Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008 May 1;90(1):171–177. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.03.005

Progesterone to ovariectomized mice enhances cognitive performance in the spontaneous alternation, object recognition, but not placement, water maze, and contextual and cued conditioned fear tasks

Cheryl A Frye a,b,c,d,*, Alicia A Walf a
PMCID: PMC2581417  NIHMSID: NIHMS73844  PMID: 18455450

Abstract

Research on how steroid hormones mediate mnemonic processes have focused on effects of 17β-estradiol (E2); yet, progesterone (P4) co-varies with E2 across endogenous hormonal milieu, and itself may influence cognitive processes. We investigated the hypothesis that acute P4 treatment enhances cognitive performance compared to vehicle. Ovariectomized (OVX) c57/BL6J mice were randomly assigned to be subcutaneously injected with oil vehicle or P4 (10 mg/kg). Mice were trained in the spontaneous alternation, object recognition, object placement, water maze, or fear conditioning tasks, and injected with vehicle or P4 before training or immediately post-training, and then were tested 1, 4, or 24 h later. The data obtained from these experiments supported our hypothesis. P4 increased the percentage of spontaneous alterations made in a T-maze more so than did vehicle. P4, compared to vehicle, increased the percentage of time spent exploring the novel object in the object recognition task, but did not alter performance in the object placement task. P4, compared to vehicle, decreased latencies to reach the location in the water maze where the platform had been during training in a probe trial, but did not alter performance in the control, cued trial. Compared to vehicle, P4 treatment increased freezing in contextual and cued fear testing. Thus, acute P4 treatment to OVX mice can improve cognitive performance across a variety of tasks.

Keywords: Allopregnanolone, Hippocampus, Cortex, Learning and memory, Neurosteroids

1. Introduction

Steroid hormones are important trophic factors that play a key role in the structure and functioning of the nervous system. Steroid hormones regulate essential neuronal and glial function (Schumacher et al., 2000), which contribute to behavioral outcomes. For example, many studies have demonstrated that 17β-estradiol (E2) has beneficial effects for learning and memory (Azcoitia, Doncarlos, & Garcia-Segura, 2003; Bisagno, Bowman, & Luine, 2003; Luine, Jacome, & Maclusky, 2004; Fillit & Luine, 1997; McEwen, 2001), neurogenesis within the hippocampus (Galea, in press; Tanapat, Hastings, & Gould, 2005), and may have neuroprotective effects (Brinton, 2004; Frye & Rhodes, 2005; Gibbs & Gabor, 2003). Progesterone (P4), like E2, is a pleiotropic hormone in the nervous system that can exert cognitive and/or neuroprotective effects. Indeed, injections of E2 and P4 to young ovariectomized (OVX) rats enhances performance in several cognitive measures, including the object recognition, object placement, and water maze (Frye, Duffy, & Walf, 2007; Gibbs, 2003; Sandstrom & Williams, 2001; Walf, Rhodes, & Frye, 2006). However, E2 and P4 do not necessarily have the same effects on cognitive performance when rodents are treated with each steroid alone or when they are co-administered. Endogenous changes in E2 over reproductive cycles of rats are accompanied by variations in progestogens, and each may influence cognitive performance differently. During the luteal phase P4 levels positively correlate with motor coordination and better performance on perceptual, visual, and verbal memory tasks (Berman et al., 1997; Broverman et al., 1981; Hampson, 1990; Hampson & Kimura, 1988; Phillips & Sherwin, 1992a). Furthermore, performance on frontal lobe tasks, implicit memory, and attention is better when P4 levels are high (Hollander, Hausmann, Hamm, & Corballis, 2005; Maki, Rich, & Rosenbaum, 2002; Solis-Ortiz, Guevara, & Corsi-Cabrera, 2004), whereas spatial performance is typically improved during menstruation, when E2 and P4 levels are lower (Hampson & Kimura, 1988; Hausmann, Slabbekoorn, Van Goozen, Cohen-Kettenis, & Gunturkun, 2000; McCormick & Teillon, 2001; Phillips & Silverman, 1997; Rosenberg & Park, 2002). In animal models, rats in proestrus (which have high E2 and P4) perform significantly better in the inhibitory avoidance, object recognition, and object placement tasks, and more readily acquire trace conditioning, than do diestrous rats with lower E2 and P4 levels (Frye et al., 2007; Walf et al., 2006; Wood, Beylin, & Shors, 2001). In contrast, rats in proestrus perform no differently or worse in the water maze (Frye, 1995; Stackman, Blasberg, Langan, & Clark, 1997). Thus, E2 and P4 may have different effects on cognitive performance, which likely depend upon many factors, such as the task utilized.

Although the majority of studies investigating ovarian steroid regulation of cognitive behavior have investigated effects of E2, or its co-administration with P4, there is evidence that P4 may have beneficial cognitive effects. Treatment with P4 alone post-training to OVX rats enhances object recognition and object placement performance and increases progestogen levels in the cortex and hippocampus (Frye et al., 2007; Walf et al., 2006). However, there may be regimen-dependent effects of P4 for cognitive performance that need to be addressed further. In studies of acute treatment of rats with dosages of P4, or its neuroactive metabolites (which would be expected to produce circulating levels of these progestogens higher than what is typically observed during proestrus), produce decrements in cognitive performance (Johansson, Birzniece, Lindblad, Olsson, & Backstrom, 2002; Zou, Yamada, Sasa, Nakata, & Nabeshima, 2000). We have demonstrated that chronic P4 treatment to aged mice enhances cognitive function in the spontaneous alternation T-maze task and object placement task, and produces levels of progestogens in plasma and brain that are akin to those observed during proestrus (Frye & Walf, 2008). Together, these studies suggest that physiological dosing with P4 may improve cognitive performance of female rats and mice. However, subtle differences, related to behavioral and/or hormonal responsiveness (Frick, Stillner, & Berger-Sweeney, 2000), in the nature of P4's effects for these processes likely exist. To begin to investigate this, the effects of acute treatment of physiological P4 to young, adult OVX mice for cognitive performance needs to be determined.

