERP responses to particular facial features [47••] (Figure 1). They found that fear is processed relatively faster than other emotions for a simple reason: cortical processing of facial features begins with the eye region of the face, which happens to be the feature most diagnostic for fear. The finding also suggests that the N170, an early cortical potential evoked by faces, reflects both automatic (early processing of the eye region of faces) as well as controlled aspects of processing (termination of processing once features diagnostic for the discrimination of a particular emotion had been reached). While this study does not resolve the debates about a direct subcortical route to the amygdala, it does suggest a mechanism to explain why fearful faces should be processed more rapidly than other expressions.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Rapid responses to eyes drive fear discrimination. ERPs recorded from right and left (dashed lines) occipitotemporal electrodes are plotted versus time (in ms) relative to the onset of sparsely revealed faces. The peak of this ERP response, corresponding to the classic “N170” is denoted by the blue boxes. Classification images derived by regressing the magnitude of the ERP response (in 4 ms time epochs) onto the randomly sampled location of facial features shown in the sparse stimuli are shown at the bottom. The ERP magnitude is shown by the blue curves; the overall sensitivity of the ERP to facial information is denoted by the red curves. Modified from [45••]; courtesy of Philippe Schyns.

What does the amygdala do?

A large prior literature has implicated the amygdala in facial emotion processing, but the specificity of this finding remains unclear. Curiously, there is no unequivocal evidence that the amygdala is critical for recognizing fear from body postures or movements ([48]; but see [49]), nor from auditory stimuli, although it may be necessary for normal fear recognition from music [50]. Within faces, the eye region of faces is used most prominently when subjects have to discriminate fear from other expressions [51]. Eye gaze direction modulates amygdala responses to fearful faces [52], pupil size modulates amygdala responses [53], and the eye region of fearful faces is most efficacious in breaking conscious suppression of the stimuli [23] and in activating the amygdala [54], even when they are presented very briefly and masked [55]. These findings have argued that perhaps particular features, notably the eyes, depend for their processing on the amygdala.

Bilateral damage to the amygdala results in an inability to fixate the eye region of faces spontaneously, and produces a consequent impairment in utilizing high spatial frequency information from the eyes in order to recognize fear [56] (Figure 2). This abnormal fixation has also been found for real people’s faces, where amygdala damage appears to result in a propensity to fixate the mouth rather than the eyes [57•]. This has led to a revision of the role of the amygdala in emotional face recognition: it does not appear to be specialized for processing threat or fear as such, or perhaps even emotion or reward as such, but rather appears to come into play when stimuli are particularly salient (which could result from them being threatening, rewarding, or unpredictable [58]). One recent study in rats and humans found increased amygdala activity in response to unpredictable compared to predictable tones, an effect that resulted from slower electrophysiological habituation to the unpredictable tones [59••]; another study found increased amygdala activation with sounds that increased in intensity [60]; and a third found amygdala activation depending on how sharp or curved object contours were, a factor that contributes to their perceived preference [61]. It may be that the amygdala is one component of a circuit that is important for processing biological relevance in a broader sense, as has been argued for emotion’s effects on attention more generally [62]. Such processing would be expected to depend on both low-level cues associated with relevance, as well as on contextual and appraisal-related evaluations relative to the circumstances in which a stimulus is encountered, and relative to the perceiver’s goals and capabilities.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Bilateral amygdala lesions impair the use of the eyes and gaze to the eyes during emotion judgment. Data are from patient SM, who has complete bilateral amygdala lesions and is impaired in recognizing fear [2]. Using sparsely revealed faces to identify face areas used during emotion judgment, patient SM (brain shown in c) differed from controls such that controls exhibited much greater use of the eyes than SM, while SM did not rely more on any area of the face than did controls (a). While looking at whole faces, SM exhibited abnormal face gaze (b), making far fewer fixations to the eyes than did controls. When SM was instructed to look at the eyes (d, “SM eyes”) in a whole face, she could do this, resulting in a remarkable recovery in ability to recognize the facial expression of fear compared to her accuracy prior to this instruction (d, “SM free”). Modified from [54].

