Abstract
We developed a new approach to investigate how the nervous system activates multiple redundant muscles by studying the endpoint force fluctuations during isometric force generation at a multi-degree-of-freedom joint. We hypothesized that, due to signal-dependent muscle force noise, endpoint force fluctuations would depend on the target direction of index finger force and that this dependence could be used to distinguish flexible from synergistic activation of the musculature. We made high-gain measurements of isometric forces generated to different target magnitudes and directions, in the plane of index finger metacarpophalangeal joint abduction–adduction/flexion–extension. Force fluctuations from each target were used to calculate a covariance ellipse, the shape of which varied as a function of target direction. Directions with narrow ellipses were approximately aligned with the estimated mechanical actions of key muscles. For example, targets directed along the mechanical action of the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) yielded narrow ellipses, with 88% of the variance directed along those target directions. It follows the FDI is likely a prime mover in this target direction and that, at most, 12% of the force variance could be explained by synergistic coupling with other muscles. In contrast, other target directions exhibited broader covariance ellipses with as little as 30% of force variance directed along those target directions. This is the result of cooperation among multiple muscles, based on independent electromyographic recordings. However, the pattern of cooperation across target directions indicates that muscles are recruited flexibly in accordance with their mechanical action, rather than in fixed groupings.
INTRODUCTION
The CNS can typically utilize many different muscle combinations when controlling multiple degrees of freedom (DOF) of the body. To simplify task control (Bernstein 1967), it has been proposed that the CNS enforces muscle synergies: fixed patterns of activation among multiple muscles acting about the relevant DOF (d'Avella et al. 2003; Drew et al. 2008; Giszter et al. 2007; Ivanenko et al. 2006; Overduin et al. 2008; Saltiel et al. 2001; Ting and Macpherson 2005; Tresch et al. 2006). Alternatively, the CNS could use a task-specific muscle coordination pattern without requiring fixed patterns, perhaps reflecting the optimization of movement according to some suitable performance criteria (Buchanan and Shreeve 1996; Harris and Wolpert 1998; Kuo 1994; Todorov and Jordan 2002; Valero-Cuevas 2000; Valero-Cuevas et al. 1998). It is also unclear whether some force in some directions is generated by a “prime mover” muscle (Thomas et al. 1986) or whether all force generation involves the cooperation of multiple muscles (Buchanan et al. 1986; Keenan et al. 2006). These questions remain unresolved, in spite of multiple attempts to characterize muscle activation patterns across multiple DOFs.
The synergistic activation hypotheses and the task-specific flexible activation hypotheses are not incompatible; strategies could apply to voluntary skilled tasks different from those applying to stereotypical reflexive tasks. To separate these hypotheses in voluntary tasks, we introduce a new method for assessing muscle force contributions to net force generation at a multiple DOF joint: the metacarpophalangeal (MCP) joint of the index finger. This method uses high-gain force measurements recorded at the finger tip to estimate the various muscle contributions to net joint force. In contrast, most prior studies investigating muscle coordination across multiple DOFs have focused on the use of electromyographic (EMG) recordings. Although such EMG recordings provide valuable information about muscle activity, they offer significant disadvantages for studying muscle coordination in multiple muscle systems. For example, it is not always possible to record EMGs from all muscles that may contribute to a task. Also, the identification of muscle-level synergies from EMGs in natural behaviors may also be complicated by the existence of biomechanical or task-planning constraints unrelated to muscle synergies. If the CNS chooses to generate force in stereotypical ways, it may cause muscle activation patterns to appear to obey simplifying activation constraints, even if other activation patterns are possible.
An alternative approach to studying muscle coordination involves mapping isometric endpoint force variability for an array of targets distributed uniformly across the endpoint force space. Stochastic effects may enter such tasks in several ways, but one of the most significant is signal-dependent noise (SDN) (Enoka et al. 1999; Galganski et al. 1993; Laidlaw et al. 2000; Schmidt et al. 1979; Slifkin and Newell 1999), where isometric force variability increases with average isometric force. Such SDN may arise from the sequential recruitment of motor units with larger twitch forces as the muscle force requirement increases (Jones et al. 2002). If muscle force variability increases with average muscle force, then differing neuromotor control strategies can generate different patterns of endpoint force variability. For example, one muscle acting alone will induce variability that not only increases with signal magnitude but is also directionally aligned with the muscle's action direction in endpoint force space. Alternatively, cooperation among multiple muscles, each with different action directions, will induce endpoint variability that reflects the multiple directions of muscle action. It follows that the overall endpoint force covariance caused by a muscle synergy will be composed of individual covariances from the constituent muscles, but will not be aligned with the covariance of any one muscle. Noise may occur at other levels in the motor cascade, such as at the level of synergy commands, and where noise is generated may have an impact on the structure of endpoint force fluctuations.
In light of these possibilities, the aim of the present study was to determine, using multidirectional forces generated by the human index finger, whether the covariance of multidirectional isometric force exhibits significant differences as a function of target force direction. Furthermore, we argue that such force covariance maps provide strong clues about the ways in which the nervous system controls muscle combinations or synergies, acting about multiple DOFs.
METHODS
We developed a method to use the variability of isometric forces exerted by the index finger as an indicator of underlying muscle coordination. The method uses the pattern of this force variability as a function of target force direction and magnitude, termed the force covariance map, to quantify the relative amount of variability that occurs in the target direction itself, termed the target-directed variance fraction (η). Muscle coordination strategies may constrain how η varies with the target direction. In the following text we describe the theoretical basis for the force covariance map, our means of experimentally measuring it, and the techniques used for relating the force covariance map to muscle action direction and activity.
Force covariance map
The variability of endpoint forces will likely be influenced by how muscles are activated. In the present study, we define the task as the exertion of isometric forces by the tip of the index finger (i.e., the endpoint), with a specified direction and magnitude in the plane perpendicular to the finger. We define the axes of this plane as flexion–extension and abduction–adduction, corresponding to vertical and horizontal forces, respectively, when the palm of the hand faces down. The relation between the pattern of endpoint force variability—the force covariance map—and hypothetical muscle coordination is based on three assumptions of muscle force production: linear superposition, signal-dependent noise, and low correlation of noise-like forces between muscles. When these assumptions are valid, the force covariance map may be used to study muscle coordination and muscle synergies.
Linear superposition provides a means to examine muscle coordination in endpoint force space. Superposition is the assumption that the endpoint force is the linear sum of individual contributions of force from each muscle. These individual contributions occur along directions in the endpoint force space, or muscle mechanical actions (illustrated hypothetically in Fig. 1 A) that depend on the musculoskeletal geometry, such as the lengths of body segments and moment arms about the degrees of freedom of joints (Kuo 1994, 2000). Action directions have been empirically quantified for cat hindlimb muscles contributing to multidirectional ankle force (Lawrence et al. 1993), monkey forearm muscles contributing to multidirectional wrist force (Hoffman and Strick 1999), and human thenar motor units contributing to thumb abduction and flexion (Westling et al. 1990). Superposition does not always apply, for example, if a single muscle also pulls on neighboring muscles. However, these effects are usually small, even among motor units within a muscle (Sandercock 2005). Mechanical actions have been measured for the muscles controlling the human index finger and the forces from individual tendons have been found to superpose linearly at the fingertip (Valero-Cuevas et al. 2000). Superposition of isometric force has been found to apply well in other biomechanical systems such as the frog hindlimb (Mussa-Ivaldi et al. 1994).
Signal-dependent noise will induce variability in endpoint forces, dependent on muscle action directions and how muscles are coordinated. We consider first the hypothetical case of a muscle activated as a prime mover, when the target force direction is aligned with a particular muscle's mechanical action. The endpoint force would be expected to fluctuate (e.g., Galganski et al. 1993; Thomas et al. 1991) along the action direction, with force variability increasing with activation magnitude (e.g., Jones et al. 2002; Moritz et al. 2005). The force covariance may be thought of as an ellipse aligned with the principal axis aligned with the target direction (Fig. 1B). In general, the force covariance ellipse may be thought of as the region of force space that contains a large proportion of the force data points for a given target. For example, if force variability is Gaussian distributed, a 2σ force covariance ellipse would contain about 87% of force data points.
We next consider the more typical case where muscles are activated together, for example, to produce a force not aligned with a single muscle's action. The individual muscle covariances then approximately add together to determine the total force covariance. The force covariance ellipse will then be aligned with neither muscle action (Fig. 1C). A single muscle synergy combines multiple muscles together (Fig. 1D) and will therefore produce a covariance ellipse in the direction of synergy action that is the same covariance as the coactivation case. Here it is important to assume that noise-like fluctuations in the individual muscle forces are relatively uncorrelated. Correlation is a concern because, for instance, very highly correlated force fluctuations between two antagonistic muscles would cancel from the measurable endpoint force variance, hiding their activation from the force covariance map. Correlation has been observed across muscles (Bremner et al. 1991a,b) and may vary depending on the training of an individual (Semmler 2002; Semmler and Nordstrom 1998b). However, the correlation across muscles is weak and its functional significance during the production of smooth force has been questioned (Deluca et al. 1993; Schieber and Santello 2004). Coherence between surface EMG pairs has been shown to be modest (<10%) at low frequencies, with higher values (30–50%) only in upper frequencies of 15–30 Hz (Farmer et al. 2007; Fisher et al. 2002; Kilner et al. 1999). Joint force contains a significant signal only in the 6- to 12-Hz range due to unfused motor unit twitches, but relatively little signal at higher frequencies due to the slow time course of muscle contraction (see Galganski et al. 1993). Therefore even if there is significant coherence in muscle electrical activity, it occurs at frequencies unlikely to produce significant muscle forces.
The force covariance map is a set of force covariances measured at equally spaced points around a constant average endpoint force magnitude. A force covariance map when no muscles are in synergies may contain two types of force covariance ellipses: 1) narrow ellipses aligned with the target direction in directions of muscle action (target-directed ellipses), indicating muscles behaving as prime movers; and 2) broad nonaligned ellipses (non-target-directed ellipses) in directions involving coactivation of multiple muscles with different action directions (Fig. 1E). Alternatively, if all muscles are grouped into synergies with muscles having different action directions, only coactivation ellipses will be observed because multiple muscles will always contribute in some fixed amount (Fig. 1F).
Experimental procedures
We experimentally measured the force covariance map for endpoint forces produced by the MCP joint of the human index finger. We also measured surface EMG from three muscles to compare with the force covariances and also to form rough estimates of muscle recruitment curves and action directions. Seven unimpaired subjects (two female, five male) participated in the study. All subjects were right-handed and used their dominant index finger to produce isometric forces in different directions and magnitudes in the flexion–extension/abduction–adduction (FEAA) force plane. The Northwestern University Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol and informed consent was obtained from each subject prior to participation.
The experimental apparatus was designed to mechanically isolate the index finger (Fig. 2). Subjects were seated upright in an adjustable chair with the shoulder abducted about 15°, with the elbow resting on a padded support. The elbow joint was flexed to 90° and the forearm was casted and secured in a plastic orthosis, in a pronated position with the palm facing down. The index finger was casted and placed in a fixed cylindrical tube, so that forces were exerted against the inside of the tube and about an “endpoint” just distal to the distal interphalangeal (DIP) joint. These forces were transmitted to the tube by three screws 120° apart that centered the finger in the tube. The screws were gently pushed into the cast while it hardened, making a secure but comfortable connection with the index finger. One subject repeated the experiment with the screws rotated to different positions relative to the finger and it was found that the screw positions did not affect the results. This setup maintained both interphalangeal joints extended and approximately aligned the MCP extension axis with the vertical load cell axis. Digits 3–5 were secured in a resting position that was slightly adducted away from the index finger. The thumb was abducted to 50° and casted with the wrist. One Velcro strap was placed around the subject's waist and two Velcro straps were placed in a crisscross fashion over each shoulder to restrain the subject and to minimize shoulder movement.
Isometric index finger forces in the FEAA plane were measured using a sensitive six-axis load cell (Model 20E12A-I25 9N.5, JR3, Woodland, CA). In the configuration it was used, this load cell had a bandwidth of 0–250 Hz. Forces were low-pass filtered by an analog fourth-order Butterworth filter with a 926-Hz cutoff frequency. The index finger exerted forces on one end of the tube and the other end transmitted and amplified these forces to the load cell. The length of the tube (10 cm) was designed to amplify potentially small endpoint forces and to allow them to be recorded by both translational forces and rotational moments on the load cell. We estimated the load cell's resolution (smallest measurable force) to be 1 mN. These forces were also presented visually to subjects in real time. We displayed a dynamic cursor representing the instantaneous two-dimensional force in the FEAA plane (see Fig. 2) on a computer screen. Forces were sampled at 1,000 Hz.
We also recorded surface EMGs from several finger muscles, so that EMG activity regions could be compared with force covariances. EMGs were recorded using miniature electrode/preamplifiers (Delsys, Boston, MA) with two silver recording surfaces, 5 mm long and 10 mm apart. The preamplifiers have bandwidth 20–450 Hz for surface recordings, with gains set to 100. EMG electrodes were placed according to standard anatomical landmarks and verified with the recommended test maneuvers (Perotto 2005). EMG signals were sampled at 2,000 Hz.
Electromyograms were recorded from the first dorsal interosseous (FDI), extensor digitorum communis (EDC), and extensor indicis proprius (EIP), but not in all subjects. EMG activity was recorded from the FDI in six subjects. Additional activity was recorded from the EDC in three of these subjects and the EIP from the other three. We used the EMGs to estimate recruitment curves as a function of target direction and to estimate muscle action directions. These were then compared qualitatively to force covariance results. We found the small number of subjects acceptable for such qualitative comparisons.
The experimental protocol called for the exertion of endpoint forces in 24 different directions and at three different magnitudes. The directions were generally distributed equally over the plane at 15° increments. One subject performed a slightly different protocol, with tighter distribution between pure abduction and pure flexion, but the same number of directions overall. A rest trial was collected for each subject to establish EMG baseline levels. For each trial, the subject viewed the desired force as a target on the visual display (Fig. 2), represented in polar coordinates by a static ray for target direction, and a static circle for target magnitude. The subject was instructed to gradually exert force in the target direction and then to hold the target force as precisely as possible for about 10–20 s, the goal being to generate a stationary time series of forces with constant magnitude and direction on average. The experimenter examined the time-domain force traces on-line. Trials were repeated if forces were found not to be approximately constant. The force feedback display was zeroed, with the subject at rest, before each set of trials. Subjects were asked to rest for ≥10 s between each trial and ≥1 min after each group of 10 trials.
The force magnitudes were also equally distributed at three levels, chosen to require very minimal effort for all subjects. Because these magnitudes were at discrete levels, we refer to them as target levels 1–3. These target levels were distributed at equal intervals, with the highest magnitude level, task level 3, at about 2 N in magnitude.
Data analysis
The experimental data were analyzed to quantify endpoint force variability, to determine regions of activity in task space for key muscles, and to estimate the action direction of these muscles.
Quantifying force variability
We used measured forces to compute force covariance maps. We first cropped each trial to isolate relatively constant forces of ≥10 s in duration (Fig. 3 A). The time series forces, FAA(t) in the adduction–abduction direction and FFE(t) in the flexion–extension direction, were combined in a vector time series F(t). The empirical target force vector Ftarget was defined to be equal to the average force vector F̄, with F̂target defined as the unit vector in that direction. Target direction can also be expressed as an angle θ in endpoint force space, measured counterclockwise from the adduction axis. To reduce the voluntary contribution to force variability, we filtered both force components (Fig. 3B) with a zero-phase-lag eighth-order Butterworth band-pass filter with 3-dB cutoffs at 5 and 30 Hz (Filter Design and Analysis Toolbox of MATLAB, The MathWorks, Natick, MA). We found substantial voluntary contribution below 5 Hz and nonphysiological noise >30 Hz. The choice of cutoff frequencies was found to have no qualitative effect on the results. Filtered force data F̃ were then used to compute the force covariance (Fig. 3, C–E). The covariances, plotted as ellipses centered at the target force for each trial, constitute the force covariance map (Fig. 3F).
To determine whether endpoint force variability could be used as an indicator of muscle activity, we examined endpoint force variability for signal-dependent noise (SDN), which should manifest itself in recordings of multidirectional force variability as a scaling of variability as a function of target magnitude level (see appendix). We tested this hypothesis by computing the total variance of force as the trace of the force covariance matrix. The total variance was averaged across all target directions for each task level and subject. Within each subject, the total variance was normalized by dividing by the total variance of task level 1. A linear regression analysis was performed (regress in MATLAB) using the equation: Normalized total variance = a × Task level + b, where a and b are coefficients to be determined.
We quantified the degree to which the covariance for each trial was aligned with that trial's target force direction. We refer to this quantity as the target-directed variance fraction (η), defined as the fraction of the total variance (in both force components) that occurs in the direction of the target force (Fig. 3G). For example, η is 100% if force fluctuates only in the target direction, 50% if it fluctuates equally in all directions, and 0% if it fluctuates entirely in a direction orthogonal to the target direction. We refer to the covariance matrix of filtered forces as cov [F̃], so that
where the numerator quantifies the amount of variance occurring in the target direction and the denominator summarizes the amount of total variance as a scalar.
Assuming that SDN applies to each individual muscle's force and that there is little correlation between these forces, the target-directed variance fraction η can be used as a bound on synergistic behavior among muscles. Suppose that the force covariance map exhibits a high target-directed variance in a muscle A's action direction. If η = 0.9 in this target direction, then at most 10% of the force variance is generated by the combined effect of muscles acting in different directions than muscle A. Such a relatively low contribution to force variability from muscles in other directions suggests that muscle A is not strongly synergistically linked to muscles that act in different directions: 1 − η is an upper bound on synergistic behavior because muscle A may contain motor units with slightly different mechanical actions, thereby generating variance outside the target direction without any synergistic coupling with other muscles.
Statistical regularities in the force covariance map were quantified using a one-way ANOVA of the target-directed variance fraction using task direction as the factor. Target direction θ was a continuous variable. To make it a discrete factor for ANOVA analysis, target direction was separated into 15° bins, corresponding to the increment of target directions presented to the subject.
Regions of muscle activity in endpoint force space
Muscle recruitment curves were computed from EMG data and plotted as a function of target direction. Raw EMG traces were first rectified and averaged across the hold period of each trial and the corresponding rest period, with the difference between the two serving as the net EMG. We fit a cosine tuning curve (Hoffman and Strick 1999; Todorov 2002) to net EMG data within each subject and target level (24 values for each target level), minimizing the sum-squared error. The EMG data for each target level and subject were then normalized to the maximum of the fit, thus avoiding normalization to a spurious maximum in the data. Once the EMG data were normalized, comparisons of directional tuning could be made across target levels and subjects. To summarize the EMG data for each muscle across target levels and subjects, a single cosine tuning curve was fit to the normalized EMG data.
Estimates of muscle activity correlation
Due to its potential effects on inferences drawn from the force covariance map, we estimated muscle activity correlation. The correlation coefficient between EMG signals during the steady portion of the contraction was calculated for the FDI–EIP and FDI–EDC pair. Correlation coefficients are reported only for target directions exhibiting significant average EMG activity in both muscles.
Estimates of muscle action in endpoint force space
We estimated the directions of muscle actions, in the endpoint force space of abduction–adduction and flexion–extension, with a cross-correlation of force and EMG data. This is a generalization of the spike-triggered averaging method, normally applied to single motor unit EMGs, to surface EMGs. Spike-triggered averaging correlates intramuscular EMGs to a single motor unit's spike waveform, to yield spike times, and then correlates spike times to endpoint force to yield an estimate of a single motor unit's mechanical action. Our method correlates surface EMGs directly to endpoint force to yield an estimate of a muscle's action. Referring to muscle i's EMG time series (normalized to its mean) as Ei[n], with n for discrete time, the cross-correlation zi[n] is defined as
where the sum was computed across the hold period of each trial. This equation quantifies the average trajectory that occurs in endpoint force space subsequent to increases in the EMG signal from muscle i. The action direction estimate αi was derived by computing the direction of the average vector z̄i[n] over a range of time shifts between force and EMG (EMG and force simultaneous to EMG leading force by 100 ms). The direction of αi is the direction of change for isometric force when the rectified EMG in muscle i is increasing, averaged across the hold period. When Ei[n] is a spike train, the preceding process reduces to spike-triggered averaging. We have recently shown on theoretical grounds that the direction αi may accurately represent muscle mechanical action despite the fact that zi[n] may not accurately represent the muscle activation magnitude or time course (Kutch et al. 2007). This analysis produced action estimates, specific to our experimental setup, for the FDI, EDC, and EIP. For muscles from which we did not record EMGs, we estimated actions from moment arm data in the literature. We used published data for index finger muscles derived from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data (Fowler et al. 2001), for finger postures that corresponded to those used here. The literature estimates were: flexor digitorum profundus (FDP) 296.5°, flexor digitorum superficialis (FDS) 292.7°, first lumbrical (LUM) 247.9°, and first palmar interosseous (FPI) 347.5°.
Computational hypothesis analysis
We examined the ability of FCM to distinguish muscle activation strategies by mathematically describing the assumptions of superposition of muscle forces, signal-dependent noise, and modest muscle force correlation. The synergistic activation hypothesis was described mathematically as a linear coupling among muscle activations (d'Avella et al. 2003; Ting and Macpherson 2005; Tresch et al. 2006). The flexible activation hypothesis was described mathematically as a minimization of the sum of squared muscle forces (Buchanan and Shreeve 1996; Todorov and Jordan 2002; van Bolhuis and Gielen 1999). This minimization produces flexible muscle activation because it can appropriately activate single muscles or muscle groups depending on the target direction, whereas the synergistic activation hypothesis can activate muscle groups only. Simulations of both hypotheses were performed across unknown parameter values unknown noise source (synergy-level vs. muscle-level noise). The computational analysis permitted a rigorous assessment of the ability of FCM to distinguish competing hypotheses (see the appendix for mathematical details).
RESULTS
We will describe results supporting the hypothesis that patterns of isometric endpoint force variability contain significant information about neuromotor control strategies. Subjects produced fairly constant average endpoint forces in all directions and magnitudes. The residual variability yielded force covariance maps with considerable dependence on target direction (see representative data in Fig. 3F). For most target directions, the covariance ellipses were relatively broad with little alignment with the target direction (non-target-directed ellipses). These likely represent cases where multiple muscles cooperate to produce the endpoint force. However, for three particular directions, covariance ellipses were quite narrow and well aligned with the target direction (target-directed ellipses), indicating cases where a single muscle potentially acted as a prime mover. This pattern was also quite consistent across subjects, as quantified by high target-directed variance fractions η in those few directions. As a test of the FCM methodology, we found a positive correlation between force magnitude and total force variance, indicating that much of the variance can indeed be attributed to signal-dependent noise, as hypothesized. EMG tuning curves showed preferred directions of recruitment that corresponded with the directions of high target-directed variance, supporting the interpretation of target-directed covariances as indication of prime-mover activity. Estimates of mechanical action also indicated that these prime mover directions corresponded to the action induced by a single muscle. The quantitative data for these results are presented in the following text.
Testing assumptions of SDN and muscle activity correlation
Total force variance generally scaled with target level, indicating that SDN was responsible for much of the observed endpoint force variability (Fig. 4). A linear regression analysis applied to the normalized total variance showed a significant increase in total variance as a function of target level (P = 0.00001), in accordance with the scaling assumption for SDN (see the appendix for mathematical details). The data also indicated a roughly linear scaling of variance with target magnitude level (slope), without a significant amount of “signal-independent noise” (intercept). The regression coefficients were a = 1.19 ± 0.16 and b = −0.19 ± 0.34 (mean ± 95% confidence interval).
The EMG–EMG correlation coefficients for the muscle pairs FDI–EIP and FDI–EDC were found to be very modest. Across target directions exhibiting significant EMG activity in both muscles of the pair, the FDI–EIP correlation coefficient was found to be 0.0078 ± 0.0122] (mean ± SD) and the FDI–EDC correlation coefficient was found to be 0.0055 ± 0.0144 (variation across targets and subjects). These correlations clearly do not represent all muscle pairings relevant to the experiment, but they are congruent with modest amounts of muscle activity correlation previously reported in the literature (Bremner et al. 1991a; Farmer et al. 2007; Fisher et al. 2002; Kilner et al. 1999). These correlations were not used to adjust the force covariance ellipses.
EMG tuning curves and directions of muscle mechanical action
Each muscle had a distinct EMG tuning curve (Fig. 5, top) and mechanical action (Fig. 5, bottom). Cosine tuning curves used for EMG data normalization generally provided a good description of how EMG activity varied with direction. R2 values for the cosine tuning fit to EMG activity were (mean ± SD) 0.82 ± 0.11 for the FDI, 0.75 ± 0.16 for the EDC, and 0.87 ± 0.05 for the EIP (variation is across subjects and target levels). When cosine tuning fits were produced for the normalized EMG data grouped across subject and target level, R2 values were 0.74 for FDI, 0.68 for EDC, and 0.74 for EIP, indicating the consistency of directional tuning in these muscles across subject and target level (Fig. 5, top). FDI was recruited in the left half of the endpoint force plane, with a peak activity in the pure abduction direction. EDC was recruited primarily in combined abduction–extension target force directions, with a peak at 122°, as measured counterclockwise from the positive adduction axis. EIP was recruited primarily in the upper half of the endpoint force plane, with peak activity in the pure extension direction.
The mechanical action directions (Fig. 5, bottom) were roughly aligned with the peak recruitment directions. The directions were (mean ± SD) 196.2 ± 20.1° for FDI, 90.5 ± 31.4° for EDC, and 70.5 ± 20.7° for EIP, measured counterclockwise with respect to the adduction axis.
Force covariance map relates to muscle activity and muscle action directions
The shape and alignment of force covariance ellipses were nonuniform as a function of target direction. The target-directed variance fraction η varied significantly with target direction (ANOVA, P = 0.000001), with peaks in three distinct target directions that cross a threshold of 0.8 (Fig. 6 A). Peaks occurred in directions of 195, 75, and 330°, with average η values of 0.88, 0.80, and 0.82, respectively. These values were averaged to define the target directedness of the data, a value of 0.83, which is used to compare hypotheses in the next section. Intersubject variability was low, with an overall η SD of 0.099 across subjects, averaging over all target directions.
We found that directions of target-directed ellipses corresponded closely to muscle action directions (Fig. 6B). The peak direction of 195° aligned well with the FDI action of 196.2° and the direction of 75° aligned well with the EIP action of 70.5°, both estimated from EMG–force cross-correlations (Fig. 6B). The peak direction of 330° aligned reasonably well with the FPI action of 347.5°, considering that the latter was obtained from MRI-based estimates obtained from the literature rather than the specific configuration and anatomy of our subjects. Configuration differences such as the greater MCP flexion angle in our setup could cause the FPI to have a greater flexion moment arm (An et al. 1983), sufficient to potentially explain any discrepancy with the anatomical studies. The close alignment between directions of target-directed ellipses and muscle actions suggests that these muscles may act as prime movers for those force targets that they are mechanically well suited.
For target directions between regions of high η and muscle mechanical actions, data indicate a greater amount of coactivation with no evidence of prime movers (Fig. 6B). For example, target directions ranging from 105 to 180° are aligned with none of the mechanical actions and were characterized by lower η. These same directions showed significant EMG coactivation in FDI, EDC, and EIP, with no clear prime mover. Evidence for other target directions is less direct but also indicates coactivation. For example, directions ranging from 225 to 300° showed significant FDI activity, even though the FDI produces too much abduction force to perform these target forces alone. These target forces could be performed with the coactivation of muscles with more flexor action, such as LUM, FDS, and FDP, although we did not collect EMG data from these muscles. Similarly, targets in the range 0–45° showed significant activity of EIP, implying the coactivation of other muscles such as FPI. Thus directions with low η corresponded to cases where either direct or indirect evidence supported the cooperation between multiple muscles with different action directions.
Analysis of endpoint force fluctuations from synergistic and flexible activation
We observed target-directed covariance for targets aligned with the action direction of several key muscles controlling the index finger, but non-target-directed covariance in other target directions. This observation suggests a flexible muscle coordination strategy, rather than the coordination of synergistic muscle groups that couple muscles with distinct action directions. We analyzed the ability of FCM to distinguish synergistic from flexible in the context of our assumptions, to estimate the conditions in which our conclusions are valid.
Synergy activation involves forming groups of muscles and then having the target commands activate these groups rather than activate muscles directly (Fig. 7 A). Flexible activation involves transforming target force commands into muscle control signals in a target-dependent way (Fig. 7B). Given the muscle action vectors, different hypotheses predict different directional tuning functions for the muscles (Fig. 7, C and D). Both hypotheses begin with two-dimensional target force commands, so both will produce low-dimensional muscle forces, regardless of whether there are synergies among the muscles (Fig. 7, E and F). Using the synergistic activation hypothesis, the first two principal components account for 80% of the total variance. Using the flexible activation hypothesis, the first two principal components account for 77% of the variance. Even though the synergy hypothesis is explicitly forming muscle groups, a dimensionality-reduction technique such as principal-components analysis (PCA) will be unable to distinguish these hypotheses because of the low target force dimensionality. However, since different hypotheses produce different directional tuning functions, and muscle force noise is assumed to be signal-dependent, the pattern of endpoint force fluctuations will be different for the two hypotheses. Synergy activation will produce non-target-directed ellipses for all tasks because the synergies require that each target force be performed by muscles with different action directions (Fig. 7G). Flexible activation will produce target-dependent force fluctuations, with target-directed ellipses in some target directions and non-target-directed ellipses in other target directions (Fig. 7H).
The source and type of noise will have an impact on FCM, so we analyzed noise occurring at the muscle level versus that at the synergy activation level (Fig. 8). As a baseline, FCM will not distinguish flexible activation from synergistic activation when noise is signal independent. When signal-dependent noise occurs at the muscle level, flexible activation produces much higher target directedness compared with synergistic activation, and certain parameter values can be found for flexible activation that describe the data, although no parameter values can be found that describe the data for synergistic activation (Fig. 8B).
This same result occurs when synergy-level SDN is made to be as strong as the muscle-level SDN (Fig. 8C). When synergy-level noise is made tenfold stronger than muscle-level noise, some parameter sets appear for which synergistic activation is consistent with the data (Fig. 8D), but these parameter sets induce large correlations in muscle force. The grand average of correlation across data-consistent synergy simulations and muscle pairs was 0.75. The average correlation across muscle pairs of the maximum across data-consistent synergy simulations was 0.96. The average correlation across muscle pairs of the minimum across data-consistent synergy simulations was 0.41. The muscle force correlations required for the synergy-level noise to reproduce the data are much larger than our estimates for FDI–EDC and FDI–EIP EMG correlation or values reported in the literature for peak EMG coherence that range from 0.1 to 0.5 (Farmer et al. 2007; Fisher et al. 2002; Kilner et al. 1999), with 0.5 occurring only at 20 Hz, which is outside the frequency range of muscle force production (Galganski et al. 1993).
DISCUSSION
We have introduced force covariance mapping (FCM) as a means of studying muscle coordination through variability of endpoint forces. We applied this to forces exerted by the human index finger to examine cooperation of muscles acting about the metacarpophalangeal (MCP) joint. One outcome was that endpoint force covariance varies with the net direction of endpoint force, indicating that muscles make varying force contributions that depend on target direction. A muscle may act as a prime mover when the target is well aligned with that muscle's mechanical action and as part of a group of coactivated muscles for other targets. These variations in contribution indicate that muscles do not always act in fixed synergies, but can act in less rigid groupings that weight each muscle's mechanical action according to the target force. The FCM approach is also helpful for indicating the degree to which multiple muscles contribute to generating a particular force, even in the absence of a full set of EMG recordings. To integrate these findings with previous literature, we first discuss the relevance of our results to theories of muscle coordination such as the muscle synergy hypothesis, prime mover activation, and optimization. We then compare the FCM technique to other methods such as uncontrolled manifold and PCA. Finally, we conclude with a discussion of potential limitations of FCM and of this study.
Implications for muscle coordination
The target-dependent alignment of force covariance ellipses has implications for coupling of muscle activations. There were three target directions with highly target-directed force covariances, indicating cases where individual muscles dominated the net endpoint force and were consequently not strongly coupled with other muscles. The variation of the FCM with target direction also indicates that muscles were not activated in fixed-ratio synergies. For example, we found highly target-directed covariance (η ≃ 0.9) for the 195° target direction, which matches well with the action of first dorsal interosseous (FDI). If the FDI were activated in synergies with other muscles, we would expect the other muscles to contribute substantially to this force in this target direction, resulting in lower η. A possible explanation is that the FDI and other index finger muscles are activated flexibly for each target force and are not in synergies.
These results also offer insight regarding the apparent conflict between individual muscles acting as prime movers and multiple muscles acting cooperatively. For example, the FDI is often assumed to be a prime mover for MCP abduction (Flament et al. 1993; Semmler and Nordstrom 1998a; Thomas et al. 1986), but this has recently been challenged by observations of substantial coactivation of extensor muscles, which are required to mechanically cancel FDI flexion moments and successfully abduct the MCP joint (Keenan et al. 2006; see also Fig. 6B). Our results show that FDI is indeed the prime mover in target forces in the 195° direction, that is, along its mechanical action (An et al. 1983; Fowler et al. 2001; Thomas et al. 1986). However, other directions, such as pure abduction (180°), necessitate the coactivation of other muscles such as EDC and EIP. Conflicting observations might therefore depend on small changes in the target direction. Given freedom to do so, subjects might perform varying degrees of flexion or extension along with the abduction target force, with varying contributions from other muscles. Subtle differences such as a 15° change in target direction could therefore make the difference between the FDI acting as a prime mover as opposed to cooperating with other muscles. Target-dependent muscle activation implies that it is helpful to measure or control for the flexion–extension direction even during a simple abduction target force.
We have shown that, if its assumptions are valid, FCM does not provide sufficient evidence to reject the flexible activation hypothesis in favor of the synergistic activation hypothesis. The term synergy has many uses in the motor control literature. Most broadly, a synergy may refer to the cooperation among a group of muscles to achieve a particular task. A synergy could also refer to a group of muscles such that each muscle belongs to only one synergy (Buchanan et al. 1986; Lee 1984; Macpherson 1991; Soechting and Lacquaniti 1989). More recently, muscle synergies have been viewed as a mechanism to reduce the number of command signals needed to activate a set of muscles—a view that allows a single muscle to belong to multiple synergies (d'Avella et al. 2003; Ting and Macpherson 2005; Tresch et al. 2006). As far as the assumptions we have made explicit are valid, FCM analysis shows that this last type of muscle synergy does not describe the muscular control of the index finger for the isometric forces we examined, thus providing a counterexample for critical analysis of the muscle synergy hypothesis.
It is unknown whether the nonsynergistic patterns we identified apply to stereotypical or involuntary tasks, such as postural reactions (e.g., Ting and Macpherson 2005), where spatiotemporal synergies are found. Physiologically, synergistic muscle activation patterns have been observed from electrical stimulation of specific anatomical structures (d'Avella and Bizzi 1998; Giszter et al. 1993; Saltiel et al. 2001) and in reflex behaviors (Hart and Giszter 2004; Kargo and Giszter 2000, 2008), but there may be other contributions to net muscle activation. For example, it is also unknown whether the flexible patterns we observed arise from inhibitory interactions or from lateral feedback between motor groups (Kuo 1994), as well as a variety of central or sensory feedback mechanisms (Cheung et al. 2005; Lockhart and Ting 2007). Many reflexes have multiple feedback sources (Wilmink and Nichols 2003) that could increase the flexibility of muscle activation patterns.
Comparison with other approaches
The FCM potentially complements other methods for studying muscle coordination. Although EMGs certainly provide the most direct measurements of muscle activity, they have some limitations. First, it may be inconvenient or difficult to record EMGs from all relevant muscles that might contribute to potential synergies. Second, EMGs alone do not quantify how active muscles contribute to the net force (Zhou et al. 2007) in muscle or endpoint force space. The FCM complements EMGs by indicating when a single muscle acts alone or nearly alone and, conversely, when multiple, distinct muscles contribute to a net force. Another powerful approach for examining synergies is to access the anatomical structures directly, for example, through stimulation of the frog spinal cord, which has also revealed evidence for dimensionality reduction (Bizzi et al. 1991; Giszter et al. 1993; Saltiel et al. 2001). The FCM could potentially complement this method by indicating individual muscle force contributions to a net action in joint torque or endpoint force space. It could also be used to determine whether actions produced through microstimulation resemble those observed during normal behavior of the animal.
The FCM approach bears some similarity to uncontrolled manifold (UCM) analysis (Latash et al. 2002), but serves a different purpose. UCM analysis exploits a mismatch between joint and task DOF to determine how the CNS coordinates redundant kinematics. An example is the task of exerting vertical force by the index and middle fingers (with one-task DOF and two-finger DOF), where the CNS distributes force between the fingers to balance the moment about a point between them, at the expense of stabilizing total force (Latash et al. 2001). This shows that the CNS has implicit control demands that resolve the joint or finger redundancy during a task of relatively lower dimensionality. In contrast, the FCM approach attempts to explicitly match kinematic and task DOF, thereby eliminating kinematic redundancy. Force covariance mapping therefore indicates how the CNS uses the remaining muscle redundancy to satisfy implicit control demands. Variability provides insight to the control of redundancy in both analyses, but the FCM focuses on muscle rather than joint redundancy.
Another class of methods for studying coordination comprises mathematical techniques that seek to represent EMG activity in different muscles as combinations of simple elements. These techniques include PCA (Ivanenko et al. 2004), independent components analysis (Hart and Giszter 2004), nonnegative matrix factorization (Ting and Macpherson 2005; Tresch et al. 2006), and cluster analysis (Krouchev et al. 2006), to cite but a few. These studies generally show that EMG signals from multiple muscles can be represented using a smaller number of variables than there are muscles, which may be reflective of neural constraints simplifying the problem of coordination. However, one issue highlighted here is low dimensionality in muscle activation space that may originate during task planning. This observation provides an explanation for how muscles could be activated flexibly and constraints in the activity among muscles could still be observed (see Fig. 7, A–C). It therefore becomes difficult to determine whether low dimensionality of EMGs is indicative of a fixed neural constraint or merely the outcome of a low-dimensional task that is resolved at the muscle level with flexible neural constraints. This difficulty can be addressed by variations in task that can enrich its dimensionality (Ting and Macpherson 2005) or to examine natural behaviors (Hart and Giszter 2004), although the issue of task planning dimension compared with muscle activity dimension is of primary importance. The FCM approach cannot answer this question directly, but our results do demonstrate how isometric force variability may contain information that goes beyond the dimensionality of EMG signals across multiple muscles.
Limitations of the force covariance map
The force covariance map is based on two assumptions that, if incorrect, could lead to erroneous conclusions. The primary assumption is one of signal-dependent noise (SDN). If a muscle's force fluctuations scaled not with its average force but according to either a systematic but nonmonotonic or a random relationship, the force covariance would reflect these other phenomena instead. In the present study, force variance scaled in direct proportion to net force and almost all of this variance indeed appeared to be signal dependent (as indicated by the slope and intercept of the SDN fit in Fig. 4).
Another assumption is that there is little correlation between force fluctuations of distinct muscles. Significant correlation has been observed in surface EMG activity of distinct muscles, with coherence values as high as 0.5 (Kilner et al. 1999). High correlations in force fluctuations would be expected to produce distortions in the force covariance map. Target-directed covariance (high η) might overestimate the actual degree to which a muscle acts alone. However, correlation would also typically cause η to peak in directions not aligned with muscle actions, in poor agreement with our results. In addition, EMG correlation occurs at relatively high-frequency ranges compared with force fluctuations in muscle, acting as a low-pass filter. Indeed, for muscle pairs examined in this study, EMG correlation, in the frequency range of interest for force generation, was generally <2%. Although cancellation of surface-detected electrical potentials influences the estimation of force correlation from EMG correlation (Keenan et al. 2007), and EMG signals from multitendon muscles such as EDC might not reflect the compartment of interest, we expect that force fluctuations would have relatively low correlation. In addition, our rigorous analysis of hypotheses (Fig. 8) indicated that the ability of FCM to distinguish between synergistic and flexible activation of the musculature is robust to modest amounts of correlation in the SDN among muscles (examined ≤10%). Nonetheless, it is important to consider correlation in the time-varying forces exerted by different muscles when considering the force covariance map.
It was our intention in this study to demonstrate that significant information exists in the pattern of multidirectional endpoint force fluctuations. EMG recordings were used in this study to show that coactivity among muscles with different action directions were associated with non-target-directed ellipses. We did not, however, record intramuscular EMG from a complete set of muscles that could contribute force to the endpoint. Follow-up studies with EMG recordings from a complete muscle set will be needed to look in more detail at three remaining issues: 1) Are target-directed force covariance ellipses associated with a large amount of electrical activity in one muscle compared with another? 2) Do any target-directed force covariance ellipses arise from the correlated activity of multiple muscles with different action directions? 3) Can the FCM be predicted from the EMG-estimated muscle coordination pattern and vice versa?
The force covariance map is most applicable to experiments where the task is defined and constrained as explicitly as possible. For example, we constrained all DOFs other than the MCP and both measured and controlled the remaining force vector. The alternative to isolating a single joint is to allow multiple kinematic DOFs to remain unconstrained and to measure them fully, for example with a multiaxis load cell. This also necessitates that force directions be controlled to span the space of possible forces. The FCM approach is therefore ill-suited to the broader question of how the CNS coordinates joints for general tasks. The CNS appears to prefer stereotypical motor strategies of low dimension and these strategies might sensibly use certain muscles, either individually or in groups, that have actions suitable to the task. For example, postural reactions that accelerate the body center of mass can utilize arbitrary combinations of joints, but intersegemental coupling is such that very fast accelerations can be achieved by coordinating the joints in a relatively fixed ratio (Kuo and Zajac 1993). Some movements might therefore favor certain combinations of muscles, not because they are coordinated in fixed synergies, but merely as an outcome of task selection.
The FCM approach is also not limited to forces involving a single joint. If multiple joints are involved, the matrix that maps muscle forces to endpoint forces will be more complex, but each muscle will still have an action vector in endpoint force space and different action vectors will add together linearly (Valero-Cuevas et al. 2000), thus satisfying the superposition assumptions of FCM.
Although we have discussed how the FCM approach may be used to infer the pattern of muscle activity from endpoint force fluctuation, it should be noted that the inference of activation may be complicated by large differences in muscle moment arms and cross-sectional areas. A muscle with a larger moment arm would transmit more force fluctuation for a given level of activity than a muscle with a smaller moment arm. Thus inferences from FCM more properly describe patterns of muscle activity corrected for moment arm and cross-sectional area.
For simplicity, we performed our hypothesis analysis on a single set of moment arm data from the literature (Fowler et al. 2001). Several authors have used different approaches to quantify these moment arms including cadaveric measurements (An et al. 1983; Valero-Cuevas et al. 2000), electrical stimulation (Kamper et al. 2006), and MRI measurements (Fowler et al. 2001). We chose to analyze the MRI data for the following reasons. The measurements of Valero-Cuevas et al. (2000) were made from a finger posture different from that in our experiment. The cadaveric data of An et al. (1983) appeared to be rotated clockwise in the endpoint force plane relative to our estimates for FDI, EIP, and EDC. Moreover, this study reported that FPI had a pure adduction moment, which is inconsistent with other studies (Fowler et al. 2001; Kamper et al. 2006) and the fact that it lays on the palmar side of the MCP joint (Brand and Hollister 1993). The MRI data (Fowler et al. 2001) and electrical stimulation data (Kamper et al. 2006) were very consistent with each other, our moment arm estimates, and the target directions exhibiting target-directed covariance ellipses. We analyzed the MRI data of Fowler et al. (2001) because relative moment arm magnitude was reported that can affect the simulated force covariance map. Nonetheless, it is important to consider the exact distribution of moment arms for a set of muscles when comparing hypotheses using force covariance mapping. For example, we used a cost function that minimized the sum of squared muscle forces to simulate the flexible activation hypothesis. Using this cost function, muscles will be dominant for targets in their action direction if there are not muscles within 90° on both sides in the endpoint force plane. Thus using this cost function, relatively small changes in a muscle's action direction could make the difference between whether a muscle is dominant for targets in its action direction. Indeed, given MRI moment arm estimates for the seven finger muscles, it is clear that the flexors have the FDI and FPI both within 90°. The flexible activation strategy would then predict that the flexors would not appear as prime movers for targets in the direction of their action. This prediction is supported by our EMG data (Fig. 6) and previous work (Thomas et al. 1986), showing activation of the FDI for target forces in flexion.
Conclusion
We hypothesized that signal-dependent noise would transform the pattern of relative activity in various muscles to the pattern of multidirectional endpoint force variability. Mapping the endpoint force variability across a set of target forces spanning a set of two-dimensional forces generated about the MCP joint of the index finger, we established that force covariance was highly dependent on target direction. We showed that the covariance dependence on target direction was consistent with different muscle coordination strategies. non-target-directed force covariance, predicted to result from activation of multiple muscles with distinct action directions, occurs for target directions that provide EMG evidence of muscle coactivation. Conversely, target-directed force covariance, predicted to result from activation of one muscle much more than others, occurs for target forces in the action direction of several muscles. The latter observation suggests that, since several index finger muscles can dominate force production for certain target directions, index finger muscles may not group into muscle synergies that are active for all target force directions. The mapping of endpoint variance—that we have termed force covariance mapping—may provide a straightforward approach for discriminating theories of neuromotor coordination.
GRANTS
This work was supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke/National Research Service Award Training Grant 1F31NS-057855-01 to J. J. Kutch and W. Z. Rymer and National Science Foundation Grants DMS-0604307 and CMS-0408542 to A. M. Bloch.
Acknowledgments
We thank M. Chardon and Dr. N. Suresh for technical assistance and helpful discussions.
APPENDIX
We analyzed the ability of the FCM approach to distinguish competing hypotheses for the coordination of multiple muscles in the presence of signal-dependent noise (SDN). We began with estimates of muscle action vectors, made from MRI measurements, of all seven muscles of the index finger (Fowler et al. 2001). Muscle action vectors were placed as columns in a 2 × 7 matrix A. As described earlier, using FCM to infer muscle activation from force variability relies on three assumptions. These assumptions are described mathematically here.
ASSUMPTION 1.
Muscles act along fixed directions in endpoint space and contributions from different muscles add linearly in endpoint space
(A1) |
where the muscle forces are listed in vector f, endpoints forces are listed in vector F, and the muscle action matrix A performs the transformation.
CONSEQUENCE 1.
Since the average of a sum is the sum of the averages, we can use Equation A1 to calculate the average endpoint forces given the average muscle forces
(A2) |
This equation converts the average muscle force vector to the average endpoint force vector, which is the center of the force covariance ellipse.
CONSEQUENCE 2.
Equation A1 also dictates that the covariance of endpoint force is
(A3) |
Since the force covariance map is described completely by F̄ and cov [F], all that remains is to calculate cov [f], which will be determined by SDN and muscle force correlation.
ASSUMPTION 2.
Muscle force variability is proportional to average muscle force. For muscle i, f = f̄i + ɛi, where ɛi is the SDN with variance
(A4) |
CONSEQUENCE 1.
If there is no muscle force correlation and the muscle activity pattern linearly scales with increasing force demands, the trace of the endpoint force covariance matrix will indicate how muscle force variability scales with average muscle force, on average across the muscles. If there is no muscle force correlation, then
(A5) |
(A6) |
where m is the number of muscles. Thus Trace {cov [F]}/m indicates the average of ‖ai‖2kif̄i across all muscles. The average muscle force will be a function of the required average endpoint force: f̄i = f̄i(F̄). If we assume that the muscle coordination pattern simply scales for increasing endpoint force demands (Valero-Cuevas 2000), then f̄i(αF̄) = αf̄i(F̄). Thus if SDN variance scales linearly with average muscle force (on average across the set of muscles), the trace of the endpoint force covariance matrix will scale linearly with the magnitude of the average endpoint force. Since we observed this scaling experimentally, we chose to model SDN variance as a linear function of average muscle force.
ASSUMPTION 3.
Noise correlations among muscles are modest. The ijth element of the noise covariance matrix cov [ɛ](f̄) can be expressed as
(A7) |
where ρij is the correlation between noise in muscle i and j.
The goal for each target was to calculate the average amount of force in each muscle so that the average endpoint force vector equaled a given target vector. For each simulation, target vectors were distributed at 15° increments along the unit circle. Average muscle forces were simulated using two hypotheses: synergistic activation and flexible activation. Flexible activation served as the null hypothesis and we analyzed evidence for rejecting it in favor of the synergistic activation.
Flexible activation hypothesis (there are no synergies).
Under this hypothesis, a target-dependent vector of average muscle forces f̄sel was selected so that
(A8) |
was minimized subject to the constraint that Af̄sel = Fd, where Fd is the desired endpoint force vector for the given target force. The force covariance map under flexible activation is then given by
(A9) |
Synergistic activation hypothesis.
Under this hypothesis, constraints are introduced among the components of the average muscle force vector so as to reduce its dimensionality. This is accomplished by expressing the muscle force vector as f = Wc + ɛ, where the columns of W encode the relative strength of each muscle in a given synergy, c is a vector of synergy weighting coefficients, and ɛ is muscle-level SDN. Since noise may also arise at the synergy level, c = c̄ + γ, where γ is synergy-level SDN
(A10) |
We chose to analyze this dependence of synergy-level noise because, if there were no SDN at the muscle level, this signal-dependent synergy-level noise would be required to produce the noise scaling in our data (see results). The object of synergies is to generate independently controllable muscle groups, so we did not consider cross-correlations in the synergy noise vector. Thus the synergy noise covariance matrix was diagonal with {cov [γ](c̄)}ii = var [γi](c̄i) = κic̄i. A target-dependent vector of average synergy coefficients c̄sel was selected so that
(A11) |
was minimized subject to the constraint AWc̄ = Fd for each target force. Simulations were performed using three synergies because this is the minimum number in a two-dimensional endpoint force space so that all target forces can be produced using the same set of synergies. The force covariance map under the synergistic activation is then given by
(A12) |
where β is a parameter used to control the strength of the force fluctuations due to synergy-level noise relative to force fluctuations due to muscle-level noise.
Unknown parameters
For each simulation, a random set of unknown parameter values were chosen from uniform distributions within specified ranges. Muscle-level noise scaling coefficients k were varied between 0 and 1, the synergy-level noise scaling coefficients κ were varied between 0 and 1, and the muscle-level noise correlation coefficients were varied between 0 and 0.1. The synergy coupling matrix for each simulation was generated as follows. A matrix Wtemp was generated with elements chosen uniformly between 0 and 1. The mechanical actions of the synergies were then calculated: Stemp = AWtemp. Wtemp was accepted and became the actual synergy matrix W if 1) the second strongest muscle in each synergy had action direction was ≥30° away from the action direction of the strongest muscle in that synergy and 2) the columns of Stemp spanned the endpoint force space with positive coefficients. These requirements ensured that synergies were not trivial (i.e., muscles with different mechanical actions were coupled) and could be activated to achieve all targets in the endpoint plane.
Hypothesis comparison
Each simulation generated a force covariance map for each hypothesis given the chosen parameters. The target-directed variance fraction η was calculated for each force covariance map in exactly the same way as for the data. We evaluated the hypotheses in their ability to generate high η when the data exhibited large η (on average across subjects and target magnitude levels). A set of target directions was identified when η peaked in the data. Average η in directions of data peaks was calculated for each simulation—we refer to this quantity as target directedness. Histograms of target directedness were generated across simulations (different parameter values) for each hypothesis and compared with the actual value of target directedness calculated from the observed data averaged across subjects and target magnitude levels.
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
REFERENCES
- An et al. 1983.An KN, Ueba Y, Chao EY, Cooney WP, Linscheid RL. Tendon excursion and moment arm of index finger muscles. J Biomech 16: 419–425, 1983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein 1967.Bernstein NA The Co-ordination and Regulation of Movements. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press, 1967.
- Bizzi et al. 1991.Bizzi E, Mussaivaldi FA, Giszter S. Computations underlying the execution of movement—a biological perspective. Science 253: 287–291, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brand and Hollister 1993.Brand P, Hollister A. Clinical Mechanics of the Hand (2nd ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby, 1993.
- Bremner et al. 1991a.Bremner FD, Baker JR, Stephens JA. Correlation between the discharges of motor units recorded from the same and from different finger muscles in man. J Physiol 432: 355–380, 1991a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bremner et al. 1991b.Bremner FD, Baker JR, Stephens JA. Variation in the degree of synchronization exhibited by motor units lying in different finger muscles in man. J Physiol 432: 381–399, 1991b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buchanan et al. 1986.Buchanan TS, Almdale DPJ, Lewis JL, Rymer WZ. Characteristics of synergic relations during isometric contractions of human elbow muscles. J Neurophysiol 56: 1225–1241, 1986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buchanan and Shreeve 1996.Buchanan TS, Shreeve DA. An evaluation of optimization techniques for the prediction of muscle activation patterns during isometric tasks. J Biomech Eng-Trans ASME 118: 565–574, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheung et al. 2005.Cheung VC, d'Avella A, Tresch MC, Bizzi E. Central and sensory contributions to the activation and organization of muscle synergies during natural motor behaviors. J Neurosci 25: 6419–6434, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- d'Avella and Bizzi 1998.d'Avella A, Bizzi E. Low dimensionality of supraspinally induced force fields. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 7711–7714, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- d'Avella et al. 2003.d'Avella A, Saltiel P, Bizzi E. Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a natural motor behavior. Nat Neurosci 6: 300–308, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deluca et al. 1993.Deluca CJ, Roy AM, Erim Z. Synchronization of motor-unit firings in several human muscles. J Neurophysiol 70: 2010–2023, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drew et al. 2008.Drew T, Kalaska J, Krouchev N. Muscle synergies during locomotion in the cat: a model for motor cortex control. J Physiol 586: 1239–1245, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enoka et al. 1999.Enoka RM, Burnett RA, Graves AE, Kornatz KW, Laidlaw DH. Task- and age-dependent variations in steadiness. In: Peripheral and Spinal Mechanisms in the Neural Control of Movement. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, 1999, p. 389–395. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Farmer et al. 2007.Farmer SF, Gibbs J, Halliday DM, Harrison LM, James LM, Mayston MJ, Stephens JA. Changes in EMG coherence between long and short thumb abductor muscles during human development. J Physiol 579: 389–402, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher et al. 2002.Fisher RJ, Galea MP, Brown P, Lemon RN. Digital nerve anaesthesia decreases EMG–EMG coherence in a human precision grip task. Exp Brain Res 145: 207–214, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flament et al. 1993.Flament D, Goldsmith P, Buckley CJ, Lemon RN. Task dependence of responses in 1st dorsal interosseous muscle to magnetic brain-stimulation in man. J Physiol 464: 361–378, 1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowler et al. 2001.Fowler NK, Nicol AC, Condon B, Hadley D. Method of determination of three dimensional index finger moment arms and tendon lines of action using high resolution MRI scans. J Biomech 34: 791–797, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galganski et al. 1993.Galganski ME, Fuglevand AJ, Enoka RM. Reduced control of motor output in a human hand muscle of elderly subjects during submaximal contractions. J Neurophysiol 69: 2108–2115, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giszter et al. 2007.Giszter S, Patil V, Hart C. Primitives, premotor drives, and pattern generation: a combined computational and neuroethological perspective. Prog Brain Res 165: 323–346, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giszter et al. 1993.Giszter SF, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Bizzi E. Convergent force fields organized in the frog's spinal cord. J Neurosci 13: 467–491, 1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris and Wolpert 1998.Harris CM, Wolpert DM. Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394: 780–784, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart and Giszter 2004.Hart CB, Giszter SF. Modular premotor drives and unit bursts as primitives for frog motor behaviors. J Neurosci 24: 5269–5282, 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman and Strick 1999.Hoffman DS, Strick PL. Step-tracking movements of the wrist. IV. Muscle activity associated with movements in different directions. J Neurophysiol 81: 319–333, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanenko et al. 2004.Ivanenko YP, Poppele RE, Lacquaniti F. Five basic muscle activation patterns account for muscle activity during human locomotion. J Physiol 556: 267–282, 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanenko et al. 2006.Ivanenko YP, Wright WG, Gurfinkel VS, Horak F, Cordo P. Interaction of involuntary post-contraction activity with locomotor movements. Exp Brain Res 169: 255–260, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones et al. 2002.Jones KE, Hamilton AF, Wolpert DM. Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. J Neurophysiol 88: 1533–1544, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamper et al. 2006.Kamper DG, Fischer HC, Cruz EG. Impact of finger posture on mapping from muscle activation to joint torque. Clin Biomech 21: 361–369, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kargo and Giszter 2000.Kargo WJ, Giszter SF. Rapid correction of aimed movements by summation of force-field primitives. J Neurosci 20: 409–426, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kargo and Giszter 2008.Kargo WJ, Giszter SF. Individual premotor drive pulses, not time-varying synergies, are the units of adjustment for limb trajectories constructed in spinal cord. J Neurosci 28: 2409–2425, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keenan et al. 2007.Keenan KG, Farina D, Meyer FG, Merletti R, Enoka RM. Sensitivity of the cross-correlation between simulated surface EMGs for two muscles to detect motor unit synchronization. J Appl Physiol 102: 1193–1201, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keenan et al. 2006.Keenan KG, McNamara RVI, Backus SI, Schieber MH, Valero-Cuevas FJ. Index finger abduction is a complex motor task. Program # 655.18. 2006 Neuroscience Meeting Planner. Atlanta, GA: Society for Neuroscience, 2006. Online.
- Kilner et al. 1999.Kilner JM, Baker SN, Salenius S, Jousmaki V, Hari R, Lemon RN. Task-dependent modulation of 15–30 Hz coherence between rectified EMGs from human hand and forearm muscles. J Physiol 516: 559–570, 1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krouchev et al. 2006.Krouchev N, Kalaska JF, Drew T. Sequential activation of muscle synergies during locomotion in the intact cat as revealed by cluster analysis and direct decomposition. J Neurophysiol 96: 1991–2010, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo 1994.Kuo AD A mechanical analysis of force distribution between redundant, multiple degree-of-freedom actuators in the human—implications for the central-nervous system. Hum Mov Sci 13: 635–663, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Kuo 2000.Kuo AD The action of two-joint muscles: the legacy of W. P. Lombard. In: Classics in Movement Science, edited by Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 2000, p. 289–316.
- Kuo and Zajac 1993.Kuo AD, Zajac FE. Human standing posture—multijoint movement strategies based on biomechanical constraints. Prog Brain Res 97: 349–358, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kutch et al. 2007.Kutch JJ, Suresh NL, Bloch AM, Rymer WZ. Analysis of the effects of firing rate and synchronization on spike-triggered averaging of multidirectional motor unit torque. J Comput Neurosci 22: 347–361, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laidlaw et al. 2000.Laidlaw DH, Bilodeau M, Enoka RM. Steadiness is reduced and motor unit discharge is more variable in old adults. Muscle Nerve 23: 600–612, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latash et al. 2001.Latash ML, Scholz JF, Danion F, Schoner G. Structure of motor variability in marginally redundant multifinger force production tasks. Exp Brain Res 141: 153–165, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latash et al. 2002.Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schoner G. Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of motor variability. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 30: 26–31, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence et al. 1993.Lawrence JH, Nichols TR, English AW. Cat hindlimb muscles exert substantial torques outside the sagittal plane. J Neurophysiol 69: 282–285, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee 1984.Lee WA Neuromotor synergies as a basis for coordinated intentional action. J Mot Behav 16: 135–170, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lockhart and Ting 2007.Lockhart DB, Ting LH. Optimal sensorimotor transformations for balance. Nat Neurosci 10: 1329–1336, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macpherson 1991.Macpherson JM How flexible are muscle synergies? In: Motor Control: Concepts and Issues, edited by Humphrey DR, Freund H-J. New York: Wiley, 1991, p. 33–47.
- Moritz et al. 2005.Moritz CT, Barry BK, Pascoe MA, Enoka RM. Discharge rate variability influences the variation in force fluctuations across the working range of a hand muscle. J Neurophysiol 93: 2449–2459, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mussa-Ivaldi et al. 1994.Mussa-Ivaldi F, Giszter S, Bizzi E. Linear combinations of primitives in vertebrate motor control. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91: 7534–7538, 1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overduin et al. 2008.Overduin SA, d'Avella A, Roh J, Bizzi E. Modulation of muscle synergy recruitment in primate grasping. J Neurosci 28: 880–892, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perotto 2005.Perotto AO Anatomical Guide for the Electromyographer. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas, 2005.
- Saltiel et al. 2001.Saltiel P, Wyler-Duda K, D'Avella A, Tresch MC, Bizzi E. Muscle synergies encoded within the spinal cord: evidence from focal intraspinal nmda iontophoresis in the frog. J Neurophysiol 85: 605–619, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandercock 2005.Sandercock TG Summation of motor unit force in passive and active muscle. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 33: 76–83, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schieber and Santello 2004.Schieber MH, Santello M. Hand function: peripheral and central constraints on performance. J Appl Physiol 96: 2293–2300, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt et al. 1979.Schmidt RA, Zelaznik H, Hawkins B, Frank JS, Quinn JT. Motor-output variability: theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychol Rev 86: 415–451, 1979. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semmler 2002.Semmler JG Motor unit synchronization and neuromuscular performance. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 30: 8–14, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semmler and Nordstrom 1998a.Semmler JG, Nordstrom MA. Hemispheric differences in motor cortex excitability during a simple index finger abduction task in humans. J Neurophysiol 79: 1246–1254, 1998a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semmler and Nordstrom 1998b.Semmler JG, Nordstrom MA. Motor unit discharge and force tremor in skill- and strength-trained individuals. Exp Brain Res 119: 27–38, 1998b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slifkin and Newell 1999.Slifkin A, Newell KM. Noise, information transmission, and force variability. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25: 837–851, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soechting and Lacquaniti 1989.Soechting JF, Lacquaniti F. An assessment of the existence of muscle synergies during load perturbations and intentional movements of the human arm. Exp Brain Res 74: 535–548, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas et al. 1991.Thomas CK, Bigland-Richie B, Johansson RS. Force-frequency relationships of human thenar motor units. J Neurophysiol 65: 1509–1516, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas et al. 1986.Thomas CK, Ross BH, Stein RB. Motor-unit recruitment in human first dorsal interosseous muscle for static contractions in three different directions. J Neurophysiol 55: 1017–1029, 1986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ting and Macpherson 2005.Ting LH, Macpherson JM. A limited set of muscle synergies for force control during a postural task. J Neurophysiol 93: 609–613, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todorov 2002.Todorov E Cosine tuning minimizes motor errors. Neural Comput 14: 1233–1260, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todorov and Jordan 2002.Todorov E, Jordan MI. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci 5: 1226–1235, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tresch et al. 2006.Tresch MC, Cheung VCK, d'Avella A. Matrix factorization algorithms for the identification of muscle synergies: Evaluation on simulated and experimental data sets. J Neurophysiol 95: 2199–2212, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valero-Cuevas 2000.Valero-Cuevas FJ Predictive modulation of muscle coordination pattern magnitude scales fingertip force magnitude over the voluntary range. J Neurophysiol 83: 1469–1479, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valero-Cuevas et al. 2000.Valero-Cuevas FJ, Towles JD, Hentz VR. Quantification of fingertip force reduction in the forefinger following simulated paralysis of extensor and intrinsic muscles. J Biomech 33: 1601–1609, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valero-Cuevas et al. 1998.Valero-Cuevas FJ, Zajac FE, Burgar CG. Large index-fingertip forces are produced by subject-independent patterns of muscle excitation. J Biomech 31: 693–703, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Bolhuis and Gielen 1999.van Bolhuis BM, Gielen C. A comparison of models explaining muscle activation patterns for isometric contractions. Biol Cybern 81: 249–261, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westling et al. 1990.Westling G, Johansson RS, Thomas CK, Bigland-Ritchie B. Measurement of contractile and electrical properties of single human thenar motor units in response to intraneural motor-axon stimulation. J Neurophysiol 64: 1331–1338, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilmink and Nichols 2003.Wilmink RJ, Nichols TR. Distribution of heterogenic reflexes among the quadriceps and triceps surae muscles of the cat hind limb. J Neurophysiol 90: 2310–2324, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou et al. 2007.Zhou P, Suresh NL, Rymer WZ. Model based sensitivity analysis of EMG-force relation with respect to motor unit properties: applications to muscle paresis in stroke. Ann Biomed Eng 35: 1521–1531, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]