Given the aging population, the potential for steroids to have beneficial effects on cognition (Brinton, 2004), and the potential relevance for human health, whether P4 can exert beneficial cognitive effects across specifies is of interest. If P4 has cognitive effects that are important and meaningful, one would expect conservation of these mnemonic effects across species. As such, we tested the hypothesis that physiological dosing with P4 to OVX mice would improve cognitive performance. OVX c57/BL6J mice were treated with P4 pre-training, immediately post-training, and with a delay of 1.5 h post-training. Mice were tested 1, 4, and 24 h later when P4 levels would expected to be physiological (1 h and 4 h post-injection) and at nadir (24 h post-injection).

2. Materials and methods

All methods utilized were pre-approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at The University at Albany-SUNY. All experimentation was conducted in accordance with accepted standards of humane animal use, the US. Public Health Service “Policy on Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals” and the National Institutes of Health “Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.”

2.1. Subjects and housing

Female C57/BL6J mice (Cohort 1, n = 24; Cohort 2, n = 16) were obtained from Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME) and bred in our laboratory. Mice were housed in groups of 4–5, in cages that had woodchips for bedding and a Nestlet square. Mice were housed in the Laboratory Animal Care Facility at the University at Albany in the Life Sciences Research Building. The room had a 12/12 h reversed light/dark cycle, with the lights going off at 8:00 am. Mice had ad libitum access to Purina Rodent Chow and tap water in their home cages.

2.2. Ovariectomy

Under sodium pentobarbital anesthesia (80 mg/kg), 2-month-old mice were OVX via bilateral incisions on their flanks. Mice recovered from surgery for one week before habituation to handling/behavioral testing began.

2.3. General procedure

Mice were randomly assigned to receive P4 or vehicle throughout the experiment. A week post-surgery, mice experienced a 5-day habituation protocol prior to behavioral testing. After habituation, mice were tested in one task per day, every 4–5 days. The behavioral tasks utilized are described below. Mice were trained and tested in the dark cycle, between 8:00 am and 2:00 pm, so that they would be in the active phase of their circadian cycle.

2.4. Screening procedure

Approximately one week after OVX, each mouse was carefully evaluated to ascertain whether they exhibited normative responses (Moy et al., 2007). The general health, reflexes, motor behavior, and/or coordination of each mouse were examined. This was done to rule out any potential baseline differences in performance that may have influenced the cognitive endpoints under examination. The general appearance (fur, whiskers, posture, gait, muscle tone) and behavior (grooming, nest building, huddling, cage climbing, paw withdrawal) was examined for each mouse. No differences were observed and all mice were included in the study.

2.5. Handling procedure

The following procedure was utilized to habituate mice to handling and behavioral observation by the experimenter before behavioral testing began (Frye et al., 2006a). On day 1, mice were picked up from their home cage, handled for 15 s, and returned to their home cage. On day 2, mice were moved from their home cage to a transport cage (without food and water). On day 3, mice were weighed and then returned to their home cage. On day 4, mice in their home cages were transferred to another room on a cart and then placed in a transport cage for 5 min. On day 5, mice were transferred in their home cages on a cart, then were placed in transport cages, and were subsequently placed in the open field utilized in the object recognition task described below) for 5 min.

2.6. Hormone treatment

Mice in Cohort 1 were assigned to receive P4 (10 mg/kg, SC; Steraloids, Newport, RI) or vehicle (90% vegetable oil, 10% ethanol) immediately after training in each of the tasks described below, except the spontaneous alternation task. In the spontaneous alternation task, mice in Cohort 1 were injected with P4 or vehicle 1 h before testing. Mice in Cohort 2 were injected with P4 or vehicle 1.5 h post-training in the object recognition and conditioned fear tasks (and were not trained or tested in spontaneous alternation, water maze, or object placement tasks in this study). This regimen of P4 produces plasma and hippocampus levels of progestogens that are typically observed during behavioral estrus of mice as measured by radioimmuno-assay in our laboratory (for P4: ∼34–38 ng/ml in plasma and ∼18–21 ng/g in hippocampus, and for 3α,5α-THP: ∼19–22 ng/ml in plasma and ∼19–21 ng/g in hippocampus—Frye, Sumida, Lydon, O'Malley, & Pfaff, 2006b).

2.7. Behavioral testing

Mice were trained and tested consecutively in the tasks in the order in which they are described below. Immediately before and after training and testing, mice were housed in transport cages. We investigated the effects of P4 in a number of cognitive tasks. Furthermore, the tasks utilized have different inter-trial intervals (described below) and mice receive P4 treatment pre-training (spontaneous alternation) or immediately post-training (object recognition, object placement, water maze, and fear conditioning). In the object recognition and fear conditioning tasks, performance of another cohort of mice (that had been similarly repeatedly injected with P4 and behaviorally tested) that were treated with vehicle or P4 1.5 h after training, was also assessed. All mice were tested in the same order in all of the tasks. This was done so that potential carry-over effects could be accounted for across experimental conditions and behavioral tasks.