Conclusions and Future Directions

Historically, research on the amygdala has emphasized two aspects: its role in social behavior (dating back to the classic studies by Kluver and Bucy in the 1930s), and its role in emotional learning and memory (such as Pavlovian fear conditioning). Both aspects rely in part on privileged allocation of processing resources to stimuli that have particular behavioral relevance, and both incorporate innate and learned components. The recent findings on the amygdala’s role in saliency and unpredictability raise the question of whether such a basic stimulus dimension could also explain other aspects of amygdala function. For instance, is it the inherently greater unpredictability of social stimuli that renders them more relevant and that engages the amygdala? While outside the focus of this review, it is also important to note here that the amygdala is known to play important roles guiding behavioral choice under uncertainty also in the context of reinforcement learning. It has been found to come into play during punishment predictions and prediction errors [63], and expectations of forthcoming reward value computed in the amygdala appear to be relayed to the prefrontal cortex in order to guide choice [64]. The amygdala is also activated during decisions that involve ambiguity (missing information), over and above risk [65]. All these findings from tasks involving decision-making are broadly consistent with the more perceptual/attentional role in processing saliency/unpredictability reviewed above.

One important extension of the amygdala’s role is to psychiatric illnesses in which the behavioral relevance of stimuli is abnormally evaluated. Illnesses ranging from phobias [66] to depression [19] to schizophrenia [67] have shown impaired emotion recognition and/or abnormal amygdala activation. One particularly interesting recent development has been in autism, where it was found that people with autism fail to make normal use of information from the eye region of faces, instead preferring to use the mouth [68], and that duration of eye gaze correlates with amygdala activation [69]. The finding is being extended also to the first-degree relatives of people with autism and may constitute part of an “endophenotype” for impaired amygdala function in autism [70,71], as has been found with respect to other structures, such as anterior cingulate cortex, that are connected with the amygdala [72].

Acknowledgements

Supported by grants from NIMH, the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation, and the Simons Foundation.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Calder AJ, Keane J, Lawrence AD, Manes F. Impaired recognition of anger following damage to the ventral striatum. Brain. 2004;127:1958–1969. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Fear and the human amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15:5879–5892. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-09-05879.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Graham R, Devinsky O, LaBar KS. Quantifying deficits in the perception of fear and anger in morphed facial expressions after bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hoffman KL, Gothard KM, Schmid MC, Logothetis NK. Facial-expression and gaze-selective responses in the monkey amygdala. Current Biology. 2007;167:766–772. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.040. A monkey fMRI study found that the basolateral amygdala responded differentially to facial expressions (threat vs. appeasement faces) whereas the central nucleus responded differentially to direction of eye gaze (and also correlated with skin-conductance response to the stimuli). The finding is an important step in further resolving the role of specific nuclei within the amygdala in facial emotion processing.
  • 5.Loughead J, Gur RC, Elliott M, Gur RE. Neural circuitry for accurate identification of facial emotions. Brain Research. 2008;1194:37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wilensky AE, Schafe GE, Kristensen MP, LeDoux JE. Rethinking the fear circuit: the central nucleus of the amygdala is required for the acquisition, consolidation, and expression of Pavlovian fear conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:12387–12396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4316-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Knapska E, Nikolaev E, Boguszewski P, Walasek G, Blaszczyk J, Kaczmarek L, Werka T. Between-subject transfer of emotional information evokes specific pattern of amygdala activation. PNAS. 2006;103:3858–3862. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0511302103. Using c-fos expression to index neuronal activation, this study found that “observer” rats paired with “demonstrator” rats showed strong amygdala activation depending on the prior treatment of the demonstrator rats (which either involved electrick shock conditioning or not). The finding shows that (unknown) social signals related to fear and threat can be transmitted from one rat to another, and result in amygdala activation in the observer.
  • 8.Olsson A, Nearing KI, Phelps EA. Learning fears by observing others: the neural systems of social fear transmission. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2007;2:3–11. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm005. In this fMRI study, the amygdala was activated differentially merely by observing another person acquire conditioned fear (through visual observation of them receiving electric shock paired with colored slides). This finding extends the amygdala's known role in Pavlovian fear conditioning through direct experience to conditioning acquired through social observation, which may be more common in many species including humans.
  • 9.Fitzgerald DA, Angstadt M, Jelsone LM, Nathan PJ, Phan KL. Beyond threat: amygdala reactivity across multiple expressions of facial affect. Neuroimage. 2006;30:1441–1448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.van der Gaag C, Minderaa RB, Keysers C. The BOLD signal in the amygdala does not differentiate between dynamic facial expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2007;2:93–103. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Britton JC, Taylor SF, Sudheimer KD, Liberzon I. Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: common and differential networks. Neuroimage. 2006;31:906–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Willis J, Todorov A. First Impressions: making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science. 2006;17:592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bar M, Neta M, Linz H. Very first impressions. Emotion. 2006;6:269–278. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.269. This study found that ratings of threat for faces shown (backward masked) for 1700 ms were highly correlated with threat ratings when the faces were shown for 39 ms, but not for 26 ms. Moreover, the low spatial frequency information from the faces was most responsible for driving the rapid threat judgments. The authors conclude that low spatial frequency information about very rapidly presented faces is sufficent for judgments related to threat, but that this still depends on conscious perception of the stimuli.
  • 14.Ohman A, Lundqvist D, Esteves F. The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:381–396. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Horstmann G, Bauland A. Search asymmetries with real faces: testing the anger-superiority effect. Emotion. 2006;6:193–207. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Phelps E, Ling S, Carrasco M. Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefit of attention. Psychological Science. 2006;17:292–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x. The location of fearful faces facilitated subsequent orientation discrimination of gratings (Gabor patches), showing that aversive emotion can influence contrast sensitivity, an aspect of early visual processing. These behavioral findings fit with neurological data for feedback from the amygdala to visual cortices [27].
  • 17.Bishop SJ, Duncan J, Lawrence AD. State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:10364–10368. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2550-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Beaver JD, Lawrence AD, Passamonti L, Calder AJ. Appetitive motivation predicts the neural response to facial signals of aggression. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:2719–2725. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0033-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Egan MF, Mattay VS, Hariri AR, Weinberger DR. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:828–834. doi: 10.1038/nn1463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Canli T, Qiu M, Omura K, Congdon E, Haas BW, Amin Z, Herrmann MJ, Constable RT, Lesch KP. Neural correlates of epigenesis. PNAS. 2007;103:16033–16038. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601674103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Anderson AK, Christoff K, Panitz D, De Rosa E, Gabrieli JDE. Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:5627–5633. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-13-05627.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Vuilleumier P, Schwartz S. Emotional facial expressions capture attention. Neurology. 2001;56:153–158. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.2.153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Yang E, Zald D, Blake R. Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. Emotion. 2007;7:882–886. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.882. Using continuous flash suppression [32], the experiment showed that upright, inverted, and just eye regions of fear faces gain access to consciousness (break the suppression) sooner than neutral of happy faces (or their equivalents). The findings suggest that the eye region of fear faces is sufficient to explain preattentive effects of fear faces.
  • 24.Bishop SJ, Jenkins R, Lawrence AD. Neural processing of fearful faces: effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:1595–1603. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Lim S-L, Padmala S, Pessoa L. Affective learning modulates spatial competition during low-load attentional conditions. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1267–1278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Smith SD, Most SB, Newsome LA, Zald DH. An emotion-induced attentional blink elicited by aversively conditioned stimuli. Emotion. 2006;6:523–527. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Anderson AK, Phelps EA. Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature. 2001;411:305–309. doi: 10.1038/35077083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hooker CI, Germine LT, Knight RT, D'Esposito M. Amygdala response to facial expressions reflects emotional learning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:8915–8922. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3048-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Vuilleumier P, Richardson MP, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:1271–1278. doi: 10.1038/nn1341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ohman A, Carlsson K, Lundqvist D, Ingvar M. On the unconscious subcortical origin of human fear. Physiology and Behavior. 2007;92:180–185. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ward R, Calder AJ, Parker M, Arend I. Emotion recognition following human pulvinar damage. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1973–1978. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Pegna AJ, Khateb A, Lazeyras F, Seghier ML. Discriminating emotional faces without primary visual cortices involves the right amygdala. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:24–25. doi: 10.1038/nn1364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Johnson MH. Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:766–774. doi: 10.1038/nrn1766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Jiang Y, He S. Cortical responses to invisible faces: dissociating subsystems for facial-information processing. Current Biology. 2006;16:2023–2029. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.08.084. Using continuous flash suppression, in which rapidly flickering patterns shown into one eye suppress conscious perception of a target stimulus shown to the other eye, this fMRI experiment found BOLD signal bilaterally in the amygdala whose magnitude did not differ between nonconscious (suppressed) versus conscious perception of fear faces. Furthermore, the effect showed substantial individual differences. A similar effect was found in the superior temporal gyrus. By contrast, BOLD signal to neutral faces was smaller in the amygdala to suppressed fear faces, and BOLD signal to suppressed fear faces was smaller in the fusiform gyrus. The finding argues that the amygdala participates in non-conscious fear processing.
  • 35.Pessoa L, Japee S, Sturman D, Ungerleider LG. Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16:366–375. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi115. As with the individual differences found in [32], this study found that briefly presented masked fear faces were sometimes detected and sometimes not, using ROC analyses. Amygdala activation was strongly modulated by whether or not the faces were detected, and thus would be expected to show strong individual differences in all studies as a function of the visibility of the stimulus for that particular subject. The finding suggests that some of the discrepancies between prior studies investigating the amygdala's role in nonconscious fear processing arise from lack of quantitative assessment of the stimulus visibility together with lack of accounting for individual differences on this measure.
  • 36.Romanski LM, Giguere M, Bates JF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Topographic organization of medial pulvinar connections with the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1997;379:313–332. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Benevento LA, Fallon JH. The ascending projections of the superior colliculus in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1975;160:339–362. doi: 10.1002/cne.901600306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Nakamura K, Mikami A, Kubota K. Activity of single neurons in the monkey amygdala during performance of a visual discrimination task. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;67:1447–1463. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.6.1447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Luo Q, Holroyd T, Jones M, Hendler T, Blair J. Neural dynamics for facial threat processing as revealed by gamma band synchronization using MEG. Neuroimage. 2007;34:839–847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Reinders AA, Glaescher J, deJong JR, Willemsen AT, den Boer JA, Buechel C. Detecting fearfula and neutral faces: BOLD latency differences in amygdala-hippocampal junction. Neuroimage. 2006;33:805–814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Leonard CM, Rolls ET, Wilson FAW, Baylis GC. Neurons in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. Behavioral Brain Research. 1985;15:159–176. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90062-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kuraoka K, Nakamura K. Responses of single neurons in monkey amygdala to facial and vocal emotions. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:1379–1387. doi: 10.1152/jn.00464.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. Category-specific visual responses of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:946–953. doi: 10.1038/78868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Oya H, Kawasaki H, Howard MA, Adolphs R. Electrophysiological responses in the human amygdala discriminate emotion categories of complex visual stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:9502–9512. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-21-09502.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Krolak-Salmon P, Henaff MA, Vighetto A, Bertrand O, Mauguiere F. Early amygdala reaction to fear spreading in occipital, temporal, and frontal cortex: a depth electrode ERP study in human. Neuron. 2004;42:665–676. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00264-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Ouellette B, Casanova C. Overlapping visual response latency distributions in visual cortices and LP-pulvinar complex of the cat. Experimental Brain Research. 2006;175:332–341. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0555-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Schyns PG, Petro LS, Smith ML. Dynamics of visual information integration in the brain for categorization facial expressions. Current Biology. 2007;17:1580–1585. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.048. This ERP study measured occipitotemporal ERP amplitude to sparsely revealed small pieces of faces (“bubbles”; cf. Figure 1 and Figure 2). Classification images were derived by regressing the ERP amplitude, in 4 ms bins, onto the location of the shown face regions. The earliest ERP responses correlated with information in the eye region of the face, and this information was sufficient for discriminating fear. The study suggests that fear perception may be rapid because of this feature of face processing that begins with processing the eye region before moving onto lower regions of the face.
  • 48.Atkinson AP, Heberlein AS, Adolphs R. Spared ability to recognise fear from static and moving whole-body cues following bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2772–2782. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Grézes J, Pichon S, de Gelder B. Perceiving fear in dynamic body expressions. Neuroimage. 2007;35:959–967. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gosselin N, Peretz I, Johnsen E, Adolphs R. Amygdala damage impairs emotion recognition from music. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:236–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Smith ML, Cottrell GW, Gosselin F, Schyns PG. Transmitting and decoding facial expressions. Psychological Science. 2005;16:184–189. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00801.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Adams RB, Gordon HL, Baird AA, Ambady N, Kleck RE. Effects of gaze on amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. Science. 2003;300:1536. doi: 10.1126/science.1082244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Demos KE, Kelley WM, Ryan SL, Davis FC, Whalen PJ. Human amygdala sensitivity to the pupil size of others. Cerebral Cortex. 2008 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn034. advance online publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Morris JS, deBonis M, Dolan RJ. Human amygdala responses to fearful eyes. Neuroimage. 2002;17:214–222. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Whalen PJ, Kagan J, Cook RG, Davis FC, Kim H, Polis S, McLaren DG, Somerville LH, McLean AA, Maxwell JS, et al. Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science. 2004;306:2061. doi: 10.1126/science.1103617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Adolphs R, Gosselin F, Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Schyns P, Damasio AR. A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature. 2005;433:68–72. doi: 10.1038/nature03086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Spezio ML, Huang P-YS, Castelli F, Adolphs R. Amygadala damage impairs eye contact during conversations with real people. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:3994–3997. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3789-06.2007. Testing subject SM (Fig. 2), who has bilateral amygdala damage, in a face-to-face eyetracking experiment, it was found that she failed to fixate the eyes of real people, instead looking more at the mouth.
  • 58.Whalen PJ. The uncertainty of it all. TICS. 2007;11:499–500. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Herry C, Bach DR, Esposito F, DiSalle F, Perrig WJ, Scheffler K, Luethi A, Seifritz E. Processing of temporal unpredictability in human and animal amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:5958–5966. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5218-06.2007. A study in mice and humans showed that the amygdala was activated (c-fos, fMRI, and electrophysiology) more for temporally unpredictable (jittered) tones than for predictable (regular interval) tones. Notably, there was nothing specifically social or emotional about these tones; i.e., they had not been associated with any other stimulus. The effect obtained not only at the neurological level, but also on a number of tasks indexing stress and anxiety. Physiologically, the effect appeared to arise from decreased habituation of neuronal responses in the amygdala to the unpredictable tones. The study suggests that unpredictability per se, rather than dimensions of emotion or social relevance, are sufficient to engage the amygdala, and raises the question of whether this more basic factor might also come into play, or even account for, the amygdala's role in emotion and social cognition.
  • 60.Bach DR, Schaechinger H, Neuhoff JG, Esposito F, Di Salle F, Lehmann C, Herdener M, Scheffler K, Seifritz E. Rising sound intensity: an intrinsic warning cue activating the amygdala. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:145–150. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bar M, Neta M. Visual elements of subjective preference modulate amygdala activation. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2191–2200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Brosch T, Sander D, Pourtois G, Scherer KR. Beyond fear: rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychological Science. 2008;19:362–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Yacubian J, Glaescher J, Schroeder K, Sommer T, Braus DF, Buechel C. Dissociable systems for gain- and loss-related value predictions and errors of prediction in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:9530–9537. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2915-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hampton AN, Adolphs R, Tyszka MJ, O’Doherty JP. Contributions of the amygdala to reward expectancy and choice signals in human prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2007;55:545–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hsu M, Bhatt M, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Camerer CF. Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science. 2005;310:1680–1683. doi: 10.1126/science.1115327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Larson CL, Schaefer HS, Siegle GJ, Jackson CAB, Anderle MJ, Davidson RJ. Fear is fast in phobic individuals: amygdala activation in response to fear-relevant stimuli. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60:410–417. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gur RE, Loughead J, Kohler CG, Elliott M, Lesko K, Ruparel K, Wolf DH, Bilker WB, Gur RC. Limbic activation associated with misidentification of fearful faces and flat affect in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:1356–1366. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Spezio ML, Adolphs R, Hurley RS, Piven J. Abnormal use of facial information in high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:929–939. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0232-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Dalton KM, Nacewicz BM, Johnstone T, Schaefer HS, Gernsbacher MA, Goldsmith HH, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ. Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:519–526. doi: 10.1038/nn1421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Adolphs R, Spezio ML, Parlier M, Piven J. Selective face processing abnormalities in parents of autistic children. Current Biology in press. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.073. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Dalton KM, Nacewicz BM, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ. Gaze-fixation, brain activation, and amygdala volume in unaffected siblings of individuals with autism. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:512–520. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Chiu PH, Kayali MA, Kishida KT, Tomlin D, Klinger LG, Klinger MR, Montague PR. Self responses along cingulate cortex reveal quantitative neural phenotype for high-functioning autism. Neuron. 57:463–473. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES