Skip to main content
Journal of Neurophysiology logoLink to Journal of Neurophysiology
. 2008 Aug 27;100(5):2486–2495. doi: 10.1152/jn.90397.2008

Postnatal Development of Disparity Sensitivity in Visual Area 2 (V2) of Macaque Monkeys

I Maruko 1, B Zhang 1, X Tao 1, J Tong 1, E L Smith III 1, Y M Chino 1
PMCID: PMC2585398  PMID: 18753321

Abstract

Macaque monkeys do not reliably discriminate binocular depth cues until about 8 wk of age. The neural factors that limit the development of fine depth perception in primates are not known. In adults, binocular depth perception critically depends on detection of relative binocular disparities and the earliest site in the primate visual brain where a substantial proportion of neurons are capable of discriminating relative disparity is visual area 2 (V2). We examined the disparity sensitivity of V2 neurons during the first 8 wk of life in infant monkeys and compared the responses of V2 neurons to those of V1 neurons. We found that the magnitude of response modulation in V2 and V1 neurons as a function of interocular spatial phase disparity was adult-like as early as 2 wk of age. However, the optimal spatial frequency and binocular response rate of these disparity sensitive neurons were more than an octave lower in 2- and 4-wk-old infants than in adults. Consequently, despite the lower variability of neuronal firing in V2 and V1 neurons of infant monkeys, the ability of these neurons to discriminate fine disparity differences was significantly reduced compared with adults. This reduction in disparity sensitivity of V2 and V1 neurons is likely to limit binocular depth perception during the first several weeks of a monkey's life.

INTRODUCTION

In macaque monkeys, stereopsis is absent during the first 2–3 postnatal wk and binocular disparities are not reliably detected until ∼6–8 wk of age (O'Dell and Boothe 1997). Similarly human infants do not show stereopsis until ∼4–6 mo of age (Birch 1993; Birch et al. 1983; Brown et al. 2007). The neural factors that constrain the early development of binocular depth perception have not been extensively studied. The ability of the primary visual cortex (V1) neurons to process local disparity information is thought to be a fundamental requirement for stereopsis and is likely to put some limits on performance (Cumming and DeAngelis 2001; Nienborg et al. 2004; Prince et al. 2000). Previously, we found that an adult-like proportion of V1 neurons is sensitive to interocular spatial phase disparity only 6 days after birth (Chino et al. 1997), indicating that the basic binocular connections in V1 required for stereoscopic vision are present near birth.

It is becoming increasingly clear, however, that in adult monkeys, V1 neurons alone could not directly support binocular depth perception (Cumming and Parker 1999; Read et al. 2002). Instead, extrastriate neurons that are sensitive to relative binocular disparity are thought to more directly underlie fine depth perception (Parker 2007; Roe et al. 2007 for reviews). Thus an emerging view on binocular vision development is that immaturities in cortical neurons beyond V1, yet to be discovered, are likely to be involved in limiting stereopsis in neonates (Chino et al. 2004; Kiorpes and Movshon 2004; O'Dell and Boothe 1997). Consistent with this view, several lines of evidence suggest that the overall functional maturation of extrastriate visual areas appears to proceed at a slower pace relative to V1 (Batardiere et al. 2002; Distler et al. 1996; Kiorpes and Movshon 2004; Zhang et al. 2005; Zheng et al. 2007).

In adults, V2 is the earliest site beyond V1 where a substantial proportion of neurons are capable of detecting relative binocular disparity (Thomas et al. 2002). Also, a link between the disparity sensitivity of cortical neurons in individual monkeys and their ability to discriminate depth cues can be established by quantifying their choice-related activity in these neurons while the monkeys perform a discrimination task. Disparity sensitive V2 neurons, but not V1 neurons, have been shown to exhibit “choice-related” activity (Nienborg and Cumming 2006). However, it is not known whether V2 neurons of infant monkeys are sensitive to relative binocular disparity or whether disparity selective V2 neurons in infants show choice-related activity. This is simply because, at these early ages, it is not feasible to conduct microelectrode recording experiments in awake behaving monkeys. In this study therefore we estimated the ability of V2 and V1 neurons to discriminate fine disparity differences by analyzing their sensitivity to binocular phase disparities, their optimal spatial frequencies, and their response amplitudes and variabilities during the first several postnatal weeks (Nienborg et al. 2004). We found that, although the abilities of V2 neurons to combine binocular signals and to detect interocular spatial phase disparities were qualitatively adult like as early as 14 days after birth, the median disparity threshold of V2 neurons was >4 times higher in 2- and 4-wk-old infants than in adults. We also found that relative changes in the median disparity threshold for V2 and V1 neurons between 2 and 8 wk of age paralleled behavioral improvement in stereoacuity (O'Dell and Boothe 1997).

METHODS

Microelectrode recording experiments were conducted in anesthetized and paralyzed monkeys (Macaca mulatta). All experimental procedures conformed to the National Institute of Health guidelines for the use of animals in research and were approved by the University of Houston's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Subjects

Five 2-wk-old, four 4-wk-old, and three 8-wk-old infant monkeys and four adult monkeys served as subjects. The weights of the infant monkeys varied between 480 and 600 g at 2 wk, between 500 and 525 g at 4 wk, and between 550 and 750 g at 8 wk of age. Some results on monocular response properties of cortical neurons from these monkeys have been previously reported (Zhang et al. 2007; Zheng et al. 2007).

Preparation

The surgical preparation and recording procedures have been described in detail elsewhere (Chino et al. 1997; Zhang et al. 2005; Zheng et al. 2007). Briefly, the monkeys were anesthetized initially with an intramuscular injection of ketamine hydrochloride (15–20 mg/kg) and acepromazine maleate (0.15–0.2 mg/kg). After all surgical procedures were completed, the animals were paralyzed by an intravenous injection of vercuronium bromide (Norcuron; 0.1 mg/kg/h) and artificially ventilated with a mixture of 59% N2O, 39% O2, and 2% CO2. Anesthesia was maintained by the continuous infusion of a mixture of propofol (4 mg/kg/h) and sufentanyl citrate (0.05 μg/kg/h). Core body temperature was kept at 37.6°C. Cycloplegia was produced by topical instillation of 1% atropine, and the animals' corneas were protected with rigid gas permeable, extended-wear contact lenses. Retinoscopy was used to determine the contact lens parameters required to focus the eyes on the stimulus screens. Additional spectacle lenses were also used if necessary.