2.8. Spontaneous alternation

Performance in the spontaneous alternation task was assessed using a T-Maze as per previous methods (Frye & Walf, 2008; Hughes, 2004). The T-maze consisted of a clear plexiglas start box with a guillotine door, which is connected to one start (30.5 × 9 × 7 cm) and two goal (17.8 × 9 × 7 cm) arms made of black plexiglas. Briefly, mice were injected with P4 or vehicle, and 1 h later, were placed in the start box and one forced arm choice was made with one goal arm blocked. After the forced trial, the blockade of the arm was removed. Mice then were tested for 13 trials (maximum latency to complete all 13 trials, 900 s) and the number of spontaneous alternations made (entering the right goal arm after the left goal arm was entered and vice versa), as a function of the total arm entries, during these trials was recorded. A higher percentage of alternations made in the free choice trials [(number of alternating entries made/the total number of free choice entries made) × 100] was used as an index of enhanced performance. A chance level of responding of rodents in this task is about 50%, and we typically see that control mice make 40–60% alternations. Differences between mouse strains in this task are clear and may account for some of the variability in the task (Gerlai, 1998).

2.9. Object recognition

Object recognition was implemented as previously described (Ennaceur & Delacour, 1988; Frye & Walf, 2008; Luine, Jacome, & Maclusky, 2003; Walf et al., 2006). During training, mice were placed in a corner (NE) of a white open field (46 × 57 cm with 35 cm high walls), which did not contain shavings, in the brightly-lit testing room. Training in this task utilized two identical objects (plastic toy mouse or bottle) that were placed in the southwest and southeast quadrants of the open field. Mice were allowed to explore these objects for 3 min. The duration of time spent within 5 cm of the object, directly in contact, investigating and/or orienting towards the objects, was automatically recording using a tracking program (Any-Maze; Stoelting Co., Wood Lawn, IL). Immediately, or 1.5 h, after training, mice were injected SC with P4 or vehicle, and were returned to their transport cages. Four hours later, mice were returned to the open field for 3 min. One of the objects used during training was replaced by a colored spherical plastic toy (“orange” or “lemon”). Whether the novel object was on the southeast or southwest side of the open field was alternated to reduce potential confounds of a side preference influencing results. The time mice spent exploring the identical and novel objects was recorded. More time spent exploring the novel object during testing (duration spent with novel object/(duration spent with novel object + duration spent with familiar object) × 100) is considered an index of cognitive performance in this task.

2.10. Object placement

Object placement testing was done in accordance with previous methods employed in our laboratory (Frye & Walf, 2008; Frye et al., 2007). The training methods utilized were the same as described above for object recognition, except that differently shaped plastic objects were utilized. Mice were trained for 3 min and then 4 h later were tested. During testing, one object was displaced from its original location during training to the opposite corner of the open field. Displacement of the novel object to the southeast or southwest side of the open field was alternated to reduce potential confounds of a side preference influencing results. The duration spent by mice exploring the identical and novel objects was recorded. An increase in time spent exploring the displaced object during testing (duration spent with displaced object/(duration spent with displaced object + duration spent with stationary object) × 100) is considered an index of cognitive performance in this task.

2.11. Water maze

The Morris Water Maze (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA; 97 cm in diameter) was used according to previously described methods (Bennett, McRae, Levy, & Frick, 2006; Frye, Rhodes, & Dudek, 2005). Temperature of the water in the maze was 22–25 °C. The water was made opaque with non-toxic, white, tempera paint. Mice were habituated to the maze by allowing them to swim in the water maze with the hidden platform (8 × 8 cm) in it. After 1 min, mice were placed on the hidden platform for 10 s. Following habituation, mice were trained in 12, 1-min trials, which were organized into three blocks of 4 trials. In each block of trials, mice were placed in one of four start positions in each of the quadrants of the maze. The inter-trial interval within each block of trials was 10 s. Each block of trials was separated by 30 min. In each trial, the maximum latency for mice to reach the platform was 60 s. Immediately after training, mice were injected with P4 or vehicle SC. Mice were tested in a cued trial of the water maze to assess their ability to swim to a platform in the maze. In this, the latency of mice to swim to a platform that is made visible and cued is determined to rule out potential differences due to treatment for swimming ability or the ability to perform the task. Twenty-four hours later, the latency of mice to swim to the precise location in the maze where the platform had been during training, and the duration spent in the quadrant where the platform was located during training (probe trial), were recorded. These measures can be utilized as indicators of performance in this task.

2.12. Conditioned contextual/cued fear

Conditioned Fear was modified from previously described methods (Frye, Edinger, & Sumida, 2008). Mice were habituated to the conditioning chamber for 4 min. During training, a tone (10 s) is sounded, followed by an electric foot-shock (2 s, 0.35 mA). Mice receive the tone-shock pairing over 3 training trials. When mice were shocked, their response to this shock was rated (0-no response, 1-twitch, 2-jump, 3-squeak, 4-jump and squeak) and recorded by investigators. Immediately after training, mice were injected with P4 or vehicle SC. Mice were tested 24 h later. During testing, freezing behavior is observed for eight 1-min intervals in the same chamber for contextual fear (mediated by the hippocampus), and in a novel environment with the same tone for cued fear (mediated by the amygdala; Phillips & LeDoux, 1992). More time spent freezing during contextual or cued fear testing trials is utilized as an indication of improved performance in this task.

2.13. Statistical analyses

One-way analyses of variance were utilized to determine effects of hormone condition (vehicle vs. P4) on performance in tasks. A P-value of ≤0.05 was considered significant.