Recording and visual stimulation

Tungsten-in-glass microelectrodes (FHC, Bowdoinham. ME) were used to record and isolate the activity from individual cortical neurons. A typical penetration in V1 began several millimeters posterior to the lunate sulcus and ∼1.5 cm from the midline and ended when the electrode tip entered the white matter. The tangential penetrations in V2 (the angle of deviations from perpendicular was ∼20°) were typically started right behind the blood vessels running along the lunate sulcus and also ∼1.5 cm from the midline. The penetration ended when the electrode tip exited V2. All receptive fields were located within 5.0° of the center of the projected fovea.

For each isolated neuron, the receptive field for each eye was mapped, and its ocular dominance was initially determined using hand heldstimuli (Hubel and Wiesel 1962). Responses to drifting sine wave gratings (3.1 Hz, 80% contrast) were measured for a broad range of stimulus orientation and spatial frequency from which the preferred orientations, direction of stimulus drift, and spatial frequencies were determined for each unit. The visual stimuli for these experiments were generated on a monochrome monitor (VRG) with ultra-short persistence (frame rate = 140 Hz; 800 × 600 pixels, screen size = 20 × 15° at 114 cm and mean luminance = 50 cd/m2). Recorded action potentials were digitized at 25 kHz and sampled at a rate of 140 Hz (7.14-ms bin widths) and compiled into peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) that were equal in duration to, and synchronized with, the temporal cycle of the grating (TDT data acquisition system; TD). Cells were classified as simple or complex on the basis of the temporal characteristics of their responses to a drifting sine wave grating of the optimal spatial frequency and orientation (Skottun et al. 1991).

Measurements of orientation, spatial frequency, and disparity tuning functions

ORIENTATION TUNING.

The preferred orientation and orientation bandwidth for each receptive field were determined by fitting the orientation tuning functions with wrapped Gaussian functions (Fig. 1A) (Swindale 1998)

graphic file with name M1.gif

where θ = orientation, m1 = the response rate, m2 = the preferred orientation, and m3 = the SD of the Gaussian function.

FIG. 1.

FIG. 1.

Methods used to measure the disparity sensitivity of a visual area 2 (V2) neuron. A: determination of the preferred orientation from its orientation tuning function fitted with a wrapped Gaussian function. B: determination of optimal spatial frequency and spatial resolution of the unit after fitting its spatial frequency response function with a Gaussian function. C: disparity tuning function of the V2 neuron. Binocular response amplitude was plotted as a function of interocular spatial phase disparity. BII, binocular interaction index; DDI, disparity discrimination index; peak B/M, peak binocular response amplitude/monocular response amplitude. Note that the calculation of these values was performed after taking the square root of firing rates. Dotted line indicates the mean binocular response amplitude. Filled triangle, dominant monocular amplitude; open triangle, nondominant monocular amplitude; asterisk, spontaneous activity.

SPATIAL FREQUENCY TUNING.

To determine each cell's optimal spatial frequency and spatial resolution, the spatial frequency response data were fitted with Gaussian functions (Fig. 1B) (DeAngelis et al. 1993)

graphic file with name M2.gif

where m0 = spatial frequency, m1 = the response rate, m2 = the optimal spatial frequency, and m3 = the SD of the Gaussian function. The spatial resolution for each unit was determined by locating the highest spatial frequency that evoked responses which were significantly higher than the cell's average spontaneous firing rate (i.e., more than ±2 SD).

BINOCULAR INTERACTIONS INDEX.

To determine the strength and the nature of binocular interactions, responses were collected for dichoptic sine wave gratings of the optimal spatial frequency and orientation as a function of the relative interocular spatial phase disparity of the grating pair (Fig. 1C). The sensitivity to relative interocular spatial phase disparities was quantified using a binocular interaction index (BII) that was calculated from the sine function fit to the binocular phase tuning data (Ohzawa and Freeman 1986a,b; Prince et al. 2002b; Smith et al. 1997). BII here is defined as

graphic file with name M3.gif

where Rmax is the largest response on the tuning function, and Rmin is the smallest response. These values were obtained from fit functions.

DISPARITY DISCRIMINATION INDEX.

To take effects of variability in neuronal firing and firing rates of individual cells into account, we also calculated the disparity discrimination index (DDI) (Prince et al. 2002b; Uka and DeAngelis 2003)

graphic file with name M4.gif

where Rmax is the largest response on the tuning function, and Rmin is the smallest response. RMSerror is the square root of the residual variance around the mean across the entire tuning function.

To characterize whether binocular signal interactions were facilitatory or suppressive in nature, the peak binocular response/dominant monocular response ratios (Peak B/M ratios) were calculated for each unit and expressed in terms of relative strength (db), i.e., 10 log Peak B/M. Negative Peak B/M values signify binocular suppression and positive values indicate binocular facilitation. For the calculation of BII and DDI, the Peak B/M ratio was performed on the square root of firing rate (Prince et al. 2002b; Uka and DeAngelis 2003).

HISTOLOGY.

At the end of each penetration, small electrolytic lesions (5 μA, 5 s, electrode negative) were made at three points along the track for later reconstruction. Experiments were terminated by administering an overdose of pentobarbital sodium (100 mg/kg), and the animals were killed by perfusion through the heart with an aldehyde fixtative. Frozen sections were stained for Nissl substance and cytochrome oxidase. The laminar distribution of individual units was estimated from the recording depths and the histologically identified electrode tracks. Our sampling frequency was generally uniform and similar in all subject groups.

RESULTS

We recorded responses from a total of 788 neurons (158 simple and 361 complex in V2 and 78 simple and 191 complex in V1) and quantitatively analyzed the binocular and monocular response properties of 104 V2 and 78 V1 neurons in 2-wk-old infants, 84 V2 and 48 V1 neurons in 4-wk-old infants, 138 V2 and 63 V1 neurons in 8-wk-old infants, and 193 V2 and 80 V1 neurons in adult monkeys. Because we did not find major differences between simple and complex cells or laminar differences in any of the response measures that we studied, we combined data in these cell types from all cortical layers for the subsequent analyses. Statistical significance was tested for group differences with Kruskal-Wallis test for median values, unless specified otherwise.

Binocular signal combination in V2 is adult-like at 2 wk of age

One of the most efficient and highly sensitive ways to assess the strength and characteristics of binocular signal combination in a visual cortical neuron is to measure its BII (Chino et al. 1997; Ohzawa and Freeman 1986a,b; Prince et al. 2002b; Smith et al. 1997). As we have previously reported for V1 neurons (Chino et al. 1997) and as can be seen in Fig. 2, the response modulation of the representative V2 neuron from a 2-wk-old infant as a function of interocular spatial phase disparity was as robust as those in older infants and adults; and the calculated BII values were also comparable at all ages. However, it is important to note that the mean binocular response rates (dotted lines) for these representative units from infants were substantially lower than those from adults.

FIG. 2.

FIG. 2.

Examples of binocular spatial phase tuning functions of V2 (A–D) and V1 neurons (E and F) of 2-, 4-, 8-wk-old infants and adults. Plotting conventions are same as in Fig. 1C. Note that the disparity tuning function of the V2 neuron at 2 wk of age was very similar to that for the adult unit except its low mean binocular response rate.

The population analysis of disparity tuning functions showed that, in V2, the median BII values were 0.18, 0.16, and 0.19 in 2-, 4- and 8-wk-old infants, respectively, and were not statistically different from the median BII value in adults (0.18) (P > 0.01; Fig. 3, left column). The median BII values in V1 also did not change over age (P > 0.01; Fig. 3, right column). Moreover, the proportion of disparity-sensitive units, defined as neurons that exhibited a statistically significant response modulation as a function of phase disparity (Prince et al. 2002b), was similar for all the infant groups and adults (P > 0.01, χ2 test; Fig. 3, filled bars). Note that the frequency distribution of the BII in V1 of our adult monkeys was similar to that previously reported for awake behaving adult monkeys (Prince et al. 2002b).

FIG. 3.

FIG. 3.

Population data for disparity tuning functions in infants and adults. Open histograms, the distribution of BII values for all V2 and V1 neurons in infants and adults; filled histograms, the distribution of V2 and V1 neurons that had statistically significant disparity tuning, i.e., disparity sensitive units (1-way ANOVA, P < 0.05, Prince et al. 2002a). Mean (±SE) and median values are indicated by triangles and circles, respectively. Note that the calculation of the BII values was performed after taking the square root of firing rates.

The BII of a give neuron does not take the variability of firing into account and tends to be negatively correlated with the mean firing rate (Prince et al. 2002b). Because V2 and V1 neurons in our infant macaques showed much lower firing rates than in adults (Chino et al. 1997; see also Fig. 6), BII values of 2- and 4-wk-old infants might have been artificially elevated and might have not reflected the credible estimate of neuron's sensitivity to binocular disparity at these early ages. To examine this possibility, we calculated the DDI (Prince et al. 2002b; Uka and DeAngelis 2003). The frequency distribution of DDI values in our adult V1 (Fig. 4) was very similar to that in awake adult monkeys (Prince et al. 2002b). More importantly, the distribution and the median DDI values in V2 and V1 neurons of all infant groups were not significantly different from those in adults (P > 0.01; Fig. 4).

FIG. 4.

FIG. 4.

Developmental changes in disparity tuning functions. Open histograms, the distribution of DDI values for V2 and V1 neurons in infants and adults; filled histograms, the distribution of DDI values for V2 and V1 neurons that had statistically significant disparity tuning (1-way ANOVA, P < 0.05, Prince et al. 2002a). Mean (±SE) and median values are indicated by triangles and circles, respectively. Note that the calculation of the DDI values was performed after taking the square root of firing rates.

The nature of binocular signal interactions (i.e., excitatory vs. inhibitory) in V2 and V1 neurons were examined by taking the ratio of the peak binocular response over the dominant monocular response, and the resulting values were expressed in terms of relative strengths (db), i.e., 10 log Peak B/M. Although the median B/M value (0.45 db) at 2 wk of age was considerably lower than that in adults (0.76 db), the difference did not reach statistical significance (P > 0.01; Fig. 5). A similar difference was found for V1 neurons at 2 wk of age (0.33 db in infants vs. 0.56 db in adults; P > 0.01). The median ratios for 4- and 8-wk-old-infants (0.83 and 0.52 db in V2, 0.69 and 0.39db in V1) were not different from the corresponding ratios in adults (P > 0.01). The overall distribution of Peak B/M ratios for V1 neurons in our anesthetized and paralyzed adults were similar to the distribution of binocular/monocular response ratios of V1 neurons found in awake behaving adult monkeys (Prince et al. 2002a).

FIG. 5.

FIG. 5.

Frequency histograms showing the distribution of peak binocular response over monocular response ratios of V2 and V1 neurons in infants and adults. Mean (±SE) and median values are indicated by triangles. The dotted lines indicate the border between excitatory (>0.0 db) and inhibitory (<0.0 db) binocular interactions. Note that the calculation of the peak binocular/monocular ratios was performed after taking the square root of firing rates.

Firing rates, response variability, and optimal spatial frequency are low in infants

The ability of individual V1 neurons to discriminate fine disparity differences depends on a cell's optimal spatial frequency, discharge rate, and response variability (Nakatsuka et al. 2007; Nienborg et al. 2004; Prince et al. 2002b; also see Nover et al. 2005; Yang and Maunsell 2004 for a similar analysis). Thus we examined the maturation of these response properties. In V2, although the binocular/monocular ratios were adult like at 2 and 4 wk of age, the median binocular response rate at 2 wk of age (8.61 spikes/s) was less than one half of the adult value (19.93 spikes/s) and was significantly lower at 4 (8.33 spikes/s) and 8 wk of age (13.41 spikes/s) than in adults (P < 0.01; Fig. 6A). Similarly the mean binocular response rate of V1 neurons in all infant groups was significantly lower than the response rate in adults (P < 0.01).

FIG. 6.

FIG. 6.

Developmental changes in neuronal firing patterns of V2 and V1 neurons. A: the average (±SE) (○) and median (▪) mean binocular response rate of V2 and V1 neurons in infants and adults. Rectangular boxes indicate the range of quartile values. B: the distribution of response variance-to-mean ratios of V2 and V1 neurons in infants and adults. Plotting conventions are same as in A. Note the lower firing rates and variance-to-mean ratios of infant's units.

As anticipated from these low firing rates in infants, however, the response variances of V1 and V2 neurons were also much lower in infants than in adults. The median variance-to-mean ratio of V2 neurons at 2 wk of age (α2/μ, calculated for the duration of 322 ms for 20 epochs at each phase disparity) was only one half (1.10) of the adult value (2.20; P < 0.01; Fig. 6B). Similarly, the median variance-to-mean ratios of V2 neurons at 4 (1.27) and 8 wk of age (1.13) were also significantly lower than that in adults (P < 0.01). The median ratio for V1 neurons was 1.04 at 2 wk, 1.06 at 4 wk, and 0.94 at 8 wk of age, all of which were significantly lower than that in adults (1.59; P < 0.01). Thus the firing pattern of V1 and V2 neurons in our infants was less noisy than in adults. It is important to note that the differences in variance-to-mean ratios between V2 and V1 were statistically significant at 8 wk of age (P < 0.01) and for adults (P < 0.01), whereas there were no significant differences at 2 and 4 wk of age (P > 0.01).

One of the most important variables affecting the disparity sensitivity of individual cortical neurons is its spatial frequency tuning characteristics. The optimal spatial frequency was determined for each neuron from its fitted spatial frequency tuning function (Fig. 1B). In V2, the median optimal spatial frequency at 2 (0.70 c/deg) and 4 (0.91 c/deg) and 8 wk of age (1.37 c/deg) were significantly lower than that in adults (2.04 c/deg, P < 0.01; Fig. 7A). Similar differences were found between infants and adults (2.04 c/deg) for V1 in 2- (1.0 c/deg, P < 0.01) and 4-wk-old infants (1.02 c/deg, P < 0.01) but not in 8-week-old infants (1.60 c/deg, P > 0.01).

FIG. 7.

FIG. 7.

Developmental changes in spatial frequency tuning. The average (±SE) (○) and median (▪) optimal spatial frequency of V2 and V1 neurons in infants and adults. Rectangular boxes indicate the range of quartile values. B: the distribution of spatial resolution of V2 and V1 neurons in infants and adults. Plotting conventions are same as in A. Note the lower optimal spatial frequency and resolution of infant's units.

The median spatial resolution of V2 neurons at 2 wk of age (3.49 c/deg), determined by locating the highest spatial frequency that evoked responses significantly higher than the average spontaneous firing of the unit (more than ±2 SD), was less than one half the adult median value (8.19 c/deg; P < 0.01; Fig. 7B). The median spatial resolutions at 4 and 8 wk of age (3.65 and 5.1 c/ded, respectively) were also significantly lower compared with adults (P < 0.01). In contrast, 2- (4.00 c/deg) and 4-wk-old infants (4.41 c/deg), but not 8-wk-old infants (5.50 c/deg), had lower median spatial resolutions for V1 neurons compared with adults (7.50 c/deg; P < 0.01). Our data on the developmental changes in the average spatial resolution of V1 neurons are similar to those previously reported (Kiorpes and Movshon et al. 2004). Although we did not directly measure the contrast threshold of V2 or V1 neurons for high spatial frequency gratings, the lower spatial resolutions found for infants using gratings of 80% contrast suggest that the contrast sensitivity of these neurons for higher spatial frequency is likely to be substantially lower than that in adults.

Normalized optimal disparity sensitivity is low at 2 and 4 wk of age

Based on the response modulation as a function of spatial phase disparity, optimal spatial frequency, and response variance of each neuron, we estimated its ability to discriminate small disparity differences, defined here as the neuronal disparity threshold (Fig. 8). The effects of the optimal spatial frequency (2.0 c/deg) on disparity sensitivity for a representative disparity-tuned V2 neuron are shown in Fig. 8A. The changes in the cell's absolute firing rate per unit of angular disparity (right ordinate, spikes/s/arc min) were calculated for the phase disparity tuning function. Note that the highest sensitivity is found at the steepest slope of the disparity tuning function (indicated by an arrow and the thick line at spatial phase disparities around 180°). The optimal disparity sensitivity for this unit therefore was 2.99 spikes/s/arc min.

FIG. 8.

FIG. 8.

Procedures for estimating the disparity threshold for a representative V2 neuron. A: disparity tuning function (solid curve) of a V2 neuron from an adult monkey from which the optimal disparity sensitivity of the cell (dotted curve) was determined by taking its optimal spatial frequency (2.0 c/d) into account. The optimal disparity sensitivity at the steepest portion of the tuning function (2.99 spikes/s/arc min at the thick line) is indicated with a large arrow. B: mean binocular responses (data points) were plotted on log-log coordinate as a function of response variances for the same unit as in A. Each data point indicates 1 spatial phase disparity including spontaneous firing (the lowest data point). The solid line is the best linear fit. The response variance was estimated from the fit function for the steepest portion of the phase tuning function (dotted lines). The optimal disparity sensitivity was normalized with its variance of responses to obtain its disparity threshold.

In adults, the ability to detect small disparity differences is also influenced by the variability of neuronal firing (Nakatsuka et al. 2007; Nover et al. 2005; Yang and Maunsell 2004). To take response variability into account, the relationship between the mean firing rate and response variance was initially determined by plotting a cell's mean response rate as a function of its response variance. Figure 8B shows data for a representative V2 neuron. From the best linear fit for the data points, the response variance of this unit was estimated for the response rate that corresponded to the steepest portion of its phase disparity function (the dotted line). The optimal disparity sensitivity for this unit (2.99 spikes/s/arc min) was normalized with the square root of response variance. The disparity threshold for this V2 neuron was calculated by taking the inverse of the ratio of cell's firing rate per unit of angular disparity/√response variance (2.13 arc min).

The population analysis for the optimal disparity sensitivities shows the effects of the low optimal spatial frequencies and the low firing rates of V2 and V1 neurons in infants (Fig. 9). Specifically, the median optimal disparity sensitivity of V2 neurons at 2 wk of age (0.14 spikes/s/arc min) was nearly 10 times lower than that in adults (1.21 spikes/s/arc min; P < 0.01). At 4 and 8 wk of age, the median disparity sensitivity was still far lower than in adults (P < 0.01). In V1, the optimal disparity sensitivity at 2 and 4 wk of age was significantly lower than in adults (P < 0.01), but at 8 wk of age, the median optimal disparity sensitivity was indistinguishable from adults (P > 0.01), primarily because optimal spatial frequencies of V1 neurons had become adult-like (Fig. 7A).

FIG. 9.

FIG. 9.

The distribution of optimal disparity sensitivity (spikes/s/arc min) of V2 and V1 neurons in infants and adults. Mean (±SE) and median values are indicated by triangles.

The median disparity threshold of V2 neurons in 2-wk-old infants was >4 times higher than that in adults. In addition, for all of the other infant groups, the median disparity threshold of V2 neurons was significantly higher than that in adults (P < 0.01; Fig. 10). The median threshold values for V1 units were also significantly higher than in adults but only for 2- and 4-wk-old infants (P < 0.001). Interestingly, the median threshold of V2 neurons in 2-wk-old infants was significantly higher than that in V1 neurons (P < 0.01). It is worthwhile to note that the adult median disparity threshold values (4.51 arc min in V2 and 5.26 arc min in V1) were very similar to the median neurometric disparity threshold value of V1 neurons in awake behaving monkeys (estimated from Fig. 13 of Prince et al. 2000), which was on average ≥4 times higher than their psychometric disparity thresholds.

FIG. 10.

FIG. 10.

Developmental changes in disparity threshold of V2 and V1 neurons. Mean (±SE) and median values are indicated by triangles.

To obtain a clearer picture of which limiting factor may have relatively larger impact on the neuronal disparity threshold, we compared the relative developmental change in neuronal disparity acuity (1/disparity threshold) with improvement in the BII/DDI, variance-to-mean ratio, spatial frequency (optimal and resolution), and the response rate of V1 and V2 neurons (Fig. 11). The median values for these response measures at 2, 4, and 8 wk of age were normalized to their respective values in adults. The BII or DDI values did not significantly change over age in V2 or V1. For V1 neurons, the improvement in disparity acuity was closely associated with their age-dependent changes in optimal spatial frequency, resolution, and response rate. In V2, however, the disparity acuity of neurons at 2 wk of age was far more immature than any of these responses. Together, in V2, but not in V1, additional limiting factor(s) might have contributed to the lower disparity acuity at 2 wk of age.

FIG. 11.

FIG. 11.

Comparisons of relative developmental changes in neuronal ‘disparity-acuity’ (1/disparity threshold) with improvement in BII/DDI, variance-to-mean ratio, spatial frequency (optimal and resolution), and response rate. The median values for these response measures at 2, 4, and 8 wk of age were normalized to their respective adult values. BII, binocular interaction index; DDI, disparity discrimination index; VMR, variance-to-mean ratio; SR, spatial resolution; SF, optimal spatial frequency; RR, response rate; DA, disparity acuity.

DISCUSSION

The primary findings of this study are that the binocular responses of V2 neurons of 2- and 4-wk-old infant monkeys modulate at near adult levels as a function of binocular spatial phase disparity but that their relatively low binocular response amplitudes and their coarser spatial frequency tuning impose severe limits on their ability to discriminate fine disparity differences.

According to the only psychophysical study in the literature on the normal maturation of steropsis in monkeys, stereoscopic vision could not be detected during the first 3 postnatal wk even for random-dot stimuli having disparities as large as 30 arc min (O'Dell and Boothe 1997). By 4 wk of age, however, nearly 80% of monkeys showed “evidence of stereopsis,” and all 11 monkeys exhibited stereopsis by 8 wk. The average stereo-acuity rapidly dropped from 21.7 arc min at 3 wk (n = 6) down to ∼3.3 arc min by 8 wk of age (n = 11), although the stereoacuity at 8 wk of age was several times worse than in adults (O'Dell and Boothe 1997; Prince et al. 2000). Assuming that these represent an estimate of the infant's optimal visual performance, what prevents the emergence of stereopsis during the first 2–3 wk of life and what limits stereoacuity development during the first 8–10 wk of life? The critical immaturities that have been previously proposed to explain reduced binocular performance in infant primates are 1) abnormal binocular alignment and vergence eye movements (O'Dell and Boothe 1997), 2) the slow maturation of cortical binocular mechanisms (Birch 1993; Held 1993; Kiorpes and Movshon 2004), and 3) the poor visibility of binocular stimuli (Brown et al. 2007; Schor et al. 1984) because of coarse spatial frequency tuning and/or low contrast sensitivity (Chino et al. 1997; Zheng et al. 2007).

Alignment and vergence

Normal binocular vision requires high degrees of coordination between motor and sensory systems. In particular, stereopsis has been shown to depend on precise interocular alignment (Harwerth et al. 1995). A critical variable to be considered for behavioral assessments of stereopsis in primate infants would be potential immaturities in alignment and vergence eye movements that compromise or prevent fusion of binocular images. For this experiment, this was not an issue because we used anesthetized and paralyzed preparations, and the binocular disparities for our experiments were optically controlled with a high degree of precision. However, according to the only data available in the literature on alignment and vergence in awake behaving infant monkeys (published only as an abstract), neonates and young infant monkeys were reported to exhibit unsteady fixation and interocular alignment (Boothe and Gong 1992). Thus O'Dell and Boothe (1997) concluded that immaturities in oculomotor mechanisms were the primary reason why they could not reliably detect stereopsis during the first several postnatal weeks.

Several lines of evidence suggest, however, that the role of errors in interocular alignment in limiting binocular vision development in monkeys may not be as substantial as these investigators suggested. For example, in human infants of equivalent ages, binocular alignment and/or vergence eye movements do not seem to significantly limit stereoscopic vision (Birch et al. 1983; Braddick 1996; Brown et al. 2007; Hainline and Riddell 1995). Consistent with this view, we found in our infant monkeys that the fundamental sensory requirements for binocular fusion and alignment (e.g., relatively well-developed binocular mechanisms and adult-like sensitivity to binocular disparity) are present in V1 and V2 as early as 6–14 days after birth, if not at birth (also see Chino et al. 1997). A recent study showed that many infants between 7 and 24 wk of age show “adult-like” latencies for accommodation and vergence responses during binocular viewing (Tondel and Candy 2008). Equally important are the previous findings that fixating eye movements (saccades) of human infants are relatively mature near birth (Aslin 1977), and as early as 2–3 mo of age (roughly equivalent to 2–3 wk in monkeys), adult-like amplitude and velocities are attained in reactive saccades (Garbutt et al. 2006; Hainline et al. 1984; Matsuzawa and Shimojo 1997; Richards and Holley 1999). These results suggest that subcortical mechanisms supporting motor components of fixation and alignment and the extraocular musculature are relatively well developed near birth provided that stimuli for fixation are visible to infants.

Maturation of binocular mechanisms

These results rule out a lack of disparity detectors in V2 (or V1) as the primary source of stereo-deficiencies in infants. We found that the binocular response modulation of neurons in these areas as a function of interocular spatial phase disparity (BII and DDI) was as robust as in adults. Moreover, the distributions of binocular/monocular response ratios of V2 and V1 neurons in our infant monkeys were very similar to those for adults and to the distribution of binocular/monocular response ratios found in V1 of awake, behaving adult monkeys (Prince et al. 2002a). Interestingly, these investigators also found that those units in which the peak binocular responses were substantially greater than the maximum monocular responses were classified as tuned excitatory (TE) or far cells (FA), whereas those with binocular to monocular ratios ∼1.0 or slightly <1.0 were classified as either near or tuned inhibitory cells, respectively. In this respect, it is tempting to speculate that cortical circuitry underlying conventional disparity cell types (i.e., near, far, tuned-excitatory, and tuned-inhibitory) may be largely adult-like in V2 and V1 at 2 wk of age.

Although qualitatively adult-like binocular mechanisms exist in the visual cortices of neonates, it is not known whether disparity sensitive V2 neurons in our infants are sensitive to relative disparity. As mentioned earlier, this is because microelectrode recording experiments in awake, behaving monkeys are not currently plausible at these ages. However, there is indirect evidence from our separate studies (Zhang et al. 2005) that relative disparity sensitivity might be very weak or absent all together in V2 near birth. To have robust sensitivity to relative binocular disparity, extrastriate neurons must be able to compare and integrate signals over relatively large areas, e.g., units with receptive field (RF) centers and surrounds that are tuned to different disparities (Nienborg and Cumming 2006; Parker 2007; Roe et al. 2007; Umeda et al. 2007). In this respect, we previously found that the RF center/surround organization of V2 neurons is exceedingly immature at 2 wk of age; for example, there are little or no measurable RF surrounds. There is considerable improvement by 4 wk of age, and the RF centers and surrounds become largely adult-like by 8 wk of age (Zhang et al. 2005), the age at which infant monkeys typically exhibit good stereoacuities (O'Dell and Boothe 1997). Thus immaturities in the relative disparity detectors of V2 could be a major factor limiting stereopsis development.

Effects of spatial frequency tuning, firing rate, and contrast sensitivity

Even if disparity tuned V2 neurons in our infant monkeys were sensitive to relative disparity, their performance in stereo-tasks could still be limited by the reduced ability of these neurons to detect fine disparity differences. We found that the median disparity threshold of V2 neurons was ∼4–5 times worse in 2-wk-old infants than in adults (Fig. 10). The primary reason for the poor discrimination performance of V2 neurons was their low optimal spatial frequencies (Fig. 5A) and their low response rates at these ages (Fig. 6A).

How might the normalized optimal disparity sensitivity of V2 neurons in our infant monkeys affect their binocular depth perception? Figure 12 compares the relative time courses for the improvement in behavioral stereoacuity (adopted from O'Dell and Boothe 1997) with the disparity thresholds of V2 and V1 neurons obtained in this study (Fig. 10). The best neuronal threshold value (∼5 arc min in our adults, see Fig. 10) on the right scale was aligned with the optimal value in the left scale for behaviorally measured stereoacuity in adults (<0.5 arc min; Harwerth et al. 1995; O'Dell and Boothe 1997) to facilitate the comparison. The different data sets were fitted with exponential functions that characterized the relative changes in disparity sensitivity between the maximum value at birth and the minimum value for adults. Relative changes in the median disparity threshold for V2 neurons (and somewhat less in V1 neurons) closely paralleled the behavioral improvement in stereoacuity between 2 and 8 wk of age. Comparing time constants (Tc) is a simple and efficient way to determine whether the relative changes in perception and cortical physiology are different. The Tc for perceptual changes was 2.92 wk, which was similar to the Tc for V2 disparity sensitivity (2.60 wk). The Tc for changes in disparity sensitivity of V1 neurons was slightly longer (4.02 wk) because at 2 wk of age disparity sensitivity of V1 neurons was far better than in V2 (Fig. 10), i.e., the V1 changes were more moderate than that in V2, not because the functional maturation of disparity mechanisms was delayed in V1. Considered together, the spatial frequency tuning, firing rate, and response variability of V2 and V1 neurons in the developing visual brain are likely to play a critical role in limiting binocular vision development because the basic cortical mechanisms in V2 and V1 for binocular combination are adult like as early as 2 wk of age.

FIG. 12.

FIG. 12.

Comparisons of developmental changes between behaviorally measured stereoacuity of infant monkeys O'Dell and Boothe 1997 and the disparity threshold (neuronal disparity acuity) of V2 (solid line) and V1 (dotted line) neurons for our infant monkeys. Note that the relative time course of improvement in the disparity threshold of V2 and V1 neurons parallels improvement in behaviorally measured stereoacuity. Data points were fitted with an exponential function, y = (RmaxRmin) × exp(−x/Tc) + Rmin; Rmax, maximum value at birth; Rmin, minimum value in adults; x, time in weeks; Tc, time constant.

Similar conclusions were reached in previous psychophysical studies in human infants. For example, the development of stereopsis was shown to largely depend on the maturation of spatial frequency tuning and contrast sensitivity (Schor 1985). A more recent study with human infants showed that the most critical factor limiting stereopsis in infants was their poor visual performance caused by insensitivities to stimulus contrast (Brown et al. 2007). Common to both studies is that binocular vision development is severely constrained by the visibility of stimuli. Our recent studies also found that, in comparison to adults, the contrast sensitivity of infant V2 neurons is substantially lower until 8 wk of age (Zhang et al. 2005; Zheng et al. 2007).

Conclusions

During early postnatal development, the sensitivity of binocular disparity mechanisms in early cortical processing (V1/V2) is constrained by the relatively coarse spatial frequency tuning, the lower response rate, the low contrast sensitivities, and/or the immaturities in the RF center-surround organization of cortical neurons. One or more of these immaturities in turn are likely to limit the development of binocular depth perception. Alternatively, immature cortical mechanisms beyond V2, yet to be discovered, could be another limiting factor. However, this study unambiguously showed that, regardless of the maturational state of extrastriate visual areas beyond V2, immaturities in the response properties of V2 and V1 neurons described in this study are likely to impose substantial constraints on binocular performance of infant primates.

GRANTS

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Research and CORE Grants EY-08128 to. Y. M. Chino, EY-03611 to E. L. Smith III, and RR-07146.

The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

REFERENCES

  • Aslin 1977.Aslin RN Development of binocular fixation in human infants. J Exp Child Psychol 23: 133–150, 1977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Batardiere 2002.Batardiere A, Barone P, Knoblauch K, Giroud P, Berland M, Dumas AM, Kennedy H. Early specification of the hierarchical organization of visual cortical areas in the macaque monkey. Cereb Cortex 12: 453–465, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Birch 1993.Birch EE Stereopsis in infants and its developmental relationship to visual acuity. In: Early Visual Development, Normal and Abnormal, edited by Simon K. New York: Oxford UP, 1993, p. 224–236.
  • Birch 1983.Birch EE, Gwiazda J, Held R. The development of vergence does not account for the onset of stereopsis. Perception 12: 331–336, 1983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Boothe 1992.Boothe R, Gong W. Development of binocular alignment in normal and visually deprived monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 33(Suppl): 899, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • Braddick 1996.Braddick O Binocularity in infancy. Eye 10: 182–188, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Brown 2007.Brown AM, Lindsey DT, Satgunam P, Miracle JA. Critical immaturities limiting infant binocular stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 48: 1424–1434, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Celebrini 1994.Celebrini S, Newsome WT. Neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity to motion signals in extrastriate area MST of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 14: 4109–4124, 1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Chino 2004.Chino YM, Bi H, Zhang B. The postnatal development of the neuronal response properties in primate visual cortex. In: Primate Vision, edited by Kaas J, Collins C. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, 2004, p. 81–108.
  • Chino 1997.Chino YM, Smith EL III, Hatta S, Cheng H. Postnatal development of binocular disparity sensitivity in neurons of the primate visual cortex. J Neurosci 17: 296–307, 1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cumming 2001.Cumming BG, DeAngelis GC. The physiology of stereopsis. Annu Rev Neurosci 24: 203–238, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cumming 1999.Cumming BG, Parker AJ. Binocular neurons in V1 of awake monkeys are selective for absolute, not relative, disparity. J Neurosci 19: 5602–5618, 1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • DeAngelis 1993.DeAngelis GC, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD. Spatiotemporal organization of simple-cell receptive fields in the cat's striate cortex. II. Linearity of temporal and spatial summation. J Neurophysiol 69: 1118–1135, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Distler 1996.Distler C, Bachevalier J, Kennedy C, Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG. Functional development of the corticocortical pathway for motion analysis in the macaque monkey: a 14C-2-deoxyglucose study. Cereb Cortex 6: 184–195, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Garbutt 2006.Garbutt S, Harwood MR, Harris CM. Infant saccades are not slow. Dev Med Child Neurol 48: 662–667, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hainline 1995.Hainline L, Riddell PM. Binocular alignment and vergence in early infancy. Vision Res 35: 3229–3236, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hainline 1984.Hainline L, Turkel J, Abramov I, Lemerise E, Harris CM. Characteristics of saccades in human infants. Vision Res 24: 1771–1780, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harwerth 1995.Harwerth RS, Smith EL III, Siderov J. Behavioral studies of local stereopsis and disparity vergence in monkeys. Vision Res 35: 1755–1770, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Held 1993.Held R Two stages in the development of binocular vision and eye aligment. In: Early Visual Development: Normal and Abnormal, edited by Simon K. New York: Oxford UP, 1993, p. 250–257.
  • Hubel 1962.Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol 160: 106–154, 1962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kiorpes 2004.Kiorpes L, Movshon JA. Development of sensitivity to visual motion in macaque monkeys. Vis Neurosci 21: 851–859, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Matsuzawa 1997.Matsuzawa M, Shimojo S. Infant's fast saccades in the gap paradigim and development of visual attention. Infant Behav Dev 20: 449–455, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • Nakatsuka 2007.Nakatsuka C, Zhang B, Watanabe I, Zheng J, Bi H, Ganz L, Smith EL, Harwerth RS, Chino YM. Effects of perceptual learning on local stereopsis and neuronal responses of V1 and V2 in prism-reared monkeys. J Neurophysiol 97: 2612–2626, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nienborg 2004.Nienborg H, Bridge H, Parker AJ, Cumming BG. Receptive field size in V1 neurons limits acuity for perceiving disparity modulation. J Neurosci 24: 2065–2076, 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nienborg 2005.Nienborg H, Bridge H, Parker AJ, Cumming BG. Neuronal computation of disparity in V1 limits temporal resolution for detecting disparity modulation. J Neurosci 25: 10207–10219, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nienborg 2006.Nienborg H, Cumming BG. Macaque V2 neurons, but not V1 neurons, show choice-related activity. J Neurosci 26: 9567–9578, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nover 2005.Nover H, Anderson CH, DeAngelis GC. A logarithmic, scale-invariant representation of speed in macaque middle temporal area accounts for speed discrimination performance. J Neurosci 25: 10049–10060, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • O'Dell 1997.O'Dell C, Boothe RG.The development of stereoacuity in infant rhesus monkeys. Vision Res 37: 2675–2684, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ohzawa 1986a.Ohzawa I, Freeman RD. The binocular organization of complex cells in the cat's visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 56: 243–259, 1986a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ohzawa 1986b.Ohzawa I, Freeman RD. The binocular organization of simple cells in the cat's visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 56: 221–242, 1986b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Orban 2006.Orban GA, Janssen P, Vogels R. Extracting 3D structure from disparity. Trends Neurosci 29: 466–473, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Parker 2007.Parker AJ Binocular depth perception and the cerebral cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 379–391, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Prince 2002a.Prince SJ, Cumming BG, Parker AJ. Range and mechanism of encoding of horizontal disparity in macaque V1. J Neurophysiol 87: 209–221, 2002a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Prince 2000.Prince SJ, Pointon AD, Cumming BG, Parker AJ. The precision of single neuron responses in cortical area V1 during stereoscopic depth judgments. J Neurosci 20: 3387–3400, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Prince 2002b.Prince SJ, Pointon AD, Cumming BG, Parker AJ. Quantitative analysis of the responses of V1 neurons to horizontal disparity in dynamic random-dot stereograms. J Neurophysiol 87: 191–208, 2002b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Quick 1992.Quick MW, Eggers HM, Boothe RG. Natural strabismus in monkeys. Convergence errors assessed by cover test and photographic methods. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 33: 2986–3004, 1992. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Read 2002.Read JC, Parker AJ, Cumming BG. A simple model accounts for the response of disparity-tuned V1 neurons to anticorrelated images. Vis Neurosci 19: 735–753, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Richards 1999.Richards JE, Holley FB. Infant attention and the development of smooth pursuit tracking. Dev Psychol 35: 856–867, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Roe 2007.Roe AW, Parker AJ, Born RT, DeAngelis GC. Disparity channels in early vision. J Neurosci 27: 11820–11831, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schor 1985.Schor CM Development of stereopsis depends upon contrast sensitivity and spatial tuning. J Am Optom Assoc 56: 628–635, 1985. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schor 1984.Schor CM, Wood IC, Ogawa J. Spatial tuning of static and dynamic local stereopsis. Vision Res 24: 573–578, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Skottun 1991.Skottun BC, De Valois RL, Grosof DH, Movshon JA, Albrecht DG, Bonds AB. Classifying simple and complex cells on the basis of response modulation. Vision Res 31: 1079–1086, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Smith 1997.Smith EL, Chino YM, Ni J, Ridder WH III, Crawford ML. Binocular spatial phase tuning characteristics of neurons in the macaque striate cortex. J Neurophysiol 78: 351–365, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Swindale 1998.Swindale NV Orientation tuning curves: empirical description and estimation of parameters. Biol Cybern 78: 45–56, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Thomas 2002.Thomas OM, Cumming BG, Parker AJ. A specialization for relative disparity in V2. Nat Neurosci 5: 472–478, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tondel 2008.Tondel GM, Candy TR. Accommodation and vergence latencies in human infants. Vision Res 48: 564–576, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Uka 2003.Uka T, DeAngelis GC. Contribution of middle temporal area to coarse depth discrimination: comparison of neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity. J Neurosci 23: 3515–3530, 2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Uka 2004.Uka T, DeAngelis GC. Contribution of area MT to stereoscopic depth perception: choice-related response modulations reflect task strategy. Neuron 42: 297–310, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Umeda 2007.Umeda K, Tanabe S, Fujita I. Representation of stereoscopic depth based on relative disparity in macaque area V4. J Neurophysiol 98: 241–252, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Yang 2004.Yang T, Maunsell JH. The effect of perceptual learning on neuronal responses in monkey visual area V4. J Neurosci 24: 1617–1626, 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zhang 2008.Zhang B, Smith EL 3rd, Chino YM. Postnatal development of onset transient responses in macaque V1 and V2 neurons. J Neurophysiol 100: 1476–1487, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zhang 2007.Zhang B, Tong J, Smith EL, Chino YM. Contrast sensitivity of transient onset responses in V1 and V2 neurons of macaque infants. In: Society for Neuroscience Program No 28025. San Diego, CA, 2007.
  • Zhang 2005.Zhang B, Zheng J, Watanabe I, Maruko I, Bi H, Smith EL III, Chino Y. Delayed maturation of receptive field center/surround mechanisms in V2. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 5862–5867, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zheng 2007.Zheng J, Zhang B, Bi H, Maruko I, Watanabe I, Nakatsuka C, Smith EL III, Chino YM. Development of temporal response properties and contrast sensitivity of V1 and V2 neurons in macaque monkeys. J Neurophysiol 97: 3905–3916, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neurophysiology are provided here courtesy of American Physiological Society

RESOURCES