3. Results

3.1. Spontaneous alternation

P4 enhanced performance in the spontaneous alternation T-maze F(1, 22 = 3.99; P ≤ 0.05). Mice treated with P4 made significantly more spontaneous alternations than did mice injected with vehicle (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The mean (± sem) percentage of spontaneous alternations made in the spontaneous alternation task of ovariectomized mice injected with vehicle (veh; open bar; n = 11) or progesterone (P4; closed bar; n = 13) 1 h before testing. * vs. vehicle condition, P < 0.05.

3.2. Object placement

During training, mice spent similar durations investigating each object located on the left (vehicle: 7.3 ± 1.4 s; P4: 8.2 ± 2.6 s) or right (vehicle: 11.0 ± 2.4 s; P4: 16.2 ± 5.5 s) side of the chamber. There were no significant differences on percent of time investigating the displaced object in the object placement task of mice that received injections of vehicle (52.1 ± 10.4% sem) or P4 (52.7 ± 8.1% sem).

3.3. Object recognition

During training, mice spent similar durations investigating each object located on the left (vehicle: 11.7 ± 3.2 s; P4: 9.5 ± 1.6 s) or right (vehicle: 10.5 ± 3.1 s; P4: 11.5 ± 3.9 s) side of the chamber. P4 enhanced performance in the object recognition task F(1, 22 = 4.69; P < 0.04). Mice treated with P4, compared to vehicle, immediately post-training spent significantly more time exploring the novel object during testing in the object recognition task (Fig. 2, left). If mice were injected with P4 1.5 h after training, performance in this task was similar to that observed in vehicle controls (Fig. 2, right).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

The mean (± sem) percentage of time spent exploring a novel object in the object placement task of ovariectomized mice injected with vehicle (veh; open bar; n = 11) or progesterone (P4; closed bar; n = 13) immediately post-training (left panel) or 1.5 h post-training (right panel; n = 8 for each condition). condition, * vs. vehicle P < 0.05.

3.4. Water maze

Differences were not observed during training in the water maze. During training, there were no differences in mean latencies, averaged across 12 training trials, for mice injected with vehicle (33.8 s ± 6.1 sem) or P4 (43.2 s ± 6.4 sem) and see Fig. 3. During testing, P4 enhanced performance in the probe trial of the water maze task. In the probe trial, mice treated with P4 had significantly shorter latencies to the location where the hidden platform had been located during training F(1, 22 = 5.21; P ≤ 0.03), but were not different in the mean time spent in the quadrant where the platform had been during training (25.8 s ± 4.0 sem), than mice treated with vehicle (23.7 s ± 4.4 sem). When the platform was made visible and a cue was used to highlight its location, there were no differences in the latencies to reach the platform of P4 and/or vehicle-treated mice (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

(Top panel) The mean (s + sem) latencies to reach the hidden platform during the training trials of the water maze. (Bottom panel) On the left, the mean (s + sem) latency to reach the location that the hidden platform had been during training in the water maze probe trial of ovariectomized mice injected with vehicle (veh; open bar; n = 11) or progesterone (P4; closed bar; n = 13) immediately post-training is depicted. On the right, the latency to reach the visible platform during the control, cued trials of these mice is depicted. * vs. vehicle condition, P < 0.05.

3.5. Conditioned contextual/cued fear

During training, there were no differences in flinch-jump scores of mice in the vehicle (3.8 ± 0.1 sem) or P4 (3.9 ± 0.1 sem) treatment groups. P4 enhanced performance in the context F(1, 22 = 5.49; P ≤ 0.03) and the cued F(1, 22 = 4.17; P < 0.05) components of the conditioned fear task. Mice injected with P4 spent significantly more time freezing in the same context as they were shocked in. Moreover, when tested in a novel context, but with the same auditory cue that had been associated with shock, mice treated with P4 spent more time freezing than did vehicle-treated mice (Fig. 4, left). If mice were injected with P4 1.5 h after training, performance in the contextual or cued tasks was similar to that observed in mice injected with vehicle (Fig. 4, right).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

The mean (s + sem) duration spent freezing in the contextual (top) and cued (bottom) testing trials of the conditioned fear task of ovariectomized mice injected with vehicle (veh; open bar; n = 11) or progesterone (P4; closed bar; n = 13) immediately post-training (left panel) or 1.5 h post-training (right panel; n = 8 for each condition). * vs. vehicle condition, P < 0.05.

4. Discussion

The hypothesis that acute physiological dosing with P4 would enhance cognitive performance of OVX mice across several tasks was supported. In the spontaneous alternation task, P4 to mice prior to testing increased the number of number of spontaneous alternations made compared to that observed in OVX mice treated with vehicle. In the object placement task, no differences due to P4 treatment were observed. In the object recognition task, mice injected with P4 immediately post-training (but not 1.5 h post-training) performed better than did vehicle controls. In the water maze probe trial, mice that were injected with P4 immediately after training had shorter latencies to swim to the location in the water maze where the hidden platform had been during training, compared to vehicle controls. There were no differences based upon condition for performance in the control, cued trial or, in the probe trial, for duration spent in the quadrant where the platform had been during training. In the conditioned fear task, P4, compared to vehicle, immediately after training (but not 1.5 h later) increased the duration of time spent freezing in response to re-exposure to the training context or when re-exposed to the tone cue that had been paired with footshock during training. Thus, P4 treatment to young, OVX mice improves cognitive performance.

The present data begin to address the nature of P4's effects on cognitive processes. In the present experiment, beneficial effects of P4 were demonstrated when P4 was injected before training in the spontaneous alternation, as well as when P4 was injected immediately post-training in the object recognition, water maze, and conditioned fear task. That post-training treatment of P4 was sufficient to enhance cognitive performance implies P4 may have effects on consolidation. Utilizing a 1 h or 1.5 h delay in post-training treatment with P4, such that P4 does not have effects during consolidation, does not improve object recognition and object placement performance of rats (Frye et al., 2007; Walf et al., 2006). We found a similar pattern of results in the present study. When mice were treated with P4 1.5 h post-training in the object recognition and conditioned fear tasks, no improvements were observed in these tasks when mice were tested 4 h or 24 h post-training. Moreover, beneficial effects of P4 were observed when P4 was circulating (i.e. 1 h after injections in the spontaneous alternation task, 4 h after priming in the object recognition task) and when at nadir (i.e. 24 h following injections in the water maze and conditioned fear tasks). We have previously found that this regimen of P4 in mice produces progestogen levels similar to those observed during behavioral estrus within 1 h that are sustained for 4–8 h, and reach nadir 24 h after treatment (Frye et al., 2006a, 2006b). Together, these data suggest that the beneficial effects of P4 are not limited to situations in which there are increases in circulating concentrations of P4, and instead suggest that they may be attributable to P4's modulation of memory consolidation.

The present findings extend the existing literature on the potential for progestogens to have mnemonic effects to demonstrate that acute, physiological P4 priming has mnemonic effects in mice. For instance, in a study using adult, intact cycling mice, performance in a spatial reference memory task was better during estrus, than metestrus, when endogenous steroid hormone levels are greater (Frick & Berger-Sweeney, 2001). A recent study from our laboratory demonstrated that chronic P4 treatment improves cognitive performance of mid-aged OVX mice. Mice were OVX at 6 months of age and then implanted with SC pellets of P4 or placebo between 6 and 12 months of age. Chronic P4 in these mice improved performance in the spontaneous alternation and object recognition tasks, than placebo implants (Frye & Walf, 2008). Similar to some of the present results, effects of P4 in the object placement task were less clear and no effects of P4 were observed in the water maze in this study. Another consideration to make in interpreting the results of the present study is whether P4 is altering the strategies utilized by mice in these cognitive tasks. Although this was not systematically investigated or manipulated in the present study, shorter latencies to the location of the hidden platform during the probe trial in mice injected with P4, and no differences between groups for time spent in the quadrant, suggest that P4-treated mice may have oriented towards the platform location faster or more reliably. Future studies could assess this directly. Other factors to consider is how the observed behavioral effects may be related to its capacity to protect the brain from damage (Gonzalez Deniselle et al., 2002; Jiang, Chopp, Stein, & Feit, 1996; Stein, 2008; Thomas, Nockels, Pan, Shaffrey, & Chopp, 1999; Yu, 1989), or promote neurological and functional recovery after damage (Frye, Rhodes, Walf, & Harney, 2002; Rhodes, McCormick, & Frye, 2004; Stein, 2008; Thomas et al., 1999; Yu, 1989). Indeed, the beneficial effects of chronic P4 are observed in a murine model of early-onset Alzheimer's Disease, in which there is overexpression of mutations of amyloid precursor protein and presenilin (Frye & Walf, 2008). Together, these data, in addition to those from the present study, suggest that mice can be responsive to the cognitive-enhancing effects of physiological dosing with acute pre- or post-training P4 and/or chronic P4. However, it must be noted that mice were repeatedly exposed to P4 (or vehicle) for five weeks. Although this type of P4 exposure may approximate what is observed for mice across endogenous estrous cycles, the cumulative effects of P4 on cell morphology or other structural changes in the brain need to be addressed in the future.

Although these data suggest that physiological levels during consolidation, rather than testing, may be critical for enhancing performance in some cognitive tasks, other factors in interpreting these results need to be considered. The beneficial effects of chronic P4 were not observed across all tasks utilized, suggesting that there may be some site specificity of P4's effects. Although some lesion studies suggest that brain areas, such as the hippocampus, cortex, and/or amygdala, may contribute to performance in specific tasks utilized (Ennaceur, Neave, & Aggleton, 1997; Gerlai, 1998; Lee, Jerman, & Kesner, 2005; Morris, Garrud, Rawlins, & O'Keefe, 1982; Phillips & LeDoux, 1992; but also see Gerlai, McNamara, Williams, & Phillips, 2002; Mumby, 2001), whether there is site specificity of P4's actions in these regions needs to be addressed directly in future studies. Additionally, the present data that P4 improved performance in both the contextual and cued test trials of the conditioned fear task suggest that P4 has actions in the hippocampus and amygdala for cognitive performance. Progestogens have actions through the amygdala and hippocampus to attenuate fear and anxiety (Akwa, Purdy, Koob, & Britton, 1999; Bitran, Foley, Audette, Leslie, & Frye, 2000; Frye & Walf, 2004). This provokes the question about whether the cognitive-enhancing effects of P4 are indirect and associated with changes in arousal (Pfaff, Martin, & Ribeiro, 2007). Evidence from the present study suggests that all effects observed were not due to changes in general arousal, altering emotional learning, and/or affect. First, we found that P4 improved performance in the water maze and conditioned fear, 24 h after P4 was injected, which is a time when P4 and other progestogen levels should be at nadir. We have previously found that cognitive performance is enhanced, and affective behavior is not changed, 24 h after OVX rats are injected with P4 SC (Frye & Lacey, 2000). As such, the cognitive-enhancing effects of progestogens can occur independent of demonstrable changes in affective behavior. Second, delay in P4 treatment following training did not enhance performance of mice when they were tested 4 h post-training when P4 would be in circulation. Third, in relation to how repeated exposure of mice to P4 may have produce cumulative effects that may be related to changes in affect, we did not see differences in flinch-jump ratings (i.e. their responsiveness to footshock) of mice during training in the conditioned fear task. Thus, these data suggest that P4 may have mnemonic effects.

The present findings that P4 can have mnemonic effects in mice are intriguing in light of potential implications for hormone-replacement therapies. Hormone-replacement therapies are used by some women to promote successful aging. E2-based hormone-replacement therapies may protect against age-related cognitive decline and/or decrease the risk, or delay the onset of, neurodegenerative disorders (Asthana et al., 1999, 2001; Henderson et al., 2000; Paganini-Hill & Henderson, 1994, 1996; Phillips & Sherwin, 1992b). Recently, re-analyses of data from the Women's Health Initiative (WHI) trials demonstrated that beneficial effects of E2 may be limited to the perimenopausal period and that older women, who are long past menopause, can experience detrimental effects of E2-based hormone-replacement therapies (Sherwin, 2007). Notably, it is a standard practice for progestogens to be given to menopausal women with E2-based hormone-replacement therapies to reduce the risk of cancers of reproductive tissue (Sitruk-Ware, 2002, 2007). As such, it is important to understand the capacity for progestogens, apart from effects of E2 or effects of these steroids in conjunction, to influence cognitive processes and/or have neurotrophic effects. Although it is likely that E2 and P4 may produce dissimilar cognitive effects depending upon the task utilized, in addition to other factors, we have found that physiological dosing with E2 and/or P4 to OVX rats produces similar enhancements in the object recognition task (Walf et al., 2006). In our laboratory, we are interested in determining whether formation, and subsequent actions, of the P4 metabolite and neurosteroid, allopregnanolone, may underlie these similar beneficial effects of E2 and P4 treatment. Indeed, P4 is a precursor of allopregnanolone and E2 can increase allopregnanolone by enhancing activity of rate-limiting enzymes required for allopregnanolone formation (Cheng & Karavolas, 1973; Frye & Rhodes, 2005). This possibility is currently being investigated in our laboratory.

In summary, the present results demonstrate that pre-training P4 treatment increased spontaneous alternations. Post-training P4 injections improved probe trial performance in the water maze when mice were tested 24 h after treatment. Mice injected with P4 immediately post-training had improved performance in the object recognition task and the cued and contextual conditioned fear tasks. These performance improvements were not observed if there was a delay in post-training P4 treatment. These data suggest that physiological P4 during consolidation enhances cognitive performance of young, OVX mice. The present data that P4 has mnemonic effects in an in vivo mouse model enables us to begin to further investigate the nature and mechanisms of P4's effects for learning and memory. This basic research is sorely needed for its clinical relevance in the etiology and/or therapeutic treatment of age-related cognitive sequelae.

References

  1. Akwa Y, Purdy RH, Koob T, Britton KT. The amygdala mediates the anxiolytic-like effect of the neurosteroid allopregnanolone in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 1999;106:119–125. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00101-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Asthana S, Baker LD, Craft S, Stanczyk FZ, Veith RC, Raskind MA, et al. High-dose estradiol improves cognition for women with AD: Results of a randomized study. Neurology. 2001;57:605–612. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.4.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Asthana S, Craft S, Baker LD, Raskind MA, Birnbaum RS, Lofgreen CP, et al. Cognitive and neuroendocrine response to transdermal estrogen in postmenopausal women with Alzheimer's disease: Results of a placebo-controlled, double-blind, pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24:657–677. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(99)00020-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Azcoitia I, Doncarlos LL, Garcia-Segura LM. Estrogen and brain vulnerability. Neurotoxicology Research. 2003;4:235–245. doi: 10.1080/10298420290033232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bennett JC, McRae PA, Levy LJ, Frick KM. Long-term continuous, but not daily, environmental enrichment reduces spatial memory decline in aged male mice. Neurobiology Learning Memory. 2006;85:139–152. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2005.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berman KF, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR, Danaceau MA, Van Horn JD, Esposito G, et al. Modulation of cognition-specific cortical activity by gonadal steroids: A positron-emission tomography study in women. Proceedings from the National Academy of Science. 1997;94:8836–8841. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.16.8836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bisagno V, Bowman R, Luine V. Functional aspects of estrogen neuroprotection. Endocrinology. 2003;21:33–41. doi: 10.1385/endo:21:1:33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bitran D, Foley M, Audette D, Leslie N, Frye CA. Activation of peripheral mitochondrial benzodiazepine receptors in the hippocampus stimulates allopregnanolone synthesis and produces anxiolytic-like effects in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 2000;151:64–71. doi: 10.1007/s002130000471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brinton RD. Impact of estrogen therapy on Alzheimer's disease: A fork in the road? Central Nervous System Drugs. 2004;18:405–422. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200418070-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Broverman DM, Vogel W, Klaiber EL, Majcher D, Shea D, Paul V. Changes in cognitive task performance across the menstrual cycle. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology. 1981;95:646–654. doi: 10.1037/h0077796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cheng YJ, Karavolas H. Conversion of progesterone to 5α-pregnane-3,20-dione and 3 α-hydroxy-5α-pregnan-20-one by rat medical basal hypothalami and the effects of estradiol and stage of estrous cycle on the conversion. Endocrinology. 1973;93:1157–1162. doi: 10.1210/endo-93-5-1157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ennaceur A, Delacour J. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behavioural Brain Research. 1988;31:47–59. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90157-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ennaceur A, Neave N, Aggleton JP. Spontaneous object recognition and object location memory in rats: The effects of lesions in the cingulate cortices, the medial prefrontal cortex, the cingulum bundle and the fornix. Experimental Brain Research. 1997;113:509–519. doi: 10.1007/pl00005603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fillit H, Luine V. The neurobiology of gonadal hormones and cognitive decline in late life. Maturitas. 1997;26:159–164. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5122(97)01101-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Frick KM, Berger-Sweeney J. Spatial reference memory and neocortical neurochemistry vary with the estrous cycle in C57BL/6 mice. Behavioural Neuroscience. 2001;115:229–237. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Frick KM, Stillner ET, Berger-Sweeney J. Mice are not little rats: Species differences in a one-day water maze task. NeuroReport. 2000:113461–113465. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200011090-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Frye CA. Estrus-associated decrements in a water maze task are limited to acquisition. Physiology and Behavior. 1995;57:5–14. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00197-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Frye CA, Duffy CK, Walf AA. Estrogens and progestins enhance spatial learning of intact and ovariectomized rats in the object placement task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2007;88:208–216. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Frye CA, Edinger K, Sumida K. Androgen administration to aged male mice increases anti-anxiety behavior and enhances cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1049–1061. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frye CA, Lacey EH. Progestins influence performance on cognitive tasks independent of changes in affective behavior. Psychobiology. 2000;28:550–563. [Google Scholar]
  21. Frye CA, Rhodes ME. Estrogen-priming can enhance progesterone's anti-seizure effects in part by increasing hippocampal levels of allopregnanolone. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2005;8:907–916. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Frye CA, Rhodes ME, Dudek B. Estradiol to aged female or male mice improves learning in inhibitory avoidance and water maze tasks. Brain Research. 2005;1036:101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Frye CA, Rhodes ME, Walf A, Harney J. Progesterone reduces pentylenetetrazol-induced ictal activity of wild-type mice but not those deficient in type I 5α-reductase. Epilepsia. 2002;43:14–17. doi: 10.1046/j.1528-1157.43.s.5.19.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Frye CA, Sumida K, Dudek BC, Harney JP, Lydon JP, O'Malley BW, et al. Progesterone's effects to reduce anxiety behavior of aged mice do not require actions via intracellular progestin receptors. Psychopharmacology. 2006a;186:312–322. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0309-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Frye CA, Sumida K, Lydon JP, O'Malley BW, Pfaff DW. Mid-aged and aged wild-type and progestin receptor knockout (PRKO) mice demonstrate rapid progesterone and 3α,5α-THP-facilitated lordosis. Psychopharmacology. 2006b;185:423–432. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0300-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Frye CA, Walf AA. Estrogen and/or progesterone administered systemically or to the amygdala can have anxiety-, fear-, and pain-reducing effects in ovariectomized rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004;118:306–313. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Frye CA, Walf A. Effects of progesterone administration and APPswe+PSEN1Δe9 mutation for cognitive performance of mid-aged mice. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2008;89:17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Galea LA. Gonadal hormone modulation of neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of adult male and female rodents. Brain Research Reviews. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.05.008. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gerlai R. A new continuous alternation task in T-maze detects hippocampal dysfunction in mice. A strain comparison and lesion study. Behavioral Brain Research. 1998;95:91–101. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00214-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gerlai RT, McNamara A, Williams S, Phillips HS. Hippocampal dysfunction and behavioral deficit in the water maze in mice: An unresolved issue? Brain Research Bulletin. 2002;57:3–9. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00630-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gibbs RB, Gabor R. Estrogen and cognition: Applying preclinical findings to clinical perspectives. Journal of Neuroscience Research. 2003;74:637–643. doi: 10.1002/jnr.10811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gibbs RB. Effects of ageing and long-term hormone replacement on cholinergic neurones in the medial septum and nucleus basalis magnocellularis of ovariectomized rats. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2003;15:477–485. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.01012.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gonzalez Deniselle MC, Lopez Costa JJ, Gonzalez SL, Labombarda F, Garay L, Guennoun R, et al. Basis of progesterone protection in spinal cord neurodegeneration. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology. 2002;83:199–209. doi: 10.1016/s0960-0760(02)00262-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hampson E. Estrogen-related variations in human spatial and articulatory-motor skills. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1990;15:97–111. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(90)90018-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hampson E, Kimura D. Reciprocal effects of hormonal fluctuations on human motor and perceptual-spatial skills. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1988;102:456–459. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.102.3.456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hausmann M, Slabbekoorn D, Van Goozen SH, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Gunturkun O. Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2000;114:1245–1250. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.6.1245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Henderson VW, Paganini-Hill A, Miller BL, Elble RJ, Reyes PF, Shoupe D, et al. Estrogen for Alzheimer's disease in women: Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Neurology. 2000;54:295–301. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.2.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hollander A, Hausmann M, Hamm JP, Corballis MC. Sex hormonal modulation of hemispheric asymmetries in the attentional blink. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2005;11:263–272. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hughes RN. The value of spontaneous alternation behavior (SAB) as a test of retention in pharmacological investigations of memory. Neuroscience Biobehavioural Reviews. 2004;28:497–505. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jiang N, Chopp M, Stein D, Feit H. Progesterone is neuroprotective after transient middle cerebral artery occlusion in male rats. Brain Research. 1996;735:101–107. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00605-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Johansson IM, Birzniece V, Lindblad C, Olsson T, Backstrom T. Allopregnanolone inhibits learning in the Morris water maze. Brain Research. 2002;934:125–131. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)02414-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lee I, Jerman TS, Kesner RP. Disruption of delayed memory for a sequence of spatial locations following CA1- or CA3-lesions of the dorsal hippocampus. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2005;84:138–147. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2005.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Luine VN, Jacome LF, Maclusky NJ. Rapid enhancement of visual and place memory by estrogens in rats. Endocrinology. 2003;144:2836–2844. doi: 10.1210/en.2003-0004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Maki PM, Rich JB, Rosenbaum RS. Implicit memory varies across the menstrual cycle: Estrogen effects in young women. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:518–529. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00126-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McCormick CM, Teillon SM. Menstrual cycle variation in spatial ability: Relation to salivary cortisol levels. Hormones and Behavior. 2001;39:29–38. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McEwen BS. Invited review: Estrogens effects on the brain: Multiple sites and molecular mechanisms. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2001;91:2785–2801. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.91.6.2785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O'Keefe J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 1982;297:681–683. doi: 10.1038/297681a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Young NB, Perez A, Holloway LP, Barbaro RP, et al. Mouse behavioral tasks relevant to autism: Phenotypes of 10 inbred strains. Behavioural Brain Research. 2007;176:4–20. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.07.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mumby DG. Perspectives on object-recognition memory following hippocampal damage: Lessons from studies in rats. Behavioral Brain Research. 2001;127:159–181. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00367-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Paganini-Hill A, Henderson VW. Estrogen deficiency and risk of Alzheimer's disease in women. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1994;140:256–261. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a117244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Paganini-Hill A, Henderson VW. The effects of hormone replacement therapy, lipoprotein cholesterol levels, and other factors on a clock drawing task in older women. Journal of the American Geriatric Society. 1996;44:818–822. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1996.tb03740.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pfaff DW, Martin EM, Ribeiro AC. Relations between mechanisms of CNS arousal and mechanisms of stress. Stress. 2007;10:316–325. doi: 10.1080/10253890701638030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1992;106:274–285. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Phillips SM, Sherwin BB. Variations in memory function and sex steroid hormones across the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1992a;17:497–506. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(92)90008-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Phillips SM, Sherwin BB. Effects of estrogen on memory function in surgically menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1992b;17:485–495. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(92)90007-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Phillips K, Silverman I. Differences in the relationship of menstrual cycle phase to spatial performance on two- and three-dimensional tasks. Hormones and Behavior. 1997;3:167–175. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1997.1418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rhodes ME, McCormick CM, Frye CA. 3α,5α-THP mediates progestins' effects to protect against adrenalectomy-induced cell death in the dentate gyrus of female and male rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2004;78:505–512. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rosenberg L, Park S. Verbal and spatial functions across the menstrual cycle in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27:835–841. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00083-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sandstrom NJ, Williams CL. Memory retention is modulated by acute estradiol and progesterone replacement. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2001;115:384–393. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Schumacher M, Akwa Y, Guennoun R, Robert F, Labombarda F, Desarnaud F, et al. Steroid synthesis and metabolism in the nervous system: Trophic and protective effects. Journal of Neurocytology. 2000;29:307–326. doi: 10.1023/a:1007152904926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sherwin BB. The clinical relevance of the relationship between estrogen and cognition in women. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology. 2007;106:151–156. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2007.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sitruk-Ware R. New hormonal therapies and regimens in the postmenopause: Routes of administration and timing of initiation. Climacteric. 2007;10:358–370. doi: 10.1080/13697130701609121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sitruk-Ware R. Progestogens in hormonal replacement therapy: New molecules, risks, and benefits. Menopause. 2002;9:6–15. doi: 10.1097/00042192-200201000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Solis-Ortiz S, Guevara MA, Corsi-Cabrera M. Performance in a test demanding prefrontal functions is favored by early luteal phase progesterone: An electroencephalographic study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:1047–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stackman RW, Blasberg ME, Langan CJ, Clark AS. Stability of spatial working memory across the estrous cycle of long-evans rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1997;67:167–171. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.3753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stein DG. Progesterone exerts neuroprotective effects after brain injury. Brain Research Reviews. 2008;57:386–397. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Tanapat P, Hastings NB, Gould E. Ovarian steroids influence cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus of the adult female rat in a dose- and time-dependent manner. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2005;481:252–265. doi: 10.1002/cne.20385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Thomas AJ, Nockels RP, Pan HQ, Shaffrey CI, Chopp M. Progesterone is neuroprotective after acute experimental spinal cord trauma in rats. Spine. 1999;24:2134–2138. doi: 10.1097/00007632-199910150-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Walf AA, Rhodes ME, Frye CA. Ovarian steroids enhance object recognition in naturally cycling and ovariectomized, hormone-primed rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2006;86:35–46. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wood GE, Beylin AV, Shors TJ. The contribution of adrenal and reproductive hormones to the opposing effects of stress on trace conditioning in males versus females. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2001;115:175–187. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Yu WH. Survival of motoneurons following axotomy is enhanced by lactation or by progesterone treatment. Brain Research. 1989;491:379–382. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)90075-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Zou LB, Yamada K, Sasa M, Nakata Y, Nabeshima T. Effects of sigma(1) receptor agonist SA4503 and neuroactive steroids on performance in a radial arm maze task in rats. Neuropharmacology. 2000;39:1617–1627. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(99)00228-